Pain empathy

Last updated

Pain empathy is a specific variety of empathy that involves recognizing and understanding another person's pain.

Contents

Empathy is the mental ability that allows one person to understand another person's mental and emotional state and how to effectively respond to that person. There are several cues that can communicate pain to another person: visualization of the injury-causing event, the injury itself, behavioral efforts of the injured to avoid further harm, and displays of pain and distress such as facial expressions, crying, and screaming. [1] When a person receives cues that another person is in pain, neural pain circuits within the receiver's brain are activated. From an evolutionary perspective, pain empathy is beneficial for human group survival since it provides motivation for non-injured people to offer aid to the injured and to avoid injury themselves.[ citation needed ]

Initiating pain empathy

Resonance

A person who perceives another person's affective state can experience changes in brain activity, known as resonance, that help initiate an empathetic response. The inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior parietal lobule are two regions of the brain associated with[ clarification needed ] empathy resonance. [2]

Self-other discrimination

In order to have empathy for another person, one must understand the context of that person's experience while maintaining a certain degree of separation from their own[ clarification needed ] experience. The ability to differentiate the source of an affective stimuli as originating from the self or from other another is known as self-other discrimination. Self-other discrimination is associated with[ clarification needed ] the extrastriate body area (EBA), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), ventral premotor cortex, and the posterior and inferior parietal cortex. [2]

Response to painful facial expressions

A painful facial expression is one way in which the experience of pain is communicated from one individual to another. One study measured test subjects' brain activity and facial muscle activity when they watched video clips of a variety of facial expressions including neutral emotion, joy, fear, and pain. The study found that when a subject is shown a painful facial expression, their late positive potential (LPP) is increased during the time period of 600–1000ms after the initial exposure of the stimulus.[ clarification needed ] This increase in LPP for painful facial expressions was higher than the increase caused by other emotional expressions. [3]

Pain processing areas of the brain (The Pain Matrix)

Several areas of the brain are related to the processing of pain and pain empathy. These are known as the pain matrix.

One functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study measured brain activity during the experience of painful stimuli or while observing someone else receiving a painful stimuli. The study group consisted of 16 couples, on the assumption that these individuals would have empathy for one another. One member of the couple would receive a painful stimulus via electrode to the back of their hand while their partner observed, and brain activity was measured in both participants. [4]

fMRI images of brain regions involved in pain processing. Borsook D, Moulton EA, Schmidt KF, Becerra LR. Schematic of cortical areas involved with pain processing and fMRI cropped.jpg
fMRI images of brain regions involved in pain processing. Borsook D, Moulton EA, Schmidt KF, Becerra LR.

First hand experience of pain

The activated brain regions in the person experiencing the pain firsthand in that study included: contralateral sensorimotor cortex, bilateral secondary sensorimotor cortex, contralateral posterior insula, bilateral mid and anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex, right ventrolateral and mediodorsal thalamus, brainstem, and mid and right lateral cerebellum. [4] Another fMRI study measured brain activity of an individual receiving unpredictable laser pain stimuli. This study showed that the primary and secondary sensorimotor cortex, posterior insula, and lateral thalamus are involved in processing aspects of nociceptive stimuli such as location and intensity. [5]

Observed pain in others

Several brain regions including the bilateral anterior insula (AI), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brainstem, and cerebellum were activated both in instances of first person painful experience and observed painful experience. The bilateral anterior insula (AI) and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) are therefore hypothesized to take part in the emotional reaction evoked from witnessing another in pain. The somatosensory region of the brain was not shown by fMRI to be excited during pain observation, rather only when the pain was experienced firsthand. [4]

One electroencephalography study recorded brain activity while subjects were shown footage of a needle penetrating a hand. There was an increase in activity in the frontal, temporal, and parietal areas of the brain when the individuals were exposed to the footage and observed pain in others. The gamma band oscillations, which reflects brain activity, increased to 50–70 Hz. The usual gamma band oscillations in the brain are around 40 Hz. The results suggest high gamma band oscillations could help provide an understanding of the neural basis of empathy for pain. [6]

Debates

It is unclear as to how much the pain matrix is involved in pain empathy. Some studies have found that when all neural components of the pain matrix are observed together, they are more related to one's reaction to the presence of stimuli than pain in particular. [7] The ability to empathize with the pain of others seems to be correlated only to certain parts of the pain matrix rather than the matrix as a whole. [4] Some have argued that only the affective components of the pain matrix, the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex, are activated when it comes to pain empathy. [8]

Detection methods

Magnetoencephalography

fMRI studies could not detect activity in the somatosensory cortex during pain empathy. Neuromagnetic oscillatory activity was recorded from the primary somatosensory cortex in order to determine if it is involved in pain empathy. When the left medial nerve was stimulated, post-stimulus rebounds of 10 Hz of somatosensory oscillations were quantified. These baseline somatosensory oscillations were suppressed when the subject observed a painful stimulus to a stranger. These results show that the somatosensory cortex is involved in the pain empathy response even though the activity could not be detected using fMRI techniques. [9]

Electroencephalography (EEG)

Mu rhythm is a neural oscillation ranging from 8–12 Hz that occurs over the sensorimotor cortex and is suppressed during motor movements. [10] Motor resonance in response to pain empathy has been quantified using EEG technology as a decrease in power of mu rhythm over the sensorimotor cortex. [11] More suppression of mu rhythm is associated to an increased empathic response. [12] A study recording from central sensorimotor EEG channels (C3, Cz, & C4) found that participants had greater suppression of mu rhythm when shown images of hands and feet in painful situations as opposed to non-painful situations. [13]

Transcranial magnetic stimulation

Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation, when used to stimulate the motor cortex of a person observing another person's actions, increases the corticospinal excitability of associated with their motor resonance[ clarification needed ]. Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies show that frontal structures of the motor resonance system[ clarification needed ] process information about other people's physical actions. [2]

Motor evoked potential

Sensorimotor contagion takes place when corticospinal excitability reduces due to observing another person experiencing pain. A study by Avenanti on pain empathy in racial bias showed that when a person sees a needle being poked into the hand of another person, there is a reduced motor evoked potential in the muscle of the observer's hand. [14]

Lack of pain empathy

Lack of empathy occurs in several conditions including autism, schizophrenia, sadistic personality disorder, psychopathy, and sociopathy. One interpretation is that an improper ratio of cortical excitability to inhibition causes empathy defects. Brain stimulation is being investigated for its potential to alter motor resonance, pain empathy, self-other discrimination, and mentalizing as a way to treat empathy related disorders. [2]

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenic patients are impaired in several empathy domains. They are less able to identify emotions, take on different perspectives, and perform low level facial mirroring, and they are less sensitive in terms of affective responsiveness. [15] They are less responsive to their own pain and are less empathetic to that of others as well. [16] Schizophrenic patients exhibit neural and structural alterations in brain regions that are normally associated with the processing of empathy, such as the temporo-parietal junction and the amygdala. This is part of the reason why schizophrenic individuals have difficulties in understanding and reacting to the pain of others, impairing their ability to empathize with pain. [17]

One EEG study observed the brain activity of individuals with schizophrenia who were shown hands and feet in painful situations. The results suggested the ability to separate self from others, which is essential to empathizing with other people, is impaired in the schizophrenic sample. [17] Another study concluded that schizophrenic individuals tend to process other-related pain stimuli in a way that is usually more associated with how self-relevant stimuli are processed, and because of this these individuals are less able to differentiate between self and other. [18]

Schizophrenic subjects who participated in studies have also had difficulty assessing and discriminating between different levels of pain. For example, in one study, those with schizophrenia were less likely to be able to discriminate between ‘strong’ and ‘moderate’ pain levels than control subjects who did not have schizophrenia. [15] Subjects with schizophrenia were also less able to rate pain intensities from looking at different facial expressions. [16] The way in which these individuals empathize with pain may be caused partially by the lack of sensitivity in terms of affective processing of pain and the severity of the increased suspiciousness seen in many schizophrenics.

Those with schizophrenia are more easily disturbed by negative emotions of others (including pain), compared with healthy samples. [17] Schizophrenics tend to feel increased levels of personal distress when perceiving any sign of pain in other people. [19] This personal distress has been described as "hyper sensitivity" to the pain of others [18] that overwhelms the individual and hence impairs their ability to empathetically respond and relate to the pain of others on a personal level. The increased levels of personal distress may increase the individual's need to reduce their own discomfort, with the result that they give less attention to and empathize less with the pain of others. [19] [20]

Sadistic personality disorder

Psychopathy is thought to be caused by normal processing of social and emotional cues, but abnormal use of these cues. [2] One fMRI study looked at the brain activity of youth with aggressive conduct disorder and socially normal youth when they observed empathy eliciting stimuli. The results showed that the aggressive conduct disorder group had activation in the amygdala and ventral striatum, which lead the researcher to believe that these subjects may get a rewarding feeling from viewing pain in others. [21]

Autism

Autism-spectrum disorders are characterized by an impairment in the processing of social and emotional affective cues. [2]

Juvenile psychopathy

Young individuals who have callous and unemotional traits (CU) exhibit a lack of empathy. If a person experiences pain empathy they will be less motivated to hurt others, since people that experience pain empathy have distress when another person is hurt.

One study showed juvenile psychopaths video clips of strangers experiencing painful stimuli. The results showed that the juvenile psychopaths processed of these pain empathy eliciting stimuli atypically in comparison with the normal juvenile controls. The central late positive potential (LPP), a late cognitive evaluative component of pain, was decreased in subjects with low CU traits. Subjects with high CU traits had both a decrease in central LPP and in frontal N120, an early affective arousal component of pain. There were also differences in the pain thresholds between normal test subjects, subjects with low CU traits, and subjects with high CU traits. The subjects with CU traits had higher pain thresholds than the controls, which suggests they were less sensitive to noxious pain. The results show the CU-trait juvenile's lack of pain empathy was due to a lack of arousal due to another person's distress rather than a lack of understanding of the other's emotional state. [22]

Bias and variation

There is evidence that individual empathetic responses to the pain of others are biased based on racial identity, in-group/out-group status, and position on a social hierarchy. Experiences of pain empathy also vary depending on differences in personality such as the level of threat sensitivity in an individual. An individual who is highly sensitive to threat will have more intense empathetic reactions to others in pain when compared to those who are not as sensitive. [23]

In-group bias

An experiment used a minimal group paradigm[ clarification needed ] to create two groups, the in-group and the outgroup, to which they assigned 30 subjects. [24] 30 healthy right handed subjects participated in a fMRI experimental session in which they were shown 128 photographs where right hands and feet were in painful situations. The results of the study did not report any sign of an in-group or out-group favoritism, meaning subjects did not feel higher levels of empathy based on the groups they were in.[ clarification needed ]

Racial bias

A transcranial magnetic stimulation experiment measured inhibition of corticospinal excitability to determine if another person's race affected a person's pain empathy. Caucasian and black participants watched video clips of a needle penetrating a muscle in the right hand of a stranger who either belonged to their racial group or the other racial group. Transcranial magnetic stimulation stimulated the left motor cortex and motor-evoked potentials were measured in the observer's first dorsal interosseus muscle of the right hand. The results of the experiment showed that when the video clip showed a hand belonging to a person of the same racial group, the measured corticospinal excitability to the right hand of the observer was reduced. This inhibition effect was not present when a subject viewed a clip of a hand belonging to a stranger outside of their racial group. This corticospinal inhibition occurs in first hand[ clarification needed ] experience of pain, and suggests that there is activation of the observer's sensorimotor system while witnessing painful stimuli. [14]

Social hierarchy bias

An experiment placed individuals in a social hierarchy based on their skills when completing a task. The subjects were then scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging while watching those of an inferior and superior status experience painful or non-painful stimulation. The results of the study concluded relative positions in a social hierarchy affects empathetic responses to pain in that individuals are more likely to be more empathetic to the pain of those in an inferior status than to those in a more superior status. [25] This suggests there is bias depending on one's position on a social hierarchy when it comes to pain empathy.

Empathy gap

Humans tend to underestimate the intensity of physical pain other individuals go through, which affects their empathetic reactions to the pain of others. [23]

Physicians and pain empathy response

Physicians are frequently exposed to people experiencing pain due to injury or illness, or have to inflict pain during the course of treatment. Physicians have to regulate their emotional response to such stimuli in order to effectively help the patient and maintain their own personal well being. Pain empathy can motivate an individual to help someone who is in pain, but repeated exposure to individuals in pain with no ability to regulate emotional arousal can cause distress.

One study sought to determine if physicians were different in their response to viewing painful stimuli. Physicians and control subjects watched video clips of a stranger being poked with a needle into the hands or feet. fMRI was used to measure the hemodynamic activity within the brain while viewing the painful stimuli. The fMRI revealed that brain areas involved in the pain matrix—somatosensory cortex, anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus (dACC), and the periaqueductal gray (PAG)—were activated in the control subject population when viewing the needle penetration videos. The physicians had activation of higher order executive functioning in the brain as shown by activation of the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortex, both involved in self-regulation, and activation of the precentral, superior parietal, and temporoparietal junction, involved with executive attention. The physicians did not have activation of the anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus (dACC), or the periaqueductal gray (PAG). The study concluded that physicians adapt to the healthcare environment by down-regulating their automatic empathetic response to patients’ pain. [26]

Pain synesthesia

Synesthesia is a phenomenon that occurs when sensory information in one cognitive pathway causes a sensation through another cognitive pathway. Pain synesthesia is a form of synesthesia that causes a person to experience pain when seeing pain empathetic eliciting stimuli. The most common group who report pain synesthesia are patients with phantom limb syndrome. [27]

Cultural variation

Evidence from several studies points to cultural variation in pain empathy. An experiment in which British participants and East Asian participants were shown a video of a needle puncturing a hand demonstrated such cultural variations. [28] The results showed British subjects demonstrated more empathetic concern and higher levels of emotional experience to do with empathy compared to the East Asian participants.

Related Research Articles

In physiology, nociception, also nocioception; from Latin nocere 'to harm/hurt') is the sensory nervous system's process of encoding noxious stimuli. It deals with a series of events and processes required for an organism to receive a painful stimulus, convert it to a molecular signal, and recognize and characterize the signal to trigger an appropriate defensive response.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Empathy</span> Capacity to understand or feel what another person is experiencing

Empathy is generally described as the ability to take on another's perspective, to understand, feel, and possibly share and respond to their experience. There are more definitions of empathy that include but is not limited to social, cognitive, and emotional processes primarily concerned with understanding others. Often times, empathy is considered to be a broad term, and broken down into more specific concepts and types that include cognitive empathy, emotional empathy, somatic empathy, and spiritual empathy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anterior cingulate cortex</span> Brain region

In the human brain, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is the frontal part of the cingulate cortex that resembles a "collar" surrounding the frontal part of the corpus callosum. It consists of Brodmann areas 24, 32, and 33.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Precuneus</span> Region of the parietal lobe of the brain

In neuroanatomy, the precuneus is the portion of the superior parietal lobule on the medial surface of each brain hemisphere. It is located in front of the cuneus. The precuneus is bounded in front by the marginal branch of the cingulate sulcus, at the rear by the parieto-occipital sulcus, and underneath by the subparietal sulcus. It is involved with episodic memory, visuospatial processing, reflections upon self, and aspects of consciousness.

A mirror neuron is a neuron that fires both when an organism acts and when the organism observes the same action performed by another. Thus, the neuron "mirrors" the behavior of the other, as though the observer were itself acting. Mirror neurons are not always physiologically distinct from other types of neurons in the brain; their main differentiating factor is their response patterns. By this definition, such neurons have been directly observed in humans and primate species, and in birds.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Insular cortex</span> Portion of the mammalian cerebral cortex

The insular cortex is a portion of the cerebral cortex folded deep within the lateral sulcus within each hemisphere of the mammalian brain.

Reduced affect display, sometimes referred to as emotional blunting or emotional numbing, is a condition of reduced emotional reactivity in an individual. It manifests as a failure to express feelings either verbally or nonverbally, especially when talking about issues that would normally be expected to engage emotions. In this condition, expressive gestures are rare and there is little animation in facial expression or vocal inflection. Additionally, reduced affect can be symptomatic of autism, schizophrenia, depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, depersonalization derealization disorder, schizoid personality disorder or brain damage. It may also be a side effect of certain medications.

Affective sensation is an occurrence of sensation accompanied with a strong compulsion to act on it. It refers, mostly in neuroscience, to the emotional sensibility in response to affective stimuli of a particular valence. It is transmitted via the spinothalamic tract through the spinal cord, and can be associated with reflex actions such as the scratch, gag, and withdrawal reflexes. Sensory processing in the brain interacts with behavioral choices, such as decisions to eat or to stop eating, in both healthy individuals and those with eating disorders.

The simulation theory of empathy holds that humans anticipate and make sense of the behavior of others by activating mental processes that, if they culminated in action, would produce similar behavior. This includes intentional behavior as well as the expression of emotions. The theory says that children use their own emotions to predict what others will do; we project our own mental states onto others.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Somatosensory system</span> Nerve system for sensing touch, temperature, body position, and pain

Touch is perceiving the environment using skin. Specialized receptors in the skin send signals to the brain indicating light and soft pressure, hot and cold, body position and pain. It is a subset of the sensory nervous system, which also includes the visual, auditory, olfactory, gustatory and vestibular senses.

Mirror-touch synesthesia is a rare condition which causes individuals to experience a similar sensation in the same part or opposite part of the body that another person feels. For example, if someone with this condition were to observe someone touching their cheek, they would feel the same sensation on their own cheek. Synesthesia, in general, is described as a condition in which a concept or sensation causes an individual to experience an additional sensation or concept. Synesthesia is usually a developmental condition; however, recent research has shown that mirror touch synesthesia can be acquired after sensory loss following amputation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mechanisms of mindfulness meditation</span>

Mindfulness has been defined in modern psychological terms as "paying attention to relevant aspects of experience in a nonjudgmental manner", and maintaining attention on present moment experience with an attitude of openness and acceptance. Meditation is a platform used to achieve mindfulness. Both practices, mindfulness and meditation, have been "directly inspired from the Buddhist tradition" and have been widely promoted by Jon Kabat-Zinn. Mindfulness meditation has been shown to have a positive impact on several psychiatric problems such as depression and therefore has formed the basis of mindfulness programs such as mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based pain management. The applications of mindfulness meditation are well established, however the mechanisms that underlie this practice are yet to be fully understood. Many tests and studies on soldiers with PTSD have shown tremendous positive results in decreasing stress levels and being able to cope with problems of the past, paving the way for more tests and studies to normalize and accept mindful based meditation and research, not only for soldiers with PTSD, but numerous mental inabilities or disabilities.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tactile hallucination</span> Hallucination involving perception of tactile input

Tactile hallucination is the false perception of tactile sensory input that creates a hallucinatory sensation of physical contact with an imaginary object. It is caused by the faulty integration of the tactile sensory neural signals generated in the spinal cord and the thalamus and sent to the primary somatosensory cortex (SI) and secondary somatosensory cortex (SII). Tactile hallucinations are recurrent symptoms of neurological diseases such as schizophrenia, Parkinson's disease, Ekbom's syndrome and delirium tremens. Patients who experience phantom limb pains also experience a type of tactile hallucination. Tactile hallucinations are also caused by drugs such as cocaine and alcohol.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Interoception</span> Sensory system that receives and integrates information from the body

Interoception is the collection of senses providing information to the organism about the internal state of the body. This can be both conscious and subconscious. It encompasses the brain's process of integrating signals relayed from the body into specific subregions—like the brainstem, thalamus, insula, somatosensory, and anterior cingulate cortex—allowing for a nuanced representation of the physiological state of the body. This is important for maintaining homeostatic conditions in the body and, potentially, facilitating self-awareness.

Meditation and pain is the study of the physiological mechanisms underlying meditation—specifically its neural components—that implicate it in the reduction of pain perception.

Social cognitive neuroscience is the scientific study of the biological processes underpinning social cognition. Specifically, it uses the tools of neuroscience to study "the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the social world". Social cognitive neuroscience uses the epistemological foundations of cognitive neuroscience, and is closely related to social neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience employs human neuroimaging, typically using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Human brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation are also used. In nonhuman animals, direct electrophysiological recordings and electrical stimulation of single cells and neuronal populations are utilized for investigating lower-level social cognitive processes.

Network neuroscience is an approach to understanding the structure and function of the human brain through an approach of network science, through the paradigm of graph theory. A network is a connection of many brain regions that interact with each other to give rise to a particular function. Network Neuroscience is a broad field that studies the brain in an integrative way by recording, analyzing, and mapping the brain in various ways. The field studies the brain at multiple scales of analysis to ultimately explain brain systems, behavior, and dysfunction of behavior in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Network neuroscience provides an important theoretical base for understanding neurobiological systems at multiple scales of analysis.

Valeria Gazzola is an Italian neuroscientist, associate professor at the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences at the University of Amsterdam (UvA) and member of the Young Academy of Europe. She is also a tenured department head at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN) in Amsterdam, where she leads her own research group and the Social Brain Lab together with neuroscientist Christian Keysers. She is a specialist in the neural basis of empathy and embodied cognition: Her research focusses on how the brain makes individuals sensitive to the actions and emotions of others and how this affects decision-making.

An empathy gap, sometimes referred to as an empathy bias, is a breakdown or reduction in empathy where it might otherwise be expected to occur. Empathy gaps may occur due to a failure in the process of empathizing or as a consequence of stable personality characteristics, and may reflect either a lack of ability or motivation to empathize.

Consoling touch is a pro-social behavior involving physical contact between a distressed individual and a caregiver. The physical contact, most commonly recognized in the form of a hand hold or embrace, is intended to comfort one or more of the participating individuals. Consoling touch is intended to provide consolation - to alleviate or lessen emotional or physical pain. This type of social support has been observed across species and cultures. Studies have found little difference in the applications of consoling touch, with minor differences in frequency occurrence across cultures. These findings suggest a degree of universality. It remains unclear whether the relationship between social touch and interpersonal emotional bonds reflect biologically driven or culturally normative behavior. Evidence of consoling touch in non-human primates, who embrace one another following distressing events, suggest a biological basis. Numerous studies of consoling touch in humans and animals unveil a consistent physiological response. An embrace from a friend, relative, or even stranger can trigger the release of oxytocin, dopamine, and serotonin into the bloodstream. These neurotransmitters are associated with positive mood, numerous health benefits, and longevity. Cortisol, a stress hormone, also decreases. Studies have found that the degree of intimacy and quality of relationship between consoler and the consoled mediates physiological effects. In other words, while subjects experience reduced cortisol levels while holding the hand of a stranger, they exhibit a larger effect when receiving comfort from a trusted friend, and greater still, when holding the hand of a high quality romantic partner.

References

  1. Jean Decety, W. I. (Ed.). (2009). The Social Neuroscience of Empathy: MIT Press.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Hetu, S.; Taschereau-Dumouchel, V.; Jackson, P. L. (2012). "Stimulating the brain to study social interactions and empathy. [Article]". Brain Stimulation. 5 (2): 95–102. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.005. PMID   22503509. S2CID   18850529.
  3. Reicherts, P.; Wieser, M. J.; Gerdes, A. B. M.; Likowski, K. U.; Weyers, P.; Muhlberger, A.; Pauli, P. (2012). "Electrocortical evidence for preferential processing of dynamic pain expressions compared to other emotional expressions. [Article]". Pain. 153 (9): 1959–1964. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2012.06.017. PMID   22795483. S2CID   22450468.
  4. 1 2 3 4 Singer, T. S. B.; O'Doherty, J; Kaube, H; Dolan, RJ; Frith, CD. (2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain". Science. 303 (5661): 1157–1162. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535. hdl: 21.11116/0000-0001-A020-5 . PMID   14976305. S2CID   14727944.
  5. Bingel, U.; Quante, M.; Knab, R.; Bromm, B.; Weiller, C.; Buchel, C. (2003). "Single trial fMRI reveals significant contralateral bias in responses to laser pain within thalamus and somatosensory cortices. [Article]". NeuroImage. 18 (3): 740–748. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00033-2. PMID   12667851. S2CID   6291627.
  6. Motoyama, Y., Ogata, K., Hoka S., & Tobimatsu, S. (2010). "P15-15 Neural substrates of empathy for pain: Simultaneous recording of high density EEG and conventional ECG". Clinical Neurophysiology. 121: S194. doi:10.1016/s1388-2457(10)60798-5. S2CID   53144387.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. Michael, J. & Fardo, F. (2014). "What (If Anything) Is Shared in Pain Empathy? A Critical Discussion of De Vignemont and Jacob's Theory of the Neural Substrate of Pain Empathy". Philosophy of Science. 81 (1): 154–160. doi:10.1086/674203. hdl: 2434/923844 . S2CID   143192758.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. Bufalari, I., Aprile, T., Avenanti, A., Di Russo, F., & Aglioti, S. (2007). "Empathy for Pain and Touch in the Human Somatosensory Cortex". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (11): 2553–2561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl161 . PMID   17205974.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. Cheng, Y., Yang, C. Y., Lin, C. P., Lee, P. L., & Decety, J. (2008). "The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: A magnetoencephalography study."
  10. Pfurtscheller, G; Neuper, Ch; Andrew, C; Edlinger, G (1997-06-01). "Foot and hand area mu rhythms". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 26 (1): 121–135. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00760-5. ISSN   0167-8760. PMID   9202999.
  11. Moore, Matthew R.; Franz, Elizabeth A. (2017-02-01). "Mu rhythm suppression is associated with the classification of emotion in faces". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 17 (1): 224–234. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0476-6 . ISSN   1531-135X. PMID   27815729. S2CID   24027633.
  12. Joyal, Christian C.; Neveu, Sarah-Michelle; Boukhalfi, Tarik; Jackson, Philip L.; Renaud, Patrice (2018). "Suppression of Sensorimotor Alpha Power Associated With Pain Expressed by an Avatar: A Preliminary EEG Study". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 12: 273. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00273 . ISSN   1662-5161. PMC   6046452 . PMID   30038564.
  13. Arnett, Kirsten; Roach, Alexandra; Elzy, Meredith; Jelsone-Swain, Laura (2019-03-04). "Childhood emotional invalidation and right hemispheric mu suppression during a pain empathy task: An EEG study". Social Neuroscience. 14 (2): 236–250. doi:10.1080/17470919.2018.1441905. ISSN   1747-0919. PMID   29451071. S2CID   3690092.
  14. 1 2 Avenanti, A.; Sirigu, A.; Aglioti, S. M. (2010). "Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain". Current Biology. 20 (11): 1018–1022. Bibcode:2010CBio...20.1018A. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071 . PMID   20537539. S2CID   7048253.
  15. 1 2 Martins, M. J., Moura, B. L., Martins, I. P., Figueira, M. L. & Prkachin, K. M. (2011). "Sensitivity to expressions of pain in schizophrenia patients". Psychiatry Research. 189 (2): 180–184. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.03.007. PMID   21470693. S2CID   26371301.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. 1 2 Wojakiewicz, A., Januel, D., Braha, S., Prkachin, K., Danziger, N. & Bouhassira, D. (2013). "Alteration of pain recognition in schizophrenia". European Journal of Pain. 17 (9): 1385–1392. doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00310.x. PMID   23529960. S2CID   32603486.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. 1 2 3 Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Brown, E. C., Tas, C., Breidenstein, A. & Brüne, M. (2016). "Alterations in event-related potential responses to empathy for pain in schizophrenia". Psychiatry Research. 241: 14–21. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.091. PMID   27152905. S2CID   4769270.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. 1 2 Horan, W., Jimenez, A., Lee, J., Wynn, J., Eisenberger, N., & Green, M. (2016). "Pain empathy in schizophrenia: An fMRI study". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (5): 154–160. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw002. PMC   4847698 . PMID   26746181.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. 1 2 Didehbani, N., Shad, M. U., Tamminga, C. A., Kandalaft, M. R., Allen, T. T., Chapman, S. B., & Krawczyk, D. C. (2012). "Insight and empathy in schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research. 142 (1–3): 246–247. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.010. PMID   23043874. S2CID   6863595.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2017). "Empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index". Psychiatry Research. 249: 293–303. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.033. hdl: 1805/11989 . PMID   28142103. S2CID   5043806.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. Decety, J.; Michalska, K. J.; Akitsuki, Y.; Lahey, B. B. (2009). "Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: A functional MRI investigation". Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–211. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC   2819310 . PMID   18940230.
  22. Cheng, Y. W.; Hung, A. Y.; Decety, J. (2012). "Dissociation between affective sharing and emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths. [Article]". Development and Psychopathology. 24 (2): 623–636. doi:10.1017/s095457941200020x. PMID   22559135. S2CID   37377507.
  23. 1 2 Auyeung, K., & Alden, W. (2016). "Social Anxiety and Empathy for Social Pain". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 40 (1): 38–45. doi:10.1007/s10608-015-9718-0. hdl: 2429/62699 . S2CID   43695016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. Ruckman, J., Bodden, M., Jansen, A., Kircher, T., Dobel, R. & Reif, W. (2015). "How pain empathy depends on ingroup/outgroup decisions: A functional magnet resonance imaging study". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 234 (1): 57–65. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.08.006. PMID   26323252. S2CID   7209426.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. Feng, C., Li, Z., Feng, X., Wang, L., Tian, T., & Luo, Y. (2016). "Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (3): 485–495. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv135. PMC   4769637 . PMID   26516169.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. Decety, J.; Yang, C. Y.; Cheng, Y. W. (2010). "Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: An event-related brain potential study. [Article]". NeuroImage. 50 (4): 1676–1682. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.025. PMID   20080194. S2CID   6081243.
  27. Fitzgibbon, B. M.; Giummarra, M. J.; Georgiou-Karistianis, N.; Enticott, P. G.; Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). "Shared pain: From empathy to synaesthesia. [Review]". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (4): 500–512. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.007. PMID   19857517. S2CID   14874873.
  28. Atkins, D., Uskul, A., & Cooper, N. (2016). "ulture shapes empathic responses to physical and social pain". Emotion. 16 (5): 587–601. doi:10.1037/emo0000162. PMC   4976796 . PMID   26950365.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)