Genetic Studies of Genius

Last updated

The Genetic Studies of Genius, later known as the Terman Study of the Gifted, [1] is currently the oldest and longest-running longitudinal study in the field of psychology. It was begun by Lewis Terman at Stanford University in 1921 to examine the development and characteristics of gifted children into adulthood. [1] :xi [2]

Contents

The results from the study have been published in five books, [3] [4] [5] [6] [1] a monograph, [7] and dozens of articles. A related retrospective study of eminent men in history by Catharine Cox, though not part of the longitudinal study, was published as part of the Genetic Studies of Genius. [8] It further inspired the ongoing Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth longitudinal study.

Origin

Lewis Terman, founder of the Genetic Studies of Genius Lewis Madison Terman.jpg
Lewis Terman, founder of the Genetic Studies of Genius

Terman had previously performed studies in intelligence, including his doctoral dissertation. [9] In 1916, he adapted Alfred Binet's intelligence test for the United States and expanded its range. The result was the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, which are still in use today (in an updated form). After his service in developing the Army Alpha during World War I, Terman returned to Stanford in order to start his study.

Terman had already found some bright children through his earlier research, and some of these were part of the sample in the Genetic Studies of Genius. [10] He hired several assistants, including Florence Goodenough and Catharine Cox, to search the public schools of California for similarly gifted children. Terman initially hoped to find the 1,000 most intelligent children, [11] but eventually found 1,444. [12] However, Terman gradually added subjects to the study through 1928 until there were 1,528 (856 males and 672 females). [1] :12 Not all subjects were discovered with the Stanford–Binet. Some were selected for the study with the National Intelligence Tests and the Army Alpha. The study subjects were born between 1900 and 1925, all lived in California, were 95–99% white, [13] and the majority came from upper- or middle-class families. [1] :11–14

Early findings

Terman's goal was to disprove the then-current belief that gifted children were sickly, socially inept, and not well-rounded. Therefore, the first volume of the study reported data on the children's family, [14] educational progress, [15] special abilities, [16] interests, [17] play, [18] and personality. [19] He also examined the children's racial and ethnic heritage. [20] Terman was a proponent of eugenics, although not as radical as many of his contemporary Social Darwinists, and believed that intelligence testing could be used as a positive tool to shape society. [2]

Based on data collected in 1921–22, Terman concluded that gifted children suffered no more health problems than normal for their age, save a little more myopia than average. He also found that the children were usually social, were well-adjusted, did better in school, and were even taller than average. [21] A follow-up performed in 1923–1924 found that the children had maintained their high IQs and were still above average overall. Data collected in the 1920s, also including a pioneering effort to implement above-level testing on a large scale, a practice that is widespread in gifted education today. [10]

Follow-ups

Terman planned later follow-ups, and in his lifetime data would be collected in 1928, 1936, 1940, 1945, 1950, and 1955. At his death, the study was directed by Melita Oden, who collected additional data in 1960. Robert Richardson Sears later took charge of the study and collected data in 1972, 1977, 1982, and 1986. [1] :18–24 Moreover, many study participants corresponded with Terman or visited Stanford University in order to keep him updated on their lives. [1] :275–276

According to those who have access to the study archives, the files also include news articles, letters, and other documents related to the study participants. [22] The later follow-ups asked questions about war service, college education, marital status and happiness, work, retirement, raising children, and other lifetime events and concerns. [23]

Well over half of men and women in Terman's study finished college, compared to 8% of the general population at the time. [24] Some of Terman's subjects reached great prominence in their fields. Among them were head I Love Lucy writer Jess Oppenheimer, [25] American Psychological Association president and educational psychologist Lee Cronbach, [26] Ancel Keys, [27] and Robert Sears himself. [25] Over fifty men became college and university faculty members. [28] However, the majority of study participants' lives were more mundane. By the 4th volume of Genetic Studies of Genius, Terman had noted that as adults, his subjects pursued common occupations "as humble as those of policeman, seaman, typist and filing clerk" [29] and concluded:

At any rate, we have seen that intellect and achievement are far from perfectly correlated. [30]

Criticism

The study has been criticized for not having a generalizable sample. [1] :11 [31] Moreover, Terman meddled in his subjects' lives, giving them letters of recommendation for jobs and college and pulling strings at Stanford to help them get admitted. [2] [22] This makes the life outcomes of the sample biased and difficult to generalize. [2]

In his book Fads and Foibles in Modern Sociology and Related Sciences (p. 70–76), sociologist Pitirim Sorokin criticized the research, saying that Terman's selected group of children with high IQs did about as well as a random group of children selected from similar family backgrounds would have done. [32]

As in any longitudinal study, it is possible that the characteristics and behaviors of the sample are a partial result of the era in which they lived. Indeed, many members of the sample could not attend college, due to the Great Depression and World War II. [1] :45 Almost half of women in the sample were homemakers for most of their lives. [1] :87 Despite these shortcomings, the data from the sample is often used for studies because there is no other group of people who have been followed for so long.

Today

As time has passed, the sample has dwindled. As of 2003, there were over 200 members of the sample still alive. [33] The study is to continue until the final member of the sample either withdraws or dies. [2] In the 21st century, researchers continue to publish articles based on the data collected in the Genetic Studies of Genius. For example, one article found that men who skipped a grade in childhood had higher incomes throughout adulthood. [34]

See also

Related Research Articles

Genius is a characteristic of original and exceptional insight in the performance of some art or endeavor that surpasses expectations, sets new standards for the future, establishes better methods of operation, or remains outside the capabilities of competitors. Genius is associated with intellectual ability and creative productivity. The term genius can also be used to refer to people characterised by genius, and/or to polymaths who excel across many subjects.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Intelligence quotient</span> Score from a test designed to assess intelligence

An intelligence quotient (IQ) is a total score derived from a set of standardised tests or subtests designed to assess human intelligence. The abbreviation "IQ" was coined by the psychologist William Stern for the German term Intelligenzquotient, his term for a scoring method for intelligence tests at University of Breslau he advocated in a 1912 book.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">William Shockley</span> American physicist, inventor, and eugenicist (1910–1989)

William Bradford Shockley Jr. was an American inventor, physicist, and eugenicist. He was the manager of a research group at Bell Labs that included John Bardeen and Walter Brattain. The three scientists were jointly awarded the 1956 Nobel Prize in Physics for "their researches on semiconductors and their discovery of the transistor effect".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alfred Binet</span> French psychologist, IQ test developer (1857–1911)

Alfred Binet, born Alfredo Binetti, was a French psychologist who invented the first practical IQ test, the Binet–Simon test. In 1904, the French Ministry of Education asked psychologist Alfred Binet to devise a method that would determine which students did not learn effectively from regular classroom instruction so they could be given remedial work. Along with his collaborator Théodore Simon, Binet published revisions of his test in 1908 and 1911, the last of which appeared just before his death.

The Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales is an individually administered intelligence test that was revised from the original Binet–Simon Scale by Alfred Binet and Théodore Simon. It is in its fifth edition (SB5), which was released in 2003.

Intellectual giftedness is an intellectual ability significantly higher than average. It is a characteristic of children, variously defined, that motivates differences in school programming. It is thought to persist as a trait into adult life, with various consequences studied in longitudinal studies of giftedness over the last century. These consequences sometimes include stigmatizing and social exclusion. There is no generally agreed definition of giftedness for either children or adults, but most school placement decisions and most longitudinal studies over the course of individual lives have followed people with IQs in the top 2.5 percent of the population—that is, IQs above 130. Definitions of giftedness also vary across cultures.

Gifted education is a sort of education used for children who have been identified as gifted and talented.

The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is an IQ test designed to measure intelligence and cognitive ability in adults and older adolescents.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lewis Terman</span> American educational psychologist, academic, and eugenicist (1877–1956)

Lewis Madison Terman was an American psychologist, academic, and proponent of eugenics. He was noted as a pioneer in educational psychology in the early 20th century at the Stanford School of Education. Terman is best known for his revision of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales and for initiating the longitudinal study of children with high IQs called the Genetic Studies of Genius. As a prominent eugenicist, he was a member of the Human Betterment Foundation, the American Eugenics Society, and the Eugenics Research Association. He also served as president of the American Psychological Association. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Terman as the 72nd most cited psychologist of the 20th century, in a tie with G. Stanley Hall.

Catharine Morris Cox Miles was an American psychologist known for her work on intelligence and genius. Born in San Jose, CA, to Lydia Shipley Bean and Charles Elwood Cox. In 1927 married psychologist Walter Richard Miles. Her sister was classics scholar and Quaker administrator Anna Cox Brinton.

A high-IQ society is an organization that limits its membership to people who have attained a specified score on an IQ test, usually in the top two percent of the population or above. These may also be referred to as genius societies. The largest and oldest such society is Mensa International, which was founded by Roland Berrill and Lancelot Ware in 1946.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mental age</span> Concept relating to intelligence

Mental age is a concept related to intelligence. It looks at how a specific individual, at a specific age, performs intellectually, compared to average intellectual performance for that individual's actual chronological age (i.e. time elapsed since birth). The intellectual performance is based on performance in tests and live assessments by a psychologist. The score achieved by the individual is compared to the median average scores at various ages, and the mental age (x, say) is derived such that the individual's score equates to the average score at age x.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Florence Goodenough</span> American psychologist

Florence Laura Goodenough was an American psychologist and professor at the University of Minnesota who studied child intelligence and various problems in the field of child development. She was president of the Society for Research in Child Development from 1946-1947. She is best known for published book The Measurement of Intelligence, where she introduced the Goodenough Draw-A-Man test to assess intelligence in young children through nonverbal measurement. She is noted for developing the Minnesota Preschool Scale. In 1931 she published two notable books titled Experimental Child Study and Anger in Young Children which analyzed the methods used in evaluating children. She wrote the Handbook of Child Psychology in 1933, becoming the first known psychologist to critique ratio I.Q.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">IQ classification</span> Categorisation of peoples intelligence based on IQ

IQ classification is the practice of categorizing human intelligence, as measured by intelligence quotient (IQ) tests, into categories such as "superior" or "average".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Leta Stetter Hollingworth</span> American psychologist

Leta Stetter Hollingworth was an American psychologist, educator, and feminist. Hollingworth also made contributions in psychology of women, clinical psychology, and educational psychology. She is best known for her work with gifted children.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Robert Richardson Sears</span>

Robert Richardson Sears was an American psychologist who specialized in child psychology and the psychology of personality. He was the head of the psychology department at Stanford and later dean of the School of Humanities and Sciences there, continued the long-term I.Q. studies of Lewis Madison Terman at Stanford, and authored many pivotal papers and books on various aspects of psychology.

Quinn Michael McNemar was an American psychologist and statistician. He is known for his work on IQ tests, for his book Psychological Statistics (1949) and for McNemar's test, the statistical test he introduced in 1947.

The McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (MSCA) is a psychological test given to young children. "the McCarthy scales present a carefully constructed individual test of human ability."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Maud A. Merrill</span> American psychologist (1888–1978)

Maud Amanda Merrill was an American psychologist. Both an alumna and faculty member of Stanford University, Merrill worked with Lewis Terman to develop the second and third editions of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales.

Barbara Stoddard Burks was an American psychologist known for her research on the nature-nurture debate as it pertained to intelligence and other human traits. She has been credited with "...pioneer[ing] the statistical techniques which continue to ground the trenchant nature/nurture debates about intelligence in American psychology."

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Holahan, C. K., & Sears, R. R. (1995) The Gifted Group in Later Maturity. Stanford University Press: Stanford, California.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 Leslie, Mitchell (2000). "The vexing legacy of Lewis Terman". Stanford Magazine. Archived from the original on 2011-06-10.
  3. Terman, Lewis M. (1925). Mental and Physical Traits of a Thousand Gifted Children. Genetic Studies of Genius, Volume 1. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Retrieved 2 June 2013.
  4. Burks, Barbara S.; Jensen, Dortha W.; Terman, Lewis M. (1930). The Promise of Youth: Follow-up Studies of a Thousand Gifted Children. Genetic Studies of Genius, Volume 3. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.
  5. Terman, Lewis M.; Oden, Melita (1947). The Gifted Child Grows Up: Twenty-five Years' Follow-up of a Superior Group (PDF). Genetic Studies of Genius, Volume 4. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.
  6. Terman, Lewis M.; Oden, Melita (1959). The Gifted Group at Mid-Life: Thirty-Five Years' Follow-Up of the Superior Child. Genetic Studies of Genius, Volume V. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Retrieved 2 June 2013.
  7. Oden, M. L. (1968). "The fulfillment of promise: 40-year follow-up of the Terman gifted group" (PDF). Genetic Psychology Monographs. 77 (1): 3–93. PMID   5640199 via gwern.net.
  8. Cox, Catherine M. (1926). The Early Mental Traits of 300 Geniuses (PDF). Genetic Studies of Genius, Volume 2. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.
  9. Terman, L. M. (1906). "Genius and stupidity: A study of some of the intellectual processes of seven "bright" and seven "stupid" boys". Pedagogical Seminary. 13 (3): 307–373. doi:10.1080/08919402.1906.10534367.
  10. 1 2 Warne, Russell T. (2012). "History and development of above-level testing of the gifted" (PDF). Roeper Review. 34 (3): 183–193. doi:10.1080/02783193.2012.686425. S2CID   144849573.
  11. Terman, 1930, p. 19.
  12. Terman, 1930, p. 39
  13. Warne, Russell T. (2019). "An evaluation (and vindication?) of Lewis Terman: What the father of gifted education can teach the 21st century" (PDF). Gifted Child Quarterly. 63: 3–21. doi:10.1177/0016986218799433. S2CID   149629495.
  14. Terman, 1926, p. 135–252
  15. Terman, 1926, p. 253–306
  16. Terman, 1926, p. 307–362
  17. Terman, 1926, p. 363–384, 441–484
  18. Terman, 1926, p. 385–440
  19. Terman, 1926, p. 485–556
  20. Terman, 1926, p. 55–112
  21. Terman, 1926
  22. 1 2 Shurkin, Joel (1992). Terman's Kids: The Groundbreaking Study of How the Gifted Grow Up . Boston, MA: Little, Brown. ISBN   978-0-316-78890-8.
  23. Holahan & Sears, 1995
  24. Herrnstein, Richard J. (1971). "I.Q." (PDF). The Atlantic. 228 (3): 52.
  25. 1 2 Leslie, 2000
  26. Shavelson, R. J.; Gleser, G. (2002). "Lee J. Cronbach (1916–2001) Obituary" (PDF). American Psychologist. 57 (5): 360–361. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.57.5.360.
  27. Shurkin, 1992
  28. Oden, 1968, p. 17.
  29. Jenkins-Friedman, Reva (1982). "Myth: Cosmetic use of multiple selection criteria!". Gifted Child Quarterly. 26 (1): 24–26. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.848.8845 . doi:10.1177/001698628202600108. S2CID   145412145.
  30. Terman & Oden 1947, page 352
  31. Keating, D. P. (1975). "Possible sampling bias in Genetic Studies of Genius" (PDF). Educational and Psychological Measurement. 35 (3): 657–662. doi:10.1177/001316447503500313. S2CID   144335870.
  32. Gladwell, Malcolm (2008). Outliers. New York. p. 90. ISBN   978-0-316-03669-6.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  33. Christmann, E. P.; Badgett, J. L. (2008). Interpreting assessment data. NTSA Press. doi:10.2505/9781933531366. ISBN   978-1-93353-136-6.
  34. Warne, Russell T.; Liu, Jonathan K (2017). "Income differences among grade skippers and non-grade skippers across genders in the Terman sample, 1936–1976" (PDF). Learning and Instruction. 47: 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.10.004 .

Further reading