Bevil Conway

Last updated
Bevil Conway
Born (1974-11-04) 4 November 1974 (age 49)
Salisbury, Rhodesia (now Harare, Zimbabwe)
Alma mater
Known forColor research, painting, and printmaking
Scientific career
FieldsNeuroscience

Bevil Conway (born 1974), is a Zimbabwean neuroscientist, visual artist, and an expert in color. [1] Conway specialises in visual perception in his scientific work, and he often explores the limitations of the visual system in his artwork. At Wellesley College, Conway was Knafel Assistant Professor of Natural Science from 2007 to 2011, and associate professor of Neuroscience until 2016. He was a founding member of the Neuroscience Department at Wellesley. Prior to joining the Wellesley faculty, Conway helped establish the Kathmandu University Medical School in Nepal, where he taught as assistant professor in 2002–03. He currently[ as of? ] runs the Sensation, Cognition and Action Unit [2] in the Laboratory of Sensorimotor Research at the National Eye Institute and the National Institute of Mental Health.

Contents

Conway was educated at McGill University and Harvard University. On finishing his PhD and post-doctoral work under Margaret Livingstone and David Hubel, Conway was elected a Junior Fellow at the Harvard Society of Fellows, and spent a year as an Alexander von Humboldt Fellow at the University of Bremen, Germany. Conway has held grants from the National Science Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, [3] the Whitehall Foundation, and the Radcliffe Institute for Advanced Study. [4]

Science

Conway's research originally set out to explore the principle of double opponency in the primate visual system, showing (in 2001 [5] and 2006 [6] ) that color cells in the first stage of cortical processing (V1) compute local ratios of cone activity, making them both color-opponent (red-green and blue-yellow) and spatially opponent, pinning them down as the likely basis for color constancy and the brain's building blocks for constructing hue.

Subsequent work has focused on the representation of color in extrastriate areas of the brain that receive input from V1. In collaboration with Doris Tsao, he used fMRI to identify such functionally defined regions and coined the term "globs" to describe them. In 2007 he used targeted single-unit recording techniques to characterise the behaviour of cells in these color areas, showing that individual neurons in these areas respond selectively to specific hues. [7] The behaviour of these cells and the networks they are involved in are the current focus of his work. [8] By comparing the responses to colors, faces, bodies, places, and objects, Conway's work uncovered the multi-stage parallel processing organization of inferior temporal cortex. This work suggests that IT implements a set of canonical operations in parallel: in Conway's framework, the face-patch network is simply one manifestation of the operations carried out by IT. [9] In a 2023 opinion essay, Conway and his coauthors rendered their final judgement: “the [Opponent Colors] theory is wrong”. [10]

Conway's scientific account of #thedress has become the standard account of the phenomenon. [11] [12] [13] Empirical work by Conway and Ted Gibson on how languages name colors provided evidence that reconciled relativist and universalist accounts, connecting color perception to behavior. [14] [15]

Art

Much of Conway's research is guided by the underlying thought that visual art can be used to reveal insights about how visual information is processed. [16] An ongoing research project examines the idea that poor stereopsis may be an asset to artists (the startling finding that Rembrandt may have lacked stereopsis was widely discussed in the media). [17] [18] His interest in the dove-tailing of science and art has also spawned an interdisciplinary upper level course at Wellesley, Vision and Art: Physics, Physiology, Perception, and Practice. [19] Conway has promoted engagement of museums with neuroscience, [20] serving as an advisor to the Peabody Essex Museum in the PEM neuroscience initiative. [21]

As an artist Conway is active in visual media, predominantly watercolors, oils, and prints. He is regularly a visiting artist at the Columbus College of Art and Design. [22] A larger, ongoing project is a series of sculptures in the shape of glass boxes. [23] [24] His interest is driven by fundamental questions of art making: How do brain and visual apparatus co-operate in making an art object? What is the role of muscle memory in making marks on paper and, more broadly, in the creative process? How do artists challenge the constraints and limitations of our visual system? His works are in the collection of the Fogg Art Museum, private collections in Europe, North America and Africa, and have been featured in books and commercials. [24]

Notes

  1. Jaffe, Eric (20 March 2014). "The Fascinating Neuroscience Of Color". Fast Company.
  2. "Bevil R. Conway, Ph.D. | Principal Investigators | NIH Intramural Research Program". Archived from the original on 10 May 2019. Retrieved 10 May 2019.
  3. Conway, Bevil. "Neural mechanisms of color" via grantome.com.{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  4. "Bevil Conway". Radcliffe Institute for Advanced Study at Harvard University.
  5. Conway, Bevil R. (15 April 2001). "Spatial Structure of Cone Inputs to Color Cells in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex (V-1)" (PDF). The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (8). Wellesley.edu: 2768–2783. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02768.2001. PMC   6762533 . PMID   11306629 . Retrieved 10 June 2010.
  6. Conway, Bevil R; Margaret S. Livingstone (18 October 2006). "Spatial and Temporal Properties of Cone Signals in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex" (PDF). The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (42). Wellesley.edu: 10826–10846. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2091-06.2006. PMC   2963176 . PMID   17050721 . Retrieved 10 June 2010.
  7. Conway, Bevil R; Sebastian Moeller; Doris Y. Tsao (8 November 2007). "Specialized Color Modules in Macaque Extrastriate Cortex" (PDF). Neuron. 56 (3). Wellesley.edu: 560–573. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.008. PMC   8162777 . PMID   17988638 . Retrieved 10 June 2010.
  8. Conway, BR; Tsao, DY (2009). "Color-tuned neurons are spatially clustered according to color preference within alert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (42): 18034–9. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10618034C. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810943106 . PMC   2764907 . PMID   19805195.
  9. Conway, Bevil R. (15 September 2018). "The Organization and Operation of Inferior Temporal Cortex". Annual Review of Vision Science. 4 (1): 381–402. doi:10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034202. PMC   6404234 . PMID   30059648.
  10. Conway, Bevil R.; Malik-Moraleda, Saima; Gibson, Edward (30 June 2023). "Color appearance and the end of Hering's Opponent-Colors Theory". Cell . 27 (9): 791-804. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2023.06.003 .
  11. Rogers, Adam. "The Science of Why No One Agrees on the Color of This Dress". Wired via www.wired.com.
  12. Fleur, Nicholas St (14 May 2015). "The Science Behind the Dress". The New York Times.
  13. Lafer-Sousa, Rosa; Conway, Bevil R. (1 October 2017). "#TheDress: Categorical perception of an ambiguous color image". Journal of Vision. 17 (12): 25. doi:10.1167/17.12.25. PMC   5672910 . PMID   29090319 via jov.arvojournals.org.
  14. Gibson, Edward; Futrell, Richard; Jara-Ettinger, Julian; Mahowald, Kyle; Bergen, Leon; Ratnasingam, Sivalogeswaran; Gibson, Mitchell; Piantadosi, Steven T.; Conway, Bevil R. (3 October 2017). "Color naming across languages reflects color use". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 114 (40): 10785–10790. Bibcode:2017PNAS..11410785G. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1619666114 . PMC   5635863 . PMID   28923921.
  15. Gutman, Rachel (18 September 2017). "The Real Difference Between Warm and Cool Colors". The Atlantic.
  16. Baker, Billy (10 November 2008). "Artist's vision: Decode color perception". Boston Globe.
  17. Blakeslee, Sandra (16 September 2004). "Deconstructing the Gaze of Rembrandt; Scientists Say a Vision Flaw May Have Aided His Genius". The New York Times. Retrieved 10 June 2010.
  18. "An eye on Rembrandt". Boston Globe. 16 September 2004.
  19. Lafer-Sousa, Rosa; Bevil R. Conway (Fall 2009). "Vision and Art: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Neuroscience Education" (PDF). Journal of Undergraduate Neuroscience Education. 8 (1): A10–A17. PMC   3592702 . PMID   23493842. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 July 2011. Retrieved 10 June 2010. |work=The Journal of Undergraduate Neuroscience Education (JUNE)
  20. "Brain matters". pem.org.
  21. Bidgood, Jess (11 March 2017). "How to Get the Brain to Like Art". The New York Times.
  22. "Visiting Artists & Scholars: Bevil Conway". Columbus College of Art & Design.
  23. "Regional Juried Exhibition 2019".
  24. 1 2 "Bevil Conway".

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Color</span> Visual perception of the light spectrum

Color or colour is the visual perception based on the electromagnetic spectrum. Though color is not an inherent property of matter, color perception is related to an object's light absorption, reflection, emission spectra and interference. For most humans, colors are perceived in the visible light spectrum with three types of cone cells (trichromacy). Other animals may have a different number of cone cell types or have eyes sensitive to different wavelength, such as bees that can distinguish ultraviolet, and thus have a different color sensitivity range. Animal perception of color originates from different light wavelength or spectral sensitivity in cone cell types, which is then processed by the brain.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Visual cortex</span> Region of the brain that processes visual information

The visual cortex of the brain is the area of the cerebral cortex that processes visual information. It is located in the occipital lobe. Sensory input originating from the eyes travels through the lateral geniculate nucleus in the thalamus and then reaches the visual cortex. The area of the visual cortex that receives the sensory input from the lateral geniculate nucleus is the primary visual cortex, also known as visual area 1 (V1), Brodmann area 17, or the striate cortex. The extrastriate areas consist of visual areas 2, 3, 4, and 5.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Color constancy</span> How humans perceive color

Color constancy is an example of subjective constancy and a feature of the human color perception system which ensures that the perceived color of objects remains relatively constant under varying illumination conditions. A green apple for instance looks green to us at midday, when the main illumination is white sunlight, and also at sunset, when the main illumination is red. This helps us identify objects.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Color vision</span> Ability to perceive differences in light frequency

Color vision, a feature of visual perception, is an ability to perceive differences between light composed of different frequencies independently of light intensity.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Visual system</span> Body parts responsible for vision

The visual system is the physiological basis of visual perception. The system detects, transduces and interprets information concerning light within the visible range to construct an image and build a mental model of the surrounding environment. The visual system is associated with the eye and functionally divided into the optical system and the neural system.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lateral geniculate nucleus</span> Component of the visual system in the brains thalamus

In neuroanatomy, the lateral geniculate nucleus is a structure in the thalamus and a key component of the mammalian visual pathway. It is a small, ovoid, ventral projection of the thalamus where the thalamus connects with the optic nerve. There are two LGNs, one on the left and another on the right side of the thalamus. In humans, both LGNs have six layers of neurons alternating with optic fibers.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parietal lobe</span> Part of the brain responsible for sensory input and some language processing

The parietal lobe is one of the four major lobes of the cerebral cortex in the brain of mammals. The parietal lobe is positioned above the temporal lobe and behind the frontal lobe and central sulcus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neuroesthetics</span> Sub-discipline of empirical aesthetics

Neuroesthetics is a relatively recent sub-discipline of applied aesthetics. Empirical aesthetics takes a scientific approach to the study of aesthetic experience of art, music, or any object that can give rise to aesthetic judgments. Neuroesthetics is a term coined by Semir Zeki in 1999 and received its formal definition in 2002 as the scientific study of the neural bases for the contemplation and creation of a work of art. Neuroesthetics uses neuroscience to explain and understand the aesthetic experiences at the neurological level. The topic attracts scholars from many disciplines including neuroscientists, art historians, artists, art therapists and psychologists.

Visual processing is a term that is used to refer to the brain's ability to use and interpret visual information from the world around us. The process of converting light energy into a meaningful image is a complex process that is facilitated by numerous brain structures and higher level cognitive processes. On an anatomical level, light energy first enters the eye through the cornea, where the light is bent. After passing through the cornea, light passes through the pupil and then lens of the eye, where it is bent to a greater degree and focused upon the retina. The retina is where a group of light-sensing cells, called photoreceptors are located. There are two types of photoreceptors: rods and cones. Rods are sensitive to dim light and cones are better able to transduce bright light. Photoreceptors connect to bipolar cells, which induce action potentials in retinal ganglion cells. These retinal ganglion cells form a bundle at the optic disc, which is a part of the optic nerve. The two optic nerves from each eye meet at the optic chiasm, where nerve fibers from each nasal retina cross which results in the right half of each eye's visual field being represented in the left hemisphere and the left half of each eye's visual fields being represented in the right hemisphere. The optic tract then diverges into two visual pathways, the geniculostriate pathway and the tectopulvinar pathway, which send visual information to the visual cortex of the occipital lobe for higher level processing.

The opponent process is a color theory that states that the human visual system interprets information about color by processing signals from photoreceptor cells in an antagonistic manner. The opponent-process theory suggests that there are three opponent channels, each comprising an opposing color pair: red versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white (luminance). The theory was first proposed in 1892 by the German physiologist Ewald Hering.

Multisensory integration, also known as multimodal integration, is the study of how information from the different sensory modalities may be integrated by the nervous system. A coherent representation of objects combining modalities enables animals to have meaningful perceptual experiences. Indeed, multisensory integration is central to adaptive behavior because it allows animals to perceive a world of coherent perceptual entities. Multisensory integration also deals with how different sensory modalities interact with one another and alter each other's processing.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Inferior temporal gyrus</span> One of three gyri of the temporal lobe of the brain

The inferior temporal gyrus is one of three gyri of the temporal lobe and is located below the middle temporal gyrus, connected behind with the inferior occipital gyrus; it also extends around the infero-lateral border on to the inferior surface of the temporal lobe, where it is limited by the inferior sulcus. This region is one of the higher levels of the ventral stream of visual processing, associated with the representation of objects, places, faces, and colors. It may also be involved in face perception, and in the recognition of numbers and words.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Colour centre</span> Brain region responsible for colour processing

The colour centre is a region in the brain primarily responsible for visual perception and cortical processing of colour signals received by the eye, which ultimately results in colour vision. The colour centre in humans is thought to be located in the ventral occipital lobe as part of the visual system, in addition to other areas responsible for recognizing and processing specific visual stimuli, such as faces, words, and objects. Many functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in both humans and macaque monkeys have shown colour stimuli to activate multiple areas in the brain, including the fusiform gyrus and the lingual gyrus. These areas, as well as others identified as having a role in colour vision processing, are collectively labelled visual area 4 (V4). The exact mechanisms, location, and function of V4 are still being investigated.

In cognitive neuroscience, visual modularity is an organizational concept concerning how vision works. The way in which the primate visual system operates is currently under intense scientific scrutiny. One dominant thesis is that different properties of the visual world require different computational solutions which are implemented in anatomically/functionally distinct regions that operate independently – that is, in a modular fashion.

Visual perception is the ability to interpret the surrounding environment through photopic vision, color vision, scotopic vision, and mesopic vision, using light in the visible spectrum reflected by objects in the environment. This is different from visual acuity, which refers to how clearly a person sees. A person can have problems with visual perceptual processing even if they have 20/20 vision.

Globs are millimeter-sized color modules found beyond the visual area V2 in the brain's color processing ventral pathway. They are scattered throughout the posterior inferior temporal cortex in an area called the V4 complex. They are clustered by color preference, and organized as color columns. They are the first part of the brain in which color is processed in terms of the full range of hues found in color space.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parasol cell</span>

A parasol cell, sometimes called an M cell or M ganglion cell, is one type of retinal ganglion cell (RGC) located in the ganglion cell layer of the retina. These cells project to magnocellular cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) as part of the magnocellular pathway in the visual system. They have large cell bodies as well as extensive branching dendrite networks and as such have large receptive fields. Relative to other RGCs, they have fast conduction velocities. While they do show clear center-surround antagonism, they receive no information about color. Parasol ganglion cells contribute information about the motion and depth of objects to the visual system.

Ralph Mitchell Siegel, a researcher who studied the neurological underpinnings of vision, was a professor of neuroscience at Rutgers University, Newark, in the Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience. He died September 2, 2011, at his home following a long illness.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Russell L. De Valois</span>

Russell L. De Valois was an American scientist recognized for his pioneering research on spatial and color vision.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Doris Tsao</span> American neuroscientist

Doris Ying Tsao is an American systems neuroscientist and professor of biology at the University of California, Berkeley. She was formerly on the faculty at the California Institute of Technology. She is recognized for pioneering the use of fMRI with single-unit electrophysiological recordings and for discovering the macaque face patch system for face perception. She is a Howard Hughes Medical Institute Investigator and the director of the T&C Chen Center for Systems Neuroscience. She won a MacArthur "Genius" fellowship in 2018. Tsao was elected a member of the National Academy of Sciences in 2020.