Generation effect

Last updated

The generation effect is a phenomenon whereby information is better remembered if it is generated from one's own mind rather than simply read. [1] Researchers have struggled to account for why the generated information is better recalled than read information, but no single explanation has been sufficient to explain everything.

Contents

In experiments

The generation effect is typically achieved in cognitive psychology experiments by asking participants to generate words from word fragments. [2] This effect has also been demonstrated using a variety of other materials, such as when generating a word after being presented with its antonym, [3] synonym, [1] picture, [4] arithmetic problems, [2] [5] or keyword in a paragraph. [6] The generation effect has been found in studies using free recall, cued recall, and recognition tests. [3] In one study, the subject was provided with a stimulus word, the first letter of the response, and a word relating the two. For example, with the rule of the opposite, the stimulus word "hot", and the letter "c", the word cold would be generated. This methodology has since been used in the majority of experiments investigating the generation effect. [7]

Causes

Lexical activation hypothesis

According to the lexical activation hypothesis, the participant must search his/her semantic memory during the process of generation. The search activates semantic features in memory that are related to the target item. During the retrieval of the target item at testing, the semantic features serve as retrieval cues and aid in the recall of the target item. [2] One study done by Payne, Neely, and Burns further tested this hypothesis. Their research question was: will the generation effect occur for words only or also for non-words? To test this they studied 168 Purdue undergraduate students. The researchers divided the participants up into two groups. The first group had a word or non-word pairs that rhymed and were told to read both out loud. The second group had a word or non-word and the first letter of the next followed by blanks, and were told to read the first word out loud and generate a word that rhymed with the first word and started with the letter presented. The results were as expected. Participants were only generating words rather than non-words according to the lexical activation hypothesis. [8]

Procedural account

The procedural account, which builds upon the lexical activation hypothesis, argues that people are more likely to engage in particular cognitive procedures during the encoding of items when generating than when reading. The process of generation induces people to connect the item to information in memory (unlike the lexical activation hypothesis, the information in memory does not necessarily reside in the lexicon). The generation effect occurs if the procedures used during encoding are reinstated during the memory test. [5] The procedural account is also linked to transfer-appropriate processing in that they both cause or do not cause the generation effect depending on encoding and retrieval processes. [9]

Multifactor transfer-appropriate processing account

According to the multifactor transfer-appropriate processing account, the generation task forces participants to focus their processing on the type of information needed to solve the generation task. When a later test is sensitive to the same type of information, a generation effect occurs. However, when there is not a match between the type of information processed to solve the generation task and the type of information needed to do well on a later test, the generation effect does not occur. [10] For example, a participant that is required to generate same-category targets from distinctive semantic cues (e.g., PURR-C_T, SADDLE-H_RS_) is likely to notice similarities between the targets (e.g., they are all animals). This type of manipulation would promote whole-list relational processing, which may enhance generation performance on a free recall test. Other manipulations can emphasize cue-target processing, thus helping generation performance on cued recall tests. [11]

It has been found that the generation effect was invariable across the different types of memory encoding. [5]

Limitations

By manipulating materials or instructions, experimenters have reduced or eliminated the generation effect. This suggests that there are instances in which reading can have the same memorial gains as generating. For example, when participants are given instructions to process information in a manner that was similar to the processing performed by the participants in the generate condition, the generation advantage between the groups was eliminated. [11] In another study, participants who used a processing strategy (imagery) that was more effective than reading performed just as well as those who generated. [12]

Although the generation effect is a robust finding, there are some studies that have found no memorial benefits of generating compared to reading. For example, one study did not find a generation effect when they used legal non-words and found a reduced generation effect when they used material unfamiliar to the participants. They concluded that generating may have limited effectiveness when applied to new or unfamiliar material. This warrants some concern because if the generation effect is to be incorporated into educational practices such as classroom teaching, we would want it to help students learn new material. [13]

It is possible that the generation effect may cause a trade-off in encoding item information and associative information. [14] The processing of item-specific features of the target item may be enhanced when generating, and generating may also enhance the processing of cue-target relation. But, encoding requires limited-capacity resources, so the better encoding of one type of information may occur at a cost to the encoding of other information. [14] [15] This also has implications for applying generation to educational practices because even if generation improves the recall of specific words, the memory for the contextual information surrounding those words may suffer.

Practical applications

The generation effect might prove a useful strategy for learning, especially for remembering educational material. Researchers at UCLA and UC Berkeley are investigating ways to incorporate learning strategies including the generation effect, as well as other "desirable difficulties", into the classroom. [16]

Using flashcards is one of the ways that generation effect could be utilized to aid in memory retention.

Exceptions

There are several reports in the literature that suggest that individuals diagnosed with DAT (dementia of the Alzheimer type) do not show the generation effect. One study included 42 healthy adults, 23 with very mild DAT, and 26 with mild DAT. Researchers Kristi S. Multhaup and David A. Balota had participants read some sentences (e.g., "The horse jumped the fence.") and generate the endings to other sentences (e.g., "The gentleman opened the"). Later, participants were given the subjects of the sentences (e.g., "horse"; "gentleman") and asked to recall the object that had been paired with the words in the previous sentences (e.g., "fence"; "door"). Healthy older adults showed higher recall for words that they had generated compared with 383 words that they had read (i.e., a significant generation effect). In contrast, individuals diagnosed with DAT did not show a difference in recall for words that they had generated and words that they had read. Similarly, another study reported four experiments with the same group of 18 DAT individuals and found little evidence for a generation effect in word recall, word recognition, or recall of action instructions. [17]

Nonhuman animals

There is some evidence that animals also display the generation effect during learning. [18] However, more recent work challenges the idea animals display the generation effect. [19]

See also

Related Research Articles

Recall in memory refers to the mental process of retrieval of information from the past. Along with encoding and storage, it is one of the three core processes of memory. There are three main types of recall: free recall, cued recall and serial recall. Psychologists test these forms of recall as a way to study the memory processes of humans and animals. Two main theories of the process of recall are the two-stage theory and the theory of encoding specificity.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Picture superiority effect</span> Psychological phenomenon

The picture superiority effect refers to the phenomenon in which pictures and images are more likely to be remembered than are words. This effect has been demonstrated in numerous experiments using different methods. It is based on the notion that "human memory is extremely sensitive to the symbolic modality of presentation of event information". Explanations for the picture superiority effect are not concrete and are still being debated.

The spacing effect demonstrates that learning is more effective when study sessions are spaced out. This effect shows that more information is encoded into long-term memory by spaced study sessions, also known as spaced repetition or spaced presentation, than by massed presentation ("cramming").

The Levels of Processing model, created by Fergus I. M. Craik and Robert S. Lockhart in 1972, describes memory recall of stimuli as a function of the depth of mental processing. Deeper levels of analysis produce more elaborate, longer-lasting, and stronger memory traces than shallow levels of analysis. Depth of processing falls on a shallow to deep continuum. Shallow processing leads to a fragile memory trace that is susceptible to rapid decay. Conversely, deep processing results in a more durable memory trace. There are three levels of processing in this model. Structural processing, or visual, is when we remember only the physical quality of the word E.g how the word is spelled and how letters look. Phonemic processing includes remembering the word by the way it sounds. E.G the word tall rhymes with fall. Lastly, we have semantic processing in which we encode the meaning of the word with another word that is similar or has similar meaning. Once the word is perceived, the brain allows for a deeper processing.

Tip of the tongue is the phenomenon of failing to retrieve a word or term from memory, combined with partial recall and the feeling that retrieval is imminent. The phenomenon's name comes from the saying, "It's on the tip of my tongue." The tip of the tongue phenomenon reveals that lexical access occurs in stages.

Memory has the ability to encode, store and recall information. Memories give an organism the capability to learn and adapt from previous experiences as well as build relationships. Encoding allows a perceived item of use or interest to be converted into a construct that can be stored within the brain and recalled later from long-term memory. Working memory stores information for immediate use or manipulation, which is aided through hooking onto previously archived items already present in the long-term memory of an individual.

Artificial grammar learning (AGL) is a paradigm of study within cognitive psychology and linguistics. Its goal is to investigate the processes that underlie human language learning by testing subjects' ability to learn a made-up grammar in a laboratory setting. It was developed to evaluate the processes of human language learning but has also been utilized to study implicit learning in a more general sense. The area of interest is typically the subjects' ability to detect patterns and statistical regularities during a training phase and then use their new knowledge of those patterns in a testing phase. The testing phase can either use the symbols or sounds used in the training phase or transfer the patterns to another set of symbols or sounds as surface structure.

In mental memory, storage is one of three fundamental stages along with encoding and retrieval. Memory is the process of storing and recalling information that was previously acquired. Storing refers to the process of placing newly acquired information into memory, which is modified in the brain for easier storage. Encoding this information makes the process of retrieval easier for the brain where it can be recalled and brought into conscious thinking. Modern memory psychology differentiates between the two distinct types of memory storage: short-term memory and long-term memory. Several models of memory have been proposed over the past century, some of them suggesting different relationships between short- and long-term memory to account for different ways of storing memory.

Redintegration refers to the restoration of the whole of something from a part of it. The everyday phenomenon is that a small part of a memory can remind a person of the entire memory, for example, “recalling an entire song when a few notes are played.” In cognitive psychology the word is used in reference to phenomena in the field of memory, where it is defined as "the use of long-term knowledge to facilitate recall." The process is hypothesised to be working as "pattern completion", where previous knowledge is used to facilitate the completion of the partially degraded memory trace.

Metamemory or Socratic awareness, a type of metacognition, is both the introspective knowledge of one's own memory capabilities and the processes involved in memory self-monitoring. This self-awareness of memory has important implications for how people learn and use memories. When studying, for example, students make judgments of whether they have successfully learned the assigned material and use these decisions, known as "judgments of learning", to allocate study time.

In psychology, context-dependent memory is the improved recall of specific episodes or information when the context present at encoding and retrieval are the same. In a simpler manner, "when events are represented in memory, contextual information is stored along with memory targets; the context can therefore cue memories containing that contextual information". One particularly common example of context-dependence at work occurs when an individual has lost an item in an unknown location. Typically, people try to systematically "retrace their steps" to determine all of the possible places where the item might be located. Based on the role that context plays in determining recall, it is not at all surprising that individuals often quite easily discover the lost item upon returning to the correct context. This concept is heavily related to the encoding specificity principle.

Priming is the idea that exposure to one stimulus may influence a response to a subsequent stimulus, without conscious guidance or intention. The priming effect refers to the positive or negative effect of a rapidly presented stimulus on the processing of a second stimulus that appears shortly after. Generally speaking, the generation of priming effect depends on the existence of some positive or negative relationship between priming and target stimuli. For example, the word nurse might be recognized more quickly following the word doctor than following the word bread. Priming can be perceptual, associative, repetitive, positive, negative, affective, semantic, or conceptual. Priming effects involve word recognition, semantic processing, attention, unconscious processing, and many other issues, and are related to differences in various writing systems. Research, however, has yet to firmly establish the duration of priming effects, yet their onset can be almost instantaneous.

Emotion can have a powerful effect on humans and animals. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.

In psychology, the misattribution of memory or source misattribution is the misidentification of the origin of a memory by the person making the memory recall. Misattribution is likely to occur when individuals are unable to monitor and control the influence of their attitudes, toward their judgments, at the time of retrieval. Misattribution is divided into three components: cryptomnesia, false memories, and source confusion. It was originally noted as one of Daniel Schacter's seven sins of memory.

The encoding specificity principle is the general principle that matching the encoding contexts of information at recall assists in the retrieval of episodic memories. It provides a framework for understanding how the conditions present while encoding information relate to memory and recall of that information.

The mental lexicon is defined as a mental dictionary that contains information regarding the word store of a language user, such as their meanings, pronunciations, and syntactic characteristics. The mental lexicon is used in linguistics and psycholinguistics to refer to individual speakers' lexical, or word, representations. However, there is some disagreement as to the utility of the mental lexicon as a scientific construct.

Retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF) is a memory phenomenon where remembering causes forgetting of other information in memory. The phenomenon was first demonstrated in 1994, although the concept of RIF has been previously discussed in the context of retrieval inhibition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bilingual memory</span>

Bilingualism is the regular use of two fluent languages, and bilinguals are those individuals who need and use two languages in their everyday lives. A person's bilingual memories are heavily dependent on the person's fluency, the age the second language was acquired, and high language proficiency to both languages. High proficiency provides mental flexibility across all domains of thought and forces them to adopt strategies that accelerate cognitive development. People who are bilingual integrate and organize the information of two languages, which creates advantages in terms of many cognitive abilities, such as intelligence, creativity, analogical reasoning, classification skills, problem solving, learning strategies, and thinking flexibility.

Age of acquisition, is a psycholinguistic variable referring to the age at which a word is typically learned. For example, the word 'penguin' is typically learned at a younger age than the word 'albatross'. Studies in psycholinguistics suggest that age of acquisition has an effect on the speed of reading words. The findings have demonstrated that early-acquired words are processed more quickly than later-acquired words. It is a particularly strong variable in predicting the speed of picture naming. It has been generally found that words that are more frequent, shorter, more familiar and refer to concrete concepts are learned earlier than the counterparts in simple words and compound words, together with their respective lexemes. In addition, the AoA effect has been demonstrated in several languages and in bilingual speakers.

The forward testing effect, also known as test potentiated new learning, is a psychological learning theory which suggests that testing old information can improve learning of new information. Unlike traditional learning theories in educational psychology which have established the positive effect testing has when later attempting to retrieve the same information, the forward testing effect instead suggests that the testing experience itself possesses unique benefits which enhance the learning of new information. This memory effect is also distinct from the 'practice effect' which typically refers to an observed improvement which results from repetition and restudy, as the testing itself is considered as the catalyst for improved recall. Instead, this theory suggests that testing serves not only as a tool for assessment but as a learning tool which can aid in memory recall. The forward testing effect indicates that educators should encourage students to study using testing techniques rather than restudying information repeatedly.

References

  1. 1 2 Jacoby, L.L. (1978). "On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution" (PDF). Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 17 (6): 649–668. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90393-6. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-01-13. Retrieved 2011-04-24.
  2. 1 2 3 Crutcher, R.J.; Healy, A.F. (1989). "Cognitive operations and the generation effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 15 (4): 669–675. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.4.669.
  3. 1 2 Mulligan, N.W. (2001). "Generation and hypermnesia". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (2): 436–450. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.436. PMID   11294442.
  4. Kinjo, H.; Snodgrass, J.G. (2000). "Does the generation effect occur for pictures?". The American Journal of Psychology. 113 (1): 95–121. doi:10.2307/1423462. JSTOR   1423462. PMID   10742845.
  5. 1 2 3 McNamara, D.S.; Healy, A.F. (2000). "A procedural explanation of the generation effect for simple and difficult multiplication problems and answers". Journal of Memory and Language. 43 (4): 652–679. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2720.
  6. deWinstanley, P.A.; Bjork, E.L. (2004). "Processing strategies and the generation effect: Implications for making a better reader". Memory & Cognition. 32 (6): 945–955. doi: 10.3758/BF03196872 . PMID   15673182.
  7. McNamara, Danielle (1992). The Generation Effect: A Detailed Analysis of the Role of Semantic Processing (PDF) (Technical report). Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.20.3266 . 92-02. Archived from the original on 2007-08-18.{{cite tech report}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  8. Payne, David; James Neely; Daniel Burns (1986). "The Generation Effect: Further Tests of the Lexical Activation Hypothesis". Memory & Cognition. 14 (3): 246–252. doi: 10.3758/BF03197700 . PMID   3736398.
  9. DeWinstanley, Patricia; Elizabeth Bjork (2004). "Processing Strategies and the Generation Effect: Implications for Making a Better Reader". Memory & Cognition. 32 (6): 945–955. doi: 10.3758/BF03196872 . PMID   15673182.
  10. deWinstanley, P.A.; Bjork, E.L.; Bjork, R.A. (1996). "Generation effects and the lack thereof: The role of transfer-appropriate processing". Memory. 4 (1): 31–48. doi:10.1080/741940667. PMID   8821084.
  11. 1 2 McDaniel, Mark A; Waddill, Paula J; Einstein, Gilles O (1988). "A contextual account of the generation effect: A three-factor theory". Journal of Memory and Language. 27 (5): 521–536. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(88)90023-X.
  12. Begg, I.; Vinski, E.; Frankovich, L.; Holgate, B. (1991). "Generating makes words memorable, but so does effective reading". Memory & Cognition. 19 (5): 487–497. doi: 10.3758/BF03199571 . PMID   1956309.
  13. Lutz, John; Briggs, A.; Cain, K. (2003). "An examination of the value of the generation effect for learning new material". The Journal of General Psychology. 130 (2): 171–188. doi:10.1080/00221300309601283. PMID   12773019. S2CID   44825557.
  14. 1 2 Mulligan, Neil W. (2004). "Generation and memory for contextual detail". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 30 (4): 838–855. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.838. PMID   15238028.
  15. Jurica, P.J.; Shimamura, A.P. (1999). "Monitoring item and source information: Evidence for a negative generation effect in source memory". Memory & Cognition. 27 (4): 648–656. doi: 10.3758/BF03211558 . PMID   10479823.
  16. "IDDEAS".
  17. Multhaup, Kristi S.; Balota, David A. (1997). "Generation effects and source memory in healthy older adults and in adults with dementia of the Alzheimer type" (PDF). Neuropsychology. 11 (3): 382–391. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.11.3.382. PMID   9223142. Archived from the original on 2010-06-14.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  18. Kornell, N.; Terrace, H.S. (2007). "The Generation Effect in monkeys". Psychological Science. 18 (8): 682–685. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.544.7370 . doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01959.x. PMID   17680937. S2CID   12034243.
  19. Staniland, J.; Colombo, M.; Scarf, D. (2015). "The Generation Effect or Simply Generating an Effect?". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 129 (4): 329–333. doi:10.1037/a0039450. PMID   26147702.