Cognitive ecology

Last updated

Cognitive ecology is the study of cognitive phenomena within social and natural contexts. [1] It is an integrative perspective drawing from aspects of ecological psychology, cognitive science, evolutionary ecology and anthropology. Notions of domain-specific modules in the brain and the cognitive biases they create are central to understanding the enacted nature of cognition within a cognitive ecological framework. This means that cognitive mechanisms not only shape the characteristics of thought, but they dictate the success of culturally transmitted ideas. Because culturally transmitted concepts can often inform ecological decision-making behaviors, group-level trends in cognition (i.e., culturally salient concepts) are hypothesized to address ecologically relevant challenges. [2]

Contents

Theoretical basis

Cognitive ecology explores the interactive relationship between organism-environment interactions and its impact on cognitive phenomena. [1] Human cognition in this framework is multimodal and viewed similarly to enactivist perspectives on cognitive processing. For cultural concepts, this emphasizes cognitive distribution across an ecosystem, which is predicated on models of the extended mind thesis.

Ecological psychology

While the multi-faceted nature of cognitive ecology is a consequence of its interdisciplinary history, it primarily derives from early work in ecological psychology. Paradigm shifts from behaviorist orientations of psychology to cognition, or the "cognitive revolution", [3] [4] gave rise to the ecological psychology approach, which distanced itself from mainstream cognitivist views by breaking down the common mind-environment dichotomy of psychological theory. [5]

One particularly influential progenitor of this work was ecological psychologist James Gibson, whose legacy is marked by his ideas on ecological and social affordances. These are the opportunistic features of environmental objects that can be exploited for human use, and are therefore particularly perceptible (e.g., a knob affords twisting, an agreeable social cue affords a warm reaction). [6] Gibson argued further that organisms cannot be disentangled from their environments, and that their cognitive constraints were consequences of a limited set of environmental invariants which shaped them over evolutionary time. [5] [7] An illustrative example for Gibson is the human capacity for three-dimensional visual perception, which he argues is a cognitive concept resulting from the way that people interact with their environment. [8]

Another foreshadowed element of cognitive ecological theory comes from ecological anthropologist Gregory Bateson, who considered the notion of informational feedback loops between mind and environment, particularly their role in generating meaning and awareness of one's surroundings. In an essay, he speculates on how an observer might best delineate the "self" of a blind man. In his treatment, he questions whether one may arbitrarily choose to carve out the man's informational processing loop at his brain or his hands or his walking stick without offering an incomplete view of his cognitive process. [9] This discussion of concept remains influential in modern cognitive ecological considerations of the densely interconnected elements of ecology that play relevant roles in cognition. [1]

Enactivism

An enactive perspective of cognition is fundamental to a cognitive ecological view. [10] Rather than a passive interpretation of internally represented information, cognition is considered to be an active process involving the transformation of information into meaningful relationships between the organism and its environment. [11] For humans then, a perceived environment is only constructed insofar as cognitive constraints will allow. In other words, they "enact a world" by building perspectives out of ecological information, using their evolved cognitive equipment. [12]

Extended cognition

Cognitive ecology borrows ideas from views of extended cognition, as articulated by Chalmers and Clark (1998). They argue that humans cognitively utilize elements of their environment to aid the cognitive process and further entangle the mind-environment relationship as a result. They illustrate their claim with a hypothetical example of two people who achieve the same navigational success through a museum by different means; a person with Alzheimer's may use a notebook with written directions, while another may use her memory. The primary difference between the two people is that the former outsourced his memory to readily available external representations of information about the museum, whereas the latter relied on internal representations. A variant of this concept they also consider is socially extended cognition, which is a similar outsourcing of cognitive representations into other peoples' minds. [13] These ideas elaborate a cognitive interpretation of broader anthropological notions maintaining that humans are a species deeply entangled in social and material elements of culture. [14]

Distributed cognition

Distributed cognition is an important model of the extended mind thesis for cognitive ecological theory put forth by Edwin Hutchins. [15] [16] This conceptualizes human groups as active networks with cognitive properties of their own, much like neural networks themselves yield emergent cognitive properties. For a social group, cognitive properties are disseminated into an individual's surrounding network. [17] The cognitive properties of a group, Hutchins notes, is completely distinct from those of an individual. [1] Distributed cognition is fundamentally contingent on and emergent from trending ideas among a collection of brains and artefacts. [18]

This is conceptually similar to models of collective cognition in other social animal groups, which use agent based models to understanding insect swarming, fish schooling, bird flocking and baboon pack behaviors. [19] [20] [21] Collective cognition in social animal groups is adaptive because the group can amplify its overall responsiveness to ecological cues. [20] Likewise, the computational power of a human group can be more effective than that of even its best individuals. [22] This idea is echoed by anthropologists noting the collective intentionality of cultural institutions. [23]

Existing models of cultural learning dynamics seem to articulate the mechanisms by which information is acquired by and distributed within groups. In particular, cultural evolution theorists assert that individual learning is required for tracking environmental dynamics, [24] but this information is retained in culture by social learning. [25] For Hutchins, this theoretical similarity is not a coincidence. After describing distributed cognitive networks and their relationships with ecological dynamics as "cognitive ecosystems", he defines culture as a "shorthand way of referring to a complex cognitive ecosystem." [1]

Applications to cultural concepts

Religious beliefs

Religious behaviors typically exist in the form of ritual and correspond to religious god concepts. [26] These behaviors are phenotypic outcomes of god concepts that are ultimately subject to natural selection. [27] [28] Cognitive ecologists who study religion predict that god concepts across cultures can be linked to coordination solutions for local socioecological challenges, such as large-scale cooperation, intragroup cohesion and commitment, and resource management. [2] [29] For example, an omniscient and morally punitive "Big God" may be adaptive for large-scale populations by motivating prosocial behavior, [30] whereas gods associated with small-scale societies are often concerned about the stability of local resources. [31]

Economic exchange

Social contracts and their associated fairness norms are thought by many economists to be contingent on means of production. A hunter-gatherer society, for instance, may operate at an equilibrium where each person contributes to the best of his or her ability and receives according to need. But if this society were to shift toward larger-scale agricultural practices, this equilibrium would be destabilized by increases in free riding and general temptations to profit by defecting. [32] This has been supported empirically in cross-cultural studies using experimental economic game data, which showed a wide range of variance in fairness expectations between populations based on culturally-specific exchange concepts. [33] [34]

This shift in fairness expectations has also been implicated in archaeological data. In particular, the relaxed sharing norms hypothesized to be built upon periods of successful maize exploitation in the pre-Hispanic Pueblo Southwest seemed to be eroded by decreases in agricultural success. In other words, when crops began to fail and supply became low, cultural exchange norms became more stringent, kin-based and based on reciprocity. [35]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive science</span> Interdisciplinary scientific study of cognitive processes

Cognitive science is the interdisciplinary, scientific study of the mind and its processes with input from linguistics, psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, computer science/artificial intelligence, and anthropology. It examines the nature, the tasks, and the functions of cognition. Cognitive scientists study intelligence and behavior, with a focus on how nervous systems represent, process, and transform information. Mental faculties of concern to cognitive scientists include language, perception, memory, attention, reasoning, and emotion; to understand these faculties, cognitive scientists borrow from fields such as linguistics, psychology, artificial intelligence, philosophy, neuroscience, and anthropology. The typical analysis of cognitive science spans many levels of organization, from learning and decision to logic and planning; from neural circuitry to modular brain organization. One of the fundamental concepts of cognitive science is that "thinking can best be understood in terms of representational structures in the mind and computational procedures that operate on those structures."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human ecology</span> Study of the relationship between humans and their natural, social, and built environments

Human ecology is an interdisciplinary and transdisciplinary study of the relationship between humans and their natural, social, and built environments. The philosophy and study of human ecology has a diffuse history with advancements in ecology, geography, sociology, psychology, anthropology, zoology, epidemiology, public health, and home economics, among others.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Social cognition</span> Study of cognitive processes involved in social interactions

Social cognition is a topic within psychology that focuses on how people process, store, and apply information about other people and social situations. It focuses on the role that cognitive processes play in social interactions.

Situated cognition is a theory that posits that knowing is inseparable from doing by arguing that all knowledge is situated in activity bound to social, cultural and physical contexts.

Distributed cognition is an approach to cognitive science research that was developed by cognitive anthropologist Edwin Hutchins during the 1990s.

Cognitive archaeology is a theoretical perspective in archaeology that focuses on the ancient mind. It is divided into two main groups: evolutionary cognitive archaeology (ECA), which seeks to understand human cognitive evolution from the material record, and ideational cognitive archaeology (ICA), which focuses on the symbolic structures discernable in or inferable from past material culture.

Michael Tomasello is an American developmental and comparative psychologist, as well as a linguist. He is professor of psychology at Duke University.

Behavioral geography is an approach to human geography that examines human behavior by separating it into different parts. In addition, behavioral geography is an ideology/approach in human geography that makes use of the methods and assumptions of behaviorism to determine the cognitive processes involved in an individual's perception of or response and reaction to their environment. Behavioral geographers focus on the cognitive processes underlying spatial reasoning, decision making, and behavior.

The evolution of human intelligence is closely tied to the evolution of the human brain and to the origin of language. The timeline of human evolution spans approximately seven million years, from the separation of the genus Pan until the emergence of behavioral modernity by 50,000 years ago. The first three million years of this timeline concern Sahelanthropus, the following two million concern Australopithecus and the final two million span the history of the genus Homo in the Paleolithic era.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cultural psychology</span> How cultures reflect and shape their psychology

Cultural psychology is the study of how cultures reflect and shape their members' psychological processes.

Dual inheritance theory (DIT), also known as gene–culture coevolution or biocultural evolution, was developed in the 1960s through early 1980s to explain how human behavior is a product of two different and interacting evolutionary processes: genetic evolution and cultural evolution. Genes and culture continually interact in a feedback loop, changes in genes can lead to changes in culture which can then influence genetic selection, and vice versa. One of the theory's central claims is that culture evolves partly through a Darwinian selection process, which dual inheritance theorists often describe by analogy to genetic evolution.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Evolutionary developmental psychology</span> Psychology field concerned with Darwinian evolution

Evolutionary developmental psychology (EDP) is a research paradigm that applies the basic principles of evolution by natural selection, to understand the development of human behavior and cognition. It involves the study of both the genetic and environmental mechanisms that underlie the development of social and cognitive competencies, as well as the epigenetic processes that adapt these competencies to local conditions.

Neuroanthropology is the study of the relationship between culture and the brain. This field of study emerged from a 2008 conference of the American Anthropological Association. It is based on the premise that lived experience leaves identifiable patterns in brain structure, which then feed back into cultural expression.The exact mechanisms are so far ill defined and remain speculative.

Cognitive science of religion is the study of religious thought, theory, and behavior from the perspective of the cognitive and evolutionary sciences. Scholars in this field seek to explain how human minds acquire, generate, and transmit religious thoughts, practices, and schemas by means of ordinary cognitive capacities.

Cultural neuroscience is a field of research that focuses on the interrelation between a human's cultural environment and neurobiological systems. The field particularly incorporates ideas and perspectives from related domains like anthropology, psychology, and cognitive neuroscience to study sociocultural influences on human behaviors. Such impacts on behavior are often measured using various neuroimaging methods, through which cross-cultural variability in neural activity can be examined.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Embodied cognition</span> Interdisciplinary theory

Embodied cognition is the theory that many features of cognition, whether human or otherwise, are shaped by aspects of an organism's entire body. The cognitive features include high-level mental constructs and performance on various cognitive tasks. The bodily aspects involve the motor system, the perceptual system, the bodily interactions with the environment (situatedness), and the assumptions about the world built the functional structure of organism's brain and body.

Cognitive ecology of religion is an integrative approach to studying how religious beliefs covary with social and natural dynamics of the environment. This is done by incorporating a cognitive ecological perspective to cross-cultural god concepts. Religious beliefs are thought to be a byproduct of domain-specific cognitive modules that give rise to religious cognition. The cognitive biases leading to religious belief are constraints on perceptions of the environment, which is part and parcel of a cognitive ecological approach. This means that they not only shape religious beliefs, but they are determinants of how successfully cultural beliefs are transmitted.

Evolutionary psychology has traditionally focused on individual-level behaviors, determined by species-typical psychological adaptations. Considerable work, though, has been done on how these adaptations shape and, ultimately govern, culture. Tooby and Cosmides (1989) argued that the mind consists of many domain-specific psychological adaptations, some of which may constrain what cultural material is learned or taught. As opposed to a domain-general cultural acquisition program, where an individual passively receives culturally-transmitted material from the group, Tooby and Cosmides (1989), among others, argue that: "the psyche evolved to generate adaptive rather than repetitive behavior, and hence critically analyzes the behavior of those surrounding it in highly structured and patterned ways, to be used as a rich source of information out of which to construct a 'private culture' or individually tailored adaptive system; in consequence, this system may or may not mirror the behavior of others in any given respect.".

Neuroarchaeology is a sub-discipline of archaeology that uses neuroscientific data to infer things about brain form and function in human cognitive evolution. The term was first suggested and thus coined by Colin Renfrew and Lambros Malafouris.

Lambros Malafouris is a Greek-British cognitive archaeologist who has pioneered the application of concepts from the philosophy of mind to the material record. He is Professor of Cognitive and Anthropological Archaeology at the University of Oxford. He is known for Material Engagement Theory, the idea that material objects in the archaeological record are part of the ancient human mind.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Hutchins, Edwin (2010-10-01). "Cognitive Ecology". Topics in Cognitive Science. 2 (4): 705–715. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01089.x. ISSN   1756-8765. PMID   25164051.
  2. 1 2 Botero, Carlos A.; Gardner, Beth; Kirby, Kathryn R.; Bulbulia, Joseph; Gavin, Michael C.; Gray, Russell D. (25 November 2014). "The ecology of religious beliefs". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (47): 16784–16789. Bibcode:2014PNAS..11116784B. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408701111 . PMC   4250141 . PMID   25385605.
  3. Miller, George A (March 2003). "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. PMID   12639696. S2CID   206129621.
  4. Neisser, Ulric (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall. ISBN   978-0131396678.
  5. 1 2 Heft, Harry (2013). "An ecological approach to psychology". Review of General Psychology. 17 (2): 162–167. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.400.9646 . doi:10.1037/a0032928. S2CID   147335659.
  6. Chemero, Anthony (2003). "An outline of a theory of affordances". Ecological Psychology. 15 (2): 181–195. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.475.4827 . doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1502_5. S2CID   143720358.
  7. Gibson, James J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception ([Nachdr.]. ed.). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN   978-0898599596.
  8. Gibson, James (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Psychology Press. ISBN   978-1848725782.
  9. Bateson, Gregory (2000). Steps to an ecology of mind (University of Chicago Press ed.). Chicago [u.a.]: University of Chicago Press. ISBN   978-0-226-03905-3.
  10. Palacios, AG; Bozinovic, F (2003). "An "enactive" approach to integrative and comparative biology: thoughts on the table". Biological Research. 36 (1): 101–5. doi: 10.4067/s0716-97602003000100008 . PMID   12795209.
  11. Noë, Alva (2004). Action in perception (Paperback ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN   978-0262140881.
  12. Stewart, John; Gapenne, Olivier; Di Paolo, Ezequiel A. (2014). Enaction: toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge, Massachusetts. ISBN   978-0-262-52601-2. OCLC   880401178.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  13. Clark, A.; Chalmers, D. (1 January 1998). "The Extended Mind". Analysis. 58 (1): 7–19. doi:10.1093/analys/58.1.7.
  14. Hodder, Ian (2012). Entangled. ; An Archaeology of the Relationships Between Humans and Things (first publ. ed.). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons, Incorporated. ISBN   978-0-470-67212-9.
  15. Hutchins, Edwin (2000). "Distributed cognition". International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences.
  16. Hutchins, Edwin (1996). Cognition in the wild (8. pr. ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press (A Bradford Book). ISBN   9780262581462.
  17. Heylighen, Heath; Van Overwalle (2003). "The Emergence of Distributed Cognition: a conceptual framework". Proceedings of Collective Intentionality IV.
  18. Rogers, Ellis (1994). "Distributed Cognition: an alternative framework for analysing and explaining collaborative working". Journal of Information Technology. 9 (2): 119–128. doi:10.1177/026839629400900203. S2CID   219981758.
  19. Strandburg-Peshkin, A.; Farine, D. R.; Couzin, I. D.; Crofoot, M. C. (18 June 2015). "Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons". Science. 348 (6241): 1358–1361. Bibcode:2015Sci...348.1358S. doi:10.1126/science.aaa5099. PMC   4801504 . PMID   26089514.
  20. 1 2 Couzin, Iain D. (January 2009). "Collective cognition in animal groups". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13 (1): 36–43. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.278.5663 . doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.002. PMID   19058992. S2CID   15175317.
  21. Couzin, Iain (15 February 2007). "Collective minds". Nature. 445 (7129): 715. Bibcode:2007Natur.445..715C. doi: 10.1038/445715a . PMID   17301775.
  22. Clément, Romain J. G.; Krause, Stefan; von Engelhardt, Nikolaus; Faria, Jolyon J.; Krause, Jens; Kurvers, Ralf H. J. M.; de Polavieja, GonzaloÂ. G. (17 October 2013). "Collective Cognition in Humans: Groups Outperform Their Best Members in a Sentence Reconstruction Task". PLOS ONE. 8 (10): e77943. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...877943C. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077943 . PMC   3798465 . PMID   24147101.
  23. Tomasello, Michael; Melis, Alicia P.; Tennie, Claudio; Wyman, Emily; Herrmann, Esther (December 2012). "Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation" (PDF). Current Anthropology. 53 (6): 673–692. doi:10.1086/668207. S2CID   210221053.
  24. Henrich, Joseph; McElreath, Richard (19 May 2003). "The evolution of cultural evolution". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110. S2CID   14302229.
  25. Boyd, R.; Richerson, P. J.; Henrich, J. (20 June 2011). "The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (Supplement_2): 10918–10925. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10810918B. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108 . PMC   3131818 . PMID   21690340.
  26. Purzycki & Sosis (2011). "Our Gods: Variation in Supernatural Minds". In Ulrich, Frey (ed.). Essential Building Blocks of Human Nature. Bibcode:2011ebbh.book.....F.
  27. Codding, Brian F.; Bird, Douglas W. (April 2015). "Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological science". Journal of Archaeological Science. 56: 9–20. Bibcode:2015JArSc..56....9C. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2015.02.027.
  28. Cronk, Lee (1991). "Human Behavioral Ecology". Annual Review of Anthropology. 20 (1): 25–53. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.20.1.25.
  29. Purzycki; McNamara (2016). "An Ecological Theory of Gods' Minds". Advances in Religion, Cognitive Science and Experimental Philosophy.
  30. Norenzayan, Ara; Shariff, Azim F.; Gervais, Will M.; Willard, Aiyana K.; McNamara, Rita A.; Slingerland, Edward; Henrich, Joseph (2 December 2014). "The cultural evolution of prosocial religions". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 39: e1. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001356 . PMID   26785995.
  31. Purzycki, Benjamin Grant (13 April 2016). "The Evolution of Gods' Minds in the Tyva Republic". Current Anthropology. 57: S88–S104. doi:10.1086/685729. S2CID   33452734.
  32. Binmore, Ken (22 December 2005). "Economic man – or straw man?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (6): 817–818. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05230145. S2CID   53375617.
  33. Henrich, Boyd; Bowles; Camerer; Fehr; Gintis; McElreath; Aivard; Barr; Ensminger; Henrich; Hill; Gil-White; Gurven; Marlowe; Patton; Tracer (2005). ""Economic man" in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (6): 795–815, discussion 815–55. doi:10.1017/s0140525x05000142. PMID   16372952. S2CID   3194574.
  34. Gachter, S.; Herrmann, B.; Thoni, C. (2 August 2010). "Culture and cooperation". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 365 (1553): 2651–2661. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0135. PMC   2936171 . PMID   20679109.
  35. Bocinsky, R. K.; Rush, J.; Kintigh, K. W.; Kohler, T. A. (1 April 2016). "Exploration and exploitation in the macrohistory of the pre-Hispanic Pueblo Southwest". Science Advances. 2 (4): e1501532. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E1532B. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1501532. PMC   4820384 . PMID   27051879.