Hierarchical structure of the Big Five

Last updated

Within personality psychology, it has become common practice to use factor analysis to derive personality traits. The Big Five model proposes that there are five basic personality traits. These traits were derived in accordance with the lexical hypothesis. [1] These five personality traits: Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness to Experience have garnered widespread support [ dubious ].

Contents

The Big Five personality characteristics represent one level in a hierarchy of traits. These traits can be subdivided into collections of aspects or facets which are related to each other but are not identical. As the sub-level of a hierarchy, these traits can be said to be made up of these aspects or facets. The Big Five traits can also be combined into higher order factors consisting of two or more traits. These superordinate factors and subcomponents and the approaches used to devise them are discussed below.

Subcomponents of the Big Five

Aspects (Big Five Aspect Scale)

The Big Five Aspect Scale contains 100 items and is designed to measure the Big Five personality characteristics both at the trait and aspect level. The aspect level is described as being "a level of trait organization located between facets and domains". [2] Previous research on this topic had focused on a two-leveled hierarchy consisting of traits and facets. The authors of the article "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five" argued that there was evidence for an intermediary level between the Big Five and its constituent facets. The authors also cite previous research suggesting two factor solutions subsumed by each of the Big Five personality Traits. [2]

The Big Five Aspect Scale measures the following aspects:

NeuroticismAgreeablenessConscientiousnessExtraversionOpenness/Intellect
Volatility Compassion IndustriousnessEnthusiasmOpenness
Withdrawal Politeness OrderlinessAssertivenessIntellect

Justification

In addition to statistical evidence presented by the creators of this scale which indicates that a two-aspect solution is appropriate for each Big Five trait, [2] there is genetic evidence for the existence of two aspects within each of the Big Five domains including separate biological correlates. [3] This evidence supports the idea that each trait can be broken down into exactly two distinct subcomponents, and it is this evidence that the authors cite to show that a two aspect sub-level for each trait "may represent more than mere coincidence or desire for parsimony". [2]

Facets (NEO-PI-R)

One of the earliest facet scales is the NEO-PI-R. [4] This scale consists of 240 questions which are designed to measure not only the Big Five personality traits, which are referred to as "domains," but also their constituent facets where "facet" refers to any personality characteristic of a lower (narrower) order than a domain. This scale is based on a "top-down approach" in which traits are viewed as broad themes that occur in personality measures. In this view traits are composed of facets, however they are not derived from lower order personality characteristics. [5]

The approach attempts to designate facets of each domain which

Justification

The authors of the NEO-PI-R cite a number of reasons justifying their top-down approach. The lexical approach (bottom up) is limited for two reasons, they claim. First, not all personality characteristics are well represented in natural language. [6] Second, personality characteristics occur at various levels of breadth, from narrow to wide, with wider characteristics taking up the majority of variance in factor analyses. These wider items tend to correlate closely with a number of narrower items while narrower items may not vary at all with each other.

A top-down approach helps to overcome both of these limitations. Items can be selected specifically to represent characteristics that are not as well represented in natural language. In addition, it allows one to develop an ordered hierarchy of items which are not necessarily related to each other "horizontally" (among items of similar breadth), by looking at their shared relationships to higher order domains or traits.

Facets (Abridged Big Five Dimensional Circumplex (AB5C) Approach)

This AB5C approach to the classification of trait adjectives uses what is called a circumplex approach. In contrast to purely hierarchical models which seek to break Big Five personality traits into aspects and facets which exist within each domain, the circumplex approach views personality characteristics as existing in multidimensional space, each dimension being represented by one of the big five domains. [7] The AB5C uses an abridged version of this approach. Rather than representing personality characteristics in a full five dimensions, they partition this space into "subsets" which are the 10 two domain combinations that can be formed by the Big Five. Characteristics are then classified according to which subset they are most strongly associated with and placed within this two-dimensional space. This model has been described as being an approach to Big Five subcomponents which is "horizontal" rather than "vertical" or hierarchical. [8]

Justification

The justification for circumplex models, which are characterized by the "multidimensional" approach mentioned above, is that they are better able to identify clusters of semantically related characteristics. [7] Although the Big Five model covers a broader range of personality trait space, it is less able to make these sorts of distinctions. This is because "trait descriptors do not fit perfectly into simple structure models". [7]

The AB5C produces different clusters of adjectives than the purely hierarchical approaches mentioned above. One proposed explanation of this is that the "external" and "internal" structure of these adjectives differ. In this case, the external structure is derived from the ratings of participants from questionnaires. Internal structure refers to the strict semantic relationships of these adjectives (as taken from dictionaries and other references). When looking at antonyms, it is often found that a word and its opposite are not in directly opposing sections of the theoretical word-space of the AB5C. [7]

Superordinate factors

In addition to levels of trait adjectives which are arranged in levels subordinate to the Big Five, there are also higher order solutions. In this case higher order solutions refer to combinations of Big Five factors which are shown to combine into "meta-traits". John Digman argued that the apparent orthogonality of the Big Five was largely due to the nature of the analysis techniques used to derive it rather than any inherent orthogonality. [9]

Alpha and beta

Through an exploratory analysis of the Big Five literature, John Digman came up with two superordinate factors on a higher level than the Big Five. He referred to these factors as "alpha" and "beta". Alpha refers to a combination of agreeableness, conscientiousness and emotional stability (the inverse of neuroticism). Beta is a combination of extraversion and intellect/openness.

Digman suggests that Alpha may represent a social desirability factor. An alternative explanation is that Alpha is a socialization factor which is causally related to the positive (or negative) expression of these socially desirable traits. Beta, he says, may be interpreted as personal growth versus restriction. An alternative explanation offered by Digman are "communion" and "agency" for alpha and beta respectively. [9] Here agency refers to "strivings for mastery, power, self-assertion, and self-expansion" and communion to "the urge toward community and the relinquishing of individuality". [9]

Plasticity and stability

Colin DeYoung and colleagues have suggested that the Alpha and Beta factors might be better interpreted as "stability" and "plasticity". [10]

He asserts that agreeableness, conscientiousness and emotional stability, the traits constituting the Alpha-factor above, are really measures of a personalities' "stability" dimension. The higher order factor stability expresses a person's general ability to maintain stable relationships, motivation and emotional states respectively. He offers that this general ability may be linked to the rostral serotonergic system which in turn has been linked to emotion and motivation regulation. [11] Reductions in a serotonin variant in the region of the midbrain associated with this system have been linked to "aggressiveness (low Agreeableness) and impulsiveness (low Conscientiousness and low Emotional Stability)" [10]

In turn, DeYoung characterizes Beta as a combination of positive affect (extraversion) and a general openness to exploration and experience (openness to experience). He combines these two characteristics into the more general "cognitive flexibility" which he calls plasticity. [10] He hypothesizes links to the central dopaminergic (DA) system to which especially Extraversion had commonly been linked. [12] In addition, extraversion and openness to experience are both linked to latent inhibition reduction [10] which is moderated by it. [13]

These underlying biological correlates combined with DeYoung's conceptual arguments form the foundation of his case for "stability" and "plasticity" as labels and interpretations of Digman's Alpha and Beta factors.

General factor of personality

It has been proposed that in addition to two superordinate factors existing "above" the Big Five, that there is a higher order single factor solution. Where the two-factor superordinates are referred to as "2nd order" factors. This single factor is referred to as a "3rd order" factor, suggesting that it occupies the top of a hierarchy that can be subdivided first into two factors which can themselves be subdivided into the Big Five for a total of three levels. [14] Proposed by Janek Musek as a blend of all socially desirable personality dimensions and referred to as "the Big One" or a "general factor of personality", [15] evidence for this third order factor exists across a number of research studies and personality scales. [16] [17] Musek argued that the general factor of personality is related to self-esteem, life satisfaction, emotionality (high positive affect and low negative affect vs. low positive and high negative affect), subjective well-being and motivation (high approach and low avoidance motivation vs. low approach and high avoidance motivation). [15]

A 2014 study of 286 undergraduate students found that "[a] composite GFP and composite measure of social-effectiveness exhibited a strong association and continued to share over 50% of their variance after controlling for socially-desirable responding." [18]

A 2010 meta-analysis of 144,117 participants found that "GFP has a substantive component as it is related to supervisor-rated job performance." Furthermore, they found positive GFP loadings on all Big Five traits except neuroticism. [19] A 2022 meta-analysis of personality and intelligence correlations (n = 55,169) found that the GFP correlated r = .06 with general intelligence. [20]

In a 2008 study using monozygotic and dizygotic twins, genetic evidence was given for a general factor of personality. [17] This work has indicated that there may be an underlying genetic component for Musek's blend of all socially desirable personality dimensions. Rushton conjectured that this highest-level personality dimension represents human evolutionary development toward "more efficient persons—those who are more level-headed, agreeable, friendly, dependable, and open." [17]

Criticism of higher-order factors

Proponents of the Big Five and the HEXACO model of personality structure respectively have argued that the dimensions of these models are orthogonal and irreducible and that Digman's two higher-order factors are not valid. [21] [22] They argued that intercorrelations between personality factors of the Big Five and the HEXACO model can be explained due to lower order traits that represent blends of otherwise orthogonal factors, and that postulating higher-order factors is unnecessary. For example, interpersonal warmth blends both extraversion and agreeableness. Costa and McCrae pointed out that in an analysis of three different personality scales designed to assess five factor model traits, Digman's two-factor solution could not be replicated across these instruments. [21] For example, in two of the scales analysed, conscientiousness loaded more strongly with extraversion and openness to experience on one factor, than with neuroticism and agreeableness on the other factor. On an adjective rating scale, openness to experience loaded nearly equally on both factors. Five factor solutions on the other hand were replicated across all three scales.

The use of factor analysis to derive verbal descriptors of human characteristics of mixed origins (biologically- and socially-based) was criticised due to the linearity of used correlations that factors are based on and for factors' independence. [23] [ dead link ] [24] [25]

A number of published studies have also argued against the existence of a general factor of personality. [26] [27] [28] For example, Muncer [26] critiqued the study by Rushton and Irwing [16] that had claimed to find a general factor of personality based on a new analysis of Digman's data. Muncer argued that Rushton and Irwing's meta-analysis was unreliable due to heterogeneous correlations between the Big Five factors analysed. Furthermore, the extent of such heterogeneity is strong evidence against the existence of such a general factor.

More importantly, Muncer argued, evolutionary theory does not support the existence of a general factor of personality. Evolutionary theory proposes that organisms survive due to the goodness of fit between their characteristics and their environment. Humans have flourished in a diverse range of environments, yet Rushton et al.'s theory proposes that high levels of a single personality dimension have been adaptive throughout all of human evolution which would require a constant environment throughout this evolutionary period. In contrast, many other evolutionary theorists have proposed that environmental heterogeneity actually supports diversity in traits, because given traits may be adaptive in some environments and not in others.

Furthermore, there is a lack of evidence that high levels of a general factor of personality would necessarily confer reproductive advantages. For example, people with antisocial personality traits (hence a low general factor of personality) may have a higher than average number of sexual partners. [26] Proponents of the GFP like Rushton would likely object to this by pointing at recent discontinuities in the human fitness landscape in particular. But then, further, evolutionary theory suggests that sex differences in personality traits may enhance reproductive success for each sex. Cross-cultural studies have, however, found that women tend to be higher than men on both neuroticism and agreeableness, even though selection for a unitary general factor would mean that high agreeableness would be associated with low neuroticism. Therefore, the existence of a unitary aggregate personality factor may appear to be problematic from an evolutionary perspective. [26] On the other hand, this could be accounted for by means of the dual constraints of the aforementioned sexual selection on the one and of especially child-morality as a function of neuroticism on the other hand.

Related Research Articles

Personality is any person's collection of interrelated behavioral, cognitive and emotional patterns that comprise a person’s unique adjustment to life. These interrelated patterns are relatively stable, but can change over long time periods.

In psychology, trait theory is an approach to the study of human personality. Trait theorists are primarily interested in the measurement of traits, which can be defined as habitual patterns of behavior, thought, and emotion. According to this perspective, traits are aspects of personality that are relatively stable over time, differ across individuals, are relatively consistent over situations, and influence behaviour. Traits are in contrast to states, which are more transitory dispositions.

Conscientiousness is the personality trait of being responsible, careful or diligent. Conscientiousness implies a desire to do a task well, and to take obligations to others seriously. Conscientious people tend to be efficient and organized as opposed to easy-going and disorderly. They tend to show self-discipline, act dutifully, and aim for achievement; they display planned rather than spontaneous behavior; and they are generally dependable. Conscientiousness manifests in characteristic behaviors such as being neat, systematic, careful, thorough, and deliberate.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Big Five personality traits</span> Personality model consisting of five broad dimensions

The Big Five personality traits, sometimes known as "the five-factor model of personality" or "OCEAN model", is a grouping of five unique characteristics used to study personality. It has been developed from the 1980s onward in psychological trait theory.

Psychoticism is one of the three traits used by the psychologist Hans Eysenck in his P–E–N model model of personality.

Neuroticism is a personality trait associated with negative emotions. It is one of the Big Five traits. Individuals with high scores on neuroticism are more likely than average to experience such feelings as anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, envy, jealousy, pessimism, guilt, depressed mood, and loneliness. Such people are thought to respond worse to stressors and are more likely to interpret ordinary situations, such as minor frustrations, as appearing hopelessly difficult. Their behavioral responses may include procrastination, substance use, and other maladaptive behaviors, which may aid in relieving negative emotions and generating positive ones.

Agreeableness is a personality trait referring to individuals that are perceived as kind, sympathetic, cooperative, warm, honest, and considerate. In personality psychology, agreeableness is one of the five major dimensions of personality structure, reflecting individual differences in cooperation and social harmony.

Openness to experience is one of the domains which are used to describe human personality in the Five Factor Model. Openness involves six facets, or dimensions: active imagination (fantasy), aesthetic sensitivity, attentiveness to inner feelings, preference for variety (adventurousness), intellectual curiosity, and challenging authority. A great deal of psychometric research has demonstrated that these facets or qualities are significantly correlated. Thus, openness can be viewed as a global personality trait consisting of a set of specific traits, habits, and tendencies that cluster together.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Extraversion and introversion</span> Personality trait

Extraversion and introversion are a central trait dimension in human personality theory. The terms were introduced into psychology by Carl Jung, though both the popular understanding and current psychological usage are not the same as Jung's original concept. Extraversion tends to be manifested in outgoing, talkative, energetic behavior, whereas introversion is manifested in more reflective and reserved behavior. Jung defined introversion as an "attitude-type characterised by orientation in life through subjective psychic contents", and extraversion as "an attitude-type characterised by concentration of interest on the external object".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">HEXACO model of personality structure</span> Six-dimensional model of human personality

The HEXACO model of personality structure is a six-dimensional model of human personality that was created by Ashton and Lee and explained in their book, The H Factor of Personality, based on findings from a series of lexical studies involving several European and Asian languages. The six factors, or dimensions, include honesty-humility (H), emotionality (E), extraversion (X), agreeableness (A), conscientiousness (C), and openness to experience (O). Each factor is composed of traits with characteristics indicating high and low levels of the factor. The HEXACO model was developed through similar methods as other trait taxonomies and builds on the work of Costa and McCrae and Goldberg. The model, therefore, shares several common elements with other trait models. However, the HEXACO model is unique mainly due to the addition of the honesty-humility dimension.

The Revised NEO Personality Inventory is a personality inventory that assesses an individual on five dimensions of personality. These are the same dimensions found in the Big Five personality traits. These traits are openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion(-introversion), agreeableness, and neuroticism. In addition, the NEO PI-R also reports on six subcategories of each Big Five personality trait.

The Big Five personality traits are Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. The Big Five Personality is a test that people can take to learn more about their personality in relation to the five personality traits. Cross-cultural psychology as a discipline examines the way that human behavior is different and/or similar across different cultures. One important and widely studied area in this subfield of psychology is personality, particularly the study of Big Five. The Big Five model of personality has become the most extensively studied model of personality and has broad support, starting in the United States and later in many different cultures. The Big Five model of personality started in the United States, and through the years has been translated into many different languages and has been used in many countries. Some researchers were attempting to determine the differences in how other cultures perceive this model. Some research shows that the Big Five holds up across cultures even with its origin in the English language. However, there is also some evidence which suggests that the Big Five traits may not be sufficient to completely explain personality in other cultures. In countries such as South America and East Asia, the results weren't as accurate because they weren't as open as some people in other countries are.

In psychology, a facet is a specific and unique aspect of a broader personality trait. Both the concept and the term "facet" were introduced by Paul Costa and Robert McCrae in the first edition of the NEO-Personality Inventory (NEO-PI) Manual. Facets were originally elaborated only for the neuroticism, openness to experience, and extraversion traits; Costa and McCrae introduced facet scales for the agreeableness and conscientiousness traits in the Revised NEO-PI. Each of the Big Five personality traits in the five factor model contains six facets, each of which is measured with a separate scale. The use of facets and facet scales has since expanded beyond the NEO PI-R, with alternative facet and domain structures derived from other models of personality. Examples include the HEXACO model of personality structure, psycholexical studies, circumplex models, the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ), and the California Psychological Inventory.

Colin G. DeYoung is an associate professor of psychology at the University of Minnesota. His research is in the field of personality psychology and personality neuroscience.

In personality pathology, dimensional models of personality disorders conceptualize personality disorders as qualitatively rather than quantitatively different from normal personality. They consist of extreme, maladaptive levels of certain personality characteristics. Within the context of personality psychology, a "dimension" refers to a continuum on which an individual can have various levels of a characteristic, in contrast to the dichotomous categorical approach in which an individual does or does not possess a characteristic. According to dimensional models personality disorders are classified according to which characteristics are expressed at which levels. This stands in contrast to the traditional categorical models of classification, which are based on the boolean presence or absence of symptoms and do not take into account levels of expression of a characteristic or the presence of any underlying dimension.

Most scientists agree that religiosity is not an independent personality trait, despite there being some commonality between their characteristics. Religiosity and personality traits both relate to one's feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. However, unlike for personality, one's level of religiosity is often measured by the presence or lack of belief in and relationship with a higher power, certain lifestyles or behaviors adopted for a higher power, and a sense of belonging with other followers of one's religion. Additionally, personality traits tend to follow a normal distribution, such that the majority of individuals' scores for a personality trait will be concentrated towards the middle, rather than being extremely high or low. Distributions for religiosity, however, follow a non-normal distribution, such that there are more individuals who score particularly high or low on religiosity scales.

The biological basis of personality it is the collection of brain systems and mechanisms that underlie human personality. Human neurobiology, especially as it relates to complex traits and behaviors, is not well understood, but research into the neuroanatomical and functional underpinnings of personality are an active field of research. Animal models of behavior, molecular biology, and brain imaging techniques have provided some insight into human personality, especially trait theories.

Intelligence and personality have traditionally been studied as separate entities in psychology, but more recent work has increasingly challenged this view. An increasing number of studies have recently explored the relationship between intelligence and personality, in particular the Big Five personality traits.

Personality neuroscience uses neuroscientific methods to study the neurobiological mechanisms underlying individual differences in stable psychological attributes. Specifically, personality neuroscience aims to investigate the relationships between inter-individual variation in brain structures as well as functions and behavioral measures of persistent psychological traits, broadly defined as "predispositions and average tendencies to be in particular states", including but are not limited to personality traits, sociobehavioral tendencies, and psychopathological risk factors. Personality neuroscience is considered as an interdisciplinary field integrating research questions and methodologies from social psychology, personality psychology, and neuroscience. It is closely related to other interdisciplinary fields, such as social, cognitive, and affective neuroscience.

References

  1. Osgood, Charles; May, W. H.; Miron, M. S. (1975), Cross-Cultural Universals of Affective Meaning, Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
  2. 1 2 3 4 DeYoung C. G.; Quilty L. C.; Peterson J. B (2007). "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880–896. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID   17983306. S2CID   8261816.
  3. Jang K. L.; Livesley W. J.; Angleitner A.; Reimann R.; Vernon P. A (2002). "Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 33: 83–101. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00137-4.
  4. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  5. 1 2 Costa P. T. Jr.; McCrae R. R (1995). "Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory". Journal of Personality Assessment. 64 (1): 21–50. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6401_2. PMID   16367732. S2CID   3065952.
  6. McCrae R. R. (1990). "Traits and trait names: How well is Openness represented in natural languages?". European Journal of Personality. 4 (2): 119–129. doi:10.1002/per.2410040205. S2CID   143886739.
  7. 1 2 3 4 Hofstee W. K. B.; de Raad B.; Goldberg L. R (1992). "Integration of the Big Five and circumplex approaches to trait structure". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63 (1): 146–163. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.146. PMID   1494982. S2CID   2622872.
  8. Goldberg, L.R. (1993). The structure of personality traits: Vertical and horizontal aspects. In D.C. Funder, R.D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying Lives through Time: Personality and Development (pp. 169-188). Washington DC: American Psychological Association
  9. 1 2 3 Digman John M (1997). "Higher-order factors of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 73 (6): 1246–1256. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1246. PMID   9418278.
  10. 1 2 3 4 DeYoung C. G.; Peterson J. B.; Higgins D. M (2002). "Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health?". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (4): 533–552. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00171-4.
  11. Meltzer H.Y. (1990). "Role of serotonin in depression". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 600 (1): 486–99. Bibcode:1990NYASA.600..486M. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb16904.x. PMID   2252328. S2CID   779213.
  12. Depue R.A.; Collins P.F (2000). "Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22 (3): 491–569. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99002046. PMID   11301519. S2CID   8217084.
  13. Gray J. A.; Moran P.M.; Grigoryan G.; Peters S.L.; Young A.M.J.; Joseph M.H (1997). "Latent inhibition: the nucleus accumbens connection revisited". Behavioural Brain Research. 88 (1): 27–34. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(97)02313-9. PMID   9401705. S2CID   19734897.
  14. J.P. Rushton, P. Irwing / Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 571–576
  15. 1 2 Musek J (2007). "A general factor of personality: Evidence for the big one in the five- factor model". Journal of Research in Personality. 41 (6): 1213–1233. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003.
  16. 1 2 Rushton J. P.; Irwing P. (2008). "A General Factor of Personality (GFP) from two meta-analyses of the Big Five: Digman (1997) and Mount, Barrick, Scullen, and Rounds (2005)". Personality and Individual Differences. 45: 679–683. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.015.
  17. 1 2 3 Rushton J. P.; Bons T. A.; Hur Y.-M. (2008). "The genetics and evolution of a General Factor of Personality". Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (5): 1173–1185. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.002.
  18. Dunkel, Curtis S.; Van Der Linden, Dimitri (2014). "Evidence for the general factor of personality as social-effectiveness". Personality and Individual Differences. 64: 147–151. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.030.
  19. Van Der Linden, Dimitri; Te Nijenhuis, Jan; Bakker, Arnold B. (2010). "The General Factor of Personality: A meta-analysis of Big Five intercorrelations and a criterion-related validity study". Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (3): 315–327. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.03.003.
  20. Anglim, Jeromy; Dunlop, Patrick D.; Wee, Serena; Horwood, Sharon; Wood, Joshua K.; Marty, Andrew (May 2022). "Personality and intelligence: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 148 (5–6): 301–336. doi:10.1037/bul0000373. hdl: 20.500.11937/89668 . ISSN   1939-1455. S2CID   253344751.
  21. 1 2 Paul T. Costa; Robert R. McRae (1992). "Reply to Eysenck". Personality and Individual Differences. 13 (8): 861–865. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90002-7.
  22. Ashton, M.C.; Lee, K.; Goldberg, L.R.; de Vries, R.E. (2009). "Higher order factors of personality: Do they exist?". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 13 (2): 79–91. doi:10.1177/1088868309338467. PMC   2810835 . PMID   19458345.
  23. Boyle, GJ (2008). "What's Critique of the five-factor model of personality". Humanities & Social Sciences Papers. 297.
  24. Trofimova, I.; Robbins, TW; W., Sulis; J., Uher (2018). "Taxonomies of psychological individual differences: biological perspectives on millennia-long challenges". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Biological Sciences. 373 (1744). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0152 . PMC   5832678 . PMID   29483338. S2CID   3718863.
  25. Trofimova, I; et al. (2022). "What's next for the neurobiology of temperament, personality and psychopathology?". Current Opinions in Behavioral Sciences. 45: 101143. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101143. S2CID   248817462.
  26. 1 2 3 4 Muncer, Steven J. (2011). "The general factor of personality: Evaluating the evidence from meta-analysis, confirmatory factor analysis and evolutionary theory". Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (6): 775–778. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.029.
  27. de Vries, R.E. (2011). "No evidence for a General Factor of Personality in the HEXACO Personality Inventory". Journal of Research in Personality. 45 (2): 229–232. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.12.002.
  28. Hopwood, C.J.; Wright, A.G.C.; Donnellan, M.B. (2011). "Evaluating the evidence for the general factor of personality across multiple inventories". Journal of Research in Personality. 45 (5): 468–478. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.06.002. PMC   3412523 . PMID   22879686.