Maternal sensitivity

Last updated
A mother and her child in 1912. Mothers who better understand their infant's signals are said to have higher maternal sensitivity. Mother and Child, 1912.jpg
A mother and her child in 1912. Mothers who better understand their infant's signals are said to have higher maternal sensitivity.

Maternal sensitivity is a mother's ability to perceive and infer the meaning behind her infant's behavioural signals, and to respond to them promptly and appropriately. Maternal sensitivity affects child development at all stages through life, from infancy, all the way to adulthood. In general, more sensitive mothers have healthier, more socially and cognitively developed children than those who are not as sensitive. [1] Also, maternal sensitivity has been found to affect the person psychologically even as an adult. [2] Adults who experienced high maternal sensitivity during their childhood were found to be more secure than those who experienced less sensitive mothers. [2] Once the adult becomes a parent themselves, their own understanding of maternal sensitivity will affect their own children's development. [2] Some research suggests that adult mothers display more maternal sensitivity than adolescent mothers who may in turn have children with a lower IQ and reading level than children of adult mothers. [3]

Contents

There are different ways of assessing maternal sensitivity, such as through the use of naturalistic observation, [4] the Strange Situation, [5] maternal-synchrony, [6] and maternal mind-mindedness. [7] There are also a number of ways of measuring maternal sensitivity in the scientific world, which include Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity Scale (AMSS), [8] the Maternal Behaviour Q-sort (MBQS), and the Pederson and Moran Sensitivity Q-Sort. [9]

Description

Maternal sensitivity was first defined by Mary Ainsworth as "a mother's ability to perceive and interpret accurately her infant's signals and communications and then respond appropriately". It was later revised by Karl and Broom in 1995 as "a mother's ability to recognize infant cues consistently and act on those cues, and the ability to monitor and accurately interpret infant cues, as evidenced by mother–child interactions that are contingent, reciprocal and affectively positive". It can be generally defined as a broad concept combining a variety of behavioral care giving attributes. [10]

The research on maternal sensitivity follows earlier work in psychoanalytics and is especially rooted in attachment theory. As the focus of psychoanalytics shifted from individuals (particularly adults) to children, research studies on mother–infant dyads, on the effects of early childhood on development, and on pregnancy became wider. A psychologist named John Bowlby eventually developed the attachment theory in 1969. Mary Ainsworth, who worked with Bowlby, along with her colleagues created the concept of maternal sensitivity in 1978 in order to describe early mother–infant interaction observed in her empirical studies. [11]

There are four important aspects of maternal sensitivity: dynamic process involving maternal abilities, reciprocal give-and-take with the infant, contingency on the infant's behavior, and quality of maternal behaviors. [10]

Maternal sensitivity is dynamic, elastic and can change over time. A sensitive mother needs to be able to perceive the cues and signals her baby gives her, interpret them correctly and act appropriately. The three most positive affecting factors for the baby are a mother's social support, maternal–fetal attachment and high self-esteem. The three most negative affecting factors are maternal depression, maternal stress, and maternal anxiety. [10] Recent studies have shown that maternal posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) can negatively impact a mother's sensitivity during stressful moments with her child that serve as traumatic reminders and that this quite likely has a neural basis in the maternal brain. [12] [13]

Assessment

Naturalistic observation

Maternal sensitivity is most commonly assessed during naturalistic observation of free play interactions between mother and child. [4] There are several factors surrounding assessment during observation that may cause differences in results, including the setting (home vs laboratory), the context (free play vs structured task), the length of observation and the frequency of observation. While some observational studies focus strictly on the relationship between mother and child during close interaction such as feeding or free play, other studies look into how well the maternal figure divides her attention between the baby and other everyday activities. [14] The latter was demonstrated in an experiment conducted by Atkinson et al. where mothers were given a questionnaire to act as a "distractor task", and were assessed on their ability to effectively divide their attention between the "distractor task" and their child. [15] In regards to length of observation, some studies require no more than a one-time 10-minute assessment, while other studies used a much lengthier time. [14]

Strange Situation

The Strange Situation was developed by Mary Ainsworth in the 1970s to assess attachment relationships between caregivers and children between 9 and 18 months old. Because maternal sensitivity is an indicator of attachment relationship, researchers sometimes use the Strange Situation to observe attachment so that they may use the results to predict and infer the level of maternal sensitivity. [5]

In the Strange Situation, the toddler's behavior and stress is observed during a 21-minute free-play session through a one-way glass window as the caregiver and strangers come into and leave the room. [5] The specific sequence of events is as follows:

  1. The mother and child are alone. Observation of the use of the mother as a secure base, who fosters exploratory behavior and independence.
  2. A stranger enters the room, which shows the effects of stranger anxiety.
  3. The mother leaves the child, which shows the effects of separation anxiety. The strange tries to comfort the child, which tests the effects of stranger anxiety.
  4. The mother returns and the stranger leaves. Observation of reunion behavior.
  5. The parent leaves and the child once more; separation anxiety is tested for.
  6. The stranger returns and tries to comfort the infant; stranger anxiety is tested for.
  7. The parent returns and the stranger leaves. Once more, observation of reunion behavior. [5]

The children are observed and categorized into one of the four attachment patterns secure attachment, anxious-ambivalent attachment, anxious-avoidant attachment, or disorganized attachment  – based on the infant's separation anxiety, willingness to explore, stranger anxiety, and reunion behavior. [5]

Mother–infant synchrony and maternal mind-mindedness

Two related qualitative concepts that are correlated with maternal sensitivity are mother–infant synchrony and maternal mind-mindedness. [6] [7]

In mother–infant synchrony, the mother and infant's ability to change their own behaviour based on the other's response is taken into consideration. Infant affect (vocal and facial) and maternal stimulation (vocal and tactile) are good indicators of mother–infant synchrony. Zentall et al. found that infants' rhythm was stronger and interactions were led better at 5 months than at 3 months. According to the study, an infant's ability to send signals and a mother's ability to perceive them increase with synchrony over time. [6] Studies have shown that mother–infant synchrony will result in the infant's development of self-control and other self-regulating behaviours later on in life. [16]

The related concept of maternal mind-mindedness assesses the mother's ability to understand and verbalize the infant's mind: thoughts, desires, intentions and memories. Maternal mind-mindedness has been found to be related to some developmental results, such as attachment security. A caregiver's comment is deemed an appropriate mind-related comment if the comment was deemed to match the infant's behaviour by the independent coder, if the comment associated the infant's current activity to past activities, and/or if the comment encouraged the infant to go on with his or her intentions when the conversation paused. This correlates to high maternal mind-mindedness. If the caregiver assigns the wrong internal state to the baby's behaviour, if the comment about the current activity is not insufficiently associated with a past event, if the comment deters the infant from proceeding with the current activity, and/or if the comment is unclear, it is deemed a in-appropriate mind-related comment and correlates to low mind-mindedness. [7]

Role of maternal sensitivity in development

Infancy

Infants whose mothers are more sensitive are more likely to display secure attachment relationships. Because the maternal figure is generally accessible and responsive to the infant's needs, the infant is able to form expectations of the mother's behaviour. Once expectations are met and the infant feels a consistency in the mother's sensitivity, the infant is able to find security in the maternal figure. Those infants whose mothers do not respond to the signals from their children or respond inappropriately to their children's cries for attention will form insecure and anxious attachments because the infants are unable to consistently depend on the maternal figures for predictable and safe responses. [17]

In order for the infant to feel that the maternal figure is accessible and responsive, a certain amount of interaction must occur. Though the most research has been done on face-to-face interaction, studies have found that bodily interaction is also important in sensitivity and development. It is not how often the baby is held that reflects attachment, but how the baby is held and whether or not the baby desires to be held that matters in attachment development. Another factor that is important is sensitivity to the infant's feeding signals. [17] There lies some controversy in whether infants who form insecure attachment relationships with their mothers do so because the mother is particularly insensitive to her child's needs or because of differences in their personality (i.e. their temperament) and due to life situations. [18]

Childhood

Children whose mothers have higher levels of maternal sensitivity are generally happier, healthier, smarter, and better behaved. Doing the best she can.jpg
Children whose mothers have higher levels of maternal sensitivity are generally happier, healthier, smarter, and better behaved.

There is a crucial interplay between parenting and child characteristics such as health, temperament, development and cognition. The children with the most sensitive, consistent mothers are the ones who are generally most healthy, happy and well adapted. [1] [19]

Health in childhood

Maternal sensitivity even in the first few months of mother–child relationships are an important factor to health in childhood, especially with obesity. A study using data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development's Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development assessed mother–child interactions and categorized them in one of two groups: sensitive or insensitive. Their child's growth (height and weight) was monitored throughout their childhood, from 24 months all the way to grade six, and body mass index was calculated. As the children grew, the percentage of overweight or obese grew too. From 24 months the overall overweight-obese percentage was 15.58% and by grade six, 34.34% of the children were classified as overweight or obese. More interesting is the difference between the maternal sensitive group and the maternal insensitive group. The children with the sensitive mothers started out with an overweight-obese percentage of 14.96% (24 months) and ended the research with 29.54% (grade six). The children classified with insensitive mothers had an overweight-obese percentage of 16.16% at 24 months and 39.28% at grade six. This shows a significant correlation between the mother's sensitivity and the child's risk for overweight-obesity during their elementary years. This is very important for obesity prevention programs for children. [1]

Temperament in childhood

Current studies have shown a correlation between maternal sensitivity or insensitivity, negative discipline and childhood aggression. An experiment sampling 117 mother–child pairs showed a unique relationship between the mother's sensitivity and the use of discipline and the child's temperament level. Observations (of the mother's sensitivity to the child's needs, the child's aggression and temperament level and the relationship between the two) were made when the children on average were 26.71 months old (range of 13.58 to 41.91 months). The data were collected again a year later. Results show a year later that negative discipline is correlated with child aggression, but only when that mother is insensitive. [19]

Development in childhood

A study by Jay Belsky and R.M. Pasco Fearon tested the correlation between childhood development and the sensitivity of the mother. [20] The hypotheses were:

  • secure attachment (observed at 15 months) and maternal sensitivity (observed at 24 months) produced the highest competencies in three-year-old children,
  • the least competent children would have a history of insecure attachment and maternal insensitivity,
  • and the children reared with mixed or inconsistency would fall in between.

The children were tested in five developmental categories: problem behavior, social competence, expressive language, receptive language and school readiness. Results highly support the hypothesis (i.e. maternal sensitivity and childhood development are positively correlated.) This is an important issue as it shows how influential the early experience of a child affects their future development. [20]

Cognition in childhood

Mothers who were found to display higher sensitivity towards their children from preschool to first grade were found to have higher achieving children than those who displayed lower maternal sensitivity. The children of maternally sensitive mothers scored higher in math and phoneme knowledge than those who had a history of lower maternal sensitivity. [21]

Maternal sensitivity has been shown to teach infants attentional skills, which are necessary later in life for emotional control, and other more complex cognitive processes. [22]

In families with more than one child (twins or triplets), it has been found that maternal sensitivity is lower, as there are more needs to be taken care of by the mother and less time to form a unique bond, which in turn results in decreased cognitive development in the infants (relative to if the child were raised alone). [23] Furthermore, in the newborn period, women who displayed high maternal sensitivity had children who were able to regulate their emotions and who had higher symbolic and cognitive skills. In the case of the triplets, the child that received the least maternal sensitivity was the one that showed the poorest outcomes cognitively and had the most medical problems. [24]

Socialization in childhood

Maternal sensitivity has been shown to have an effect on children's socialization skills. In particular, some research suggests that children of more sensitive caregivers have high levels of effortful (i.e. emotional and behavioural) control. Such control is proposed to have been fostered from the infancy stage when the a sensitive mother's quick and appropriate responses to the baby's distress teaches the baby to adjust his/her arousal. This speedy regulation of arousal is then adapted into childhood resulting in the ability to regulate emotion and behaviour well. [25]

Caregiver sensitivity has also been found to have a connection with empathy in children. Generally, securely attached children have been found to be more empathetic compared with insecurely attached children. The reasoning suggested for this result is that because securely attached children receive more empathy from caregivers during times that they themselves are distressed, they are more likely to show empathy in a situation where someone else is distressed. [26]

Adulthood

Adults' own understanding of maternal sensitivity affects their sensitivity towards their own children. [2] Adults who had insensitive mothers during infancy were found to not be able to remember specific childhood events or their importance. They were not able to present an accurate description of their parents by use of memories, they were found to idealize experiences and are more likely to remember situations in which they were rejected. [27] Adults who experienced higher maternal sensitivity during both infancy and adulthood were found to be less dismissive and more secure than those who did not. Adults who are preoccupied were found to also try to please their parents as they were young, and have a sense of anger towards them. [27] About half of the adults who were found more preoccupied than others were found to have experienced divorce between their parents earlier in life, as well as other negative life events such as death of a parent or sexual abuse. These life events cause the security of attachment between mother and child to decrease as the mother's availability, as well as responsiveness may decrease, no matter the maternal sensitivity experienced prior to these events. [27] Male adults were found to have experienced less maternal sensitivity earlier in life than females and were more likely to be classified as dismissive than females were. [2]

Difference in maternal sensitivity in adult and teen mothers

Maternal sensitivity has been found to be greater for adult mothers than for adolescent mothers. [3] The level and quality of mind-mindedness, which refers to how prone the mother is to comment about the infant's mental activity during interaction, is higher in adult mothers, and has been related to greater maternal sensitivity. The comments made by adult mothers were found to be more positive than those made by adolescent mothers. Adolescent mothers used almost no positive comments, but instead negative comments. This causes the adolescent mother to be more insensitive to their baby's needs, possibly because of lack of need understanding, and therefore have lower maternal sensitivity and a less secure attachment to their infants. [3]

Maternal sensitivity in adolescent mothers can be predicted prenatally. [28] Mothers who talked lively and positively about their future relationship with the child were found to display higher maternal sensitivity than those who did not (classified as autonomous mothers). Autonomous mothers were also found to have infants with a more secure attachment. Adolescent mothers who were not classified as autonomous were found to have anxiously attached infants. [28] Furthermore, adolescent mothers were found to have children four–eight years old with lower IQs and a below-average reading level, than did adult mothers. [29]

Although adolescent mothers have been found to display lower maternal sensitivity, there is no evidence that maternal age itself has a negative effect on child development, as other factors at that age such as education and financial status may play a role in the insensitivity of the mother towards the child as well. [30]

Measurement

Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity Scale (AMSS)

Mary Ainsworth developed Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity Scale (AMSS) to use as a measure in her Baltimore longitudinal study (1963). The scale is based on naturalistic observations completed by Ainsworth over a period of several hours and thus has no short procedure outline. Her method uses a nine-point scale (nine being very high and one being very low) in a number of important maternal traits. In order for this measurement to be accurate, it is essential that the researcher has developed good observations and insight into the behaviour of the caregiver. [8]

Maternal Behaviour Q-sort (MBQS)

Maternal Behaviour Q-sort (MBQS) was developed by David Pederson, Greg Moran and Sandi Bento to measure maternal sensitivity. It has been used to measure a variety of studies including home based and video-recorded observations. The measures are defined using q-factor analyses. [32] The standard version of the Q-sort consists of 90 items that measure maternal sensitivity with regards to accessibility, responsiveness and promptness to the child's needs and there are many variations. In order to measure sensitivity, observers sort the items into nine piles of ten based on correspondence between the observed behaviour and the item. The maternal sensitivity score is calculated by comparing the descriptive sort and the criterion sort (prototypical sensitive mother). Pederson and Moran based their Q-sort on the Waters Attachment Q-Set, which is an assessment of the behavior of children. [33]

Pederson and Moran Sensitivity Q-Sort

The Pederson and Moran Sensitivity Q-Sort was developed by Pederson D.R., Moran G., Sitko C., Campbell K., and Ghesquire K. in 1990. Similar to Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity Scales, the Pederson and Moran Sensitivity Q-Sort was designed to detect changes in maternal sensitivity with relation to infant behaviour. [9]

The Atypical Maternal Behavior Instrument for Assessment and Classification (AMBIANCE) scale was developed by Elisa Bronfman, Elizabeth Parsons and Karlen Lyons-Ruth. It was developed to measure the extent to which the parent failed to follow into the intentional or affective direction of the baby's communications by engaging in contradictory responses to infant cues or failing to respond to infant cues altogether. AMBIANCE has the following five dimensions: [34]

Related Research Articles

Attachment disorder is a broad term intended to describe disorders of mood, behavior, and social relationships arising from unavailability of normal socializing care and attention from primary care giving figures in early childhood. Such a failure would result from unusual early experiences of neglect, abuse, abrupt separation from caregivers between three months and three years of age, frequent change or excessive numbers of caregivers, or lack of caregiver responsiveness to child communicative efforts resulting in a lack of basic trust. A person's attachment style is permanently established before the age of three. A problematic history of social relationships occurring after about age three may be distressing to a child, but does not result in attachment disorder.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reactive attachment disorder</span> Psychological disorder that can affect children

Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) is described in clinical literature as a severe and relatively uncommon disorder that can affect children, although these issues do occasionally persist into adulthood. RAD is characterized by markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate ways of relating socially in most contexts. It can take the form of a persistent failure to initiate or respond to most social interactions in a developmentally appropriate way—known as the "inhibited form". In the DSM-5, the "disinhibited form" is considered a separate diagnosis named "disinhibited attachment disorder".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Attachment theory</span> Psychological ethological theory about human relationships

Attachment theory is a psychological, evolutionary and ethological theory concerning relationships between humans. The most important tenet is that young children need to develop a relationship with at least one primary caregiver for normal social and emotional development. The theory was formulated by psychiatrist and psychoanalyst John Bowlby.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">John Bowlby</span> British psychiatrist and psychoanalyst (1907–1990)

Edward John Mostyn Bowlby, CBE, FBA, FRCP, FRCPsych was a British psychologist, psychiatrist, and psychoanalyst, notable for his interest in child development and for his pioneering work in attachment theory. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Bowlby as the 49th most cited psychologist of the 20th century.

Mary Dinsmore Ainsworth was an American-Canadian developmental psychologist known for her work in the development of the attachment theory. She designed the strange situation procedure to observe early emotional attachment between a child and its primary caregiver.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Attachment in children</span> Biological instinct

Attachment in children is "a biological instinct in which proximity to an attachment figure is sought when the child senses or perceives threat or discomfort. Attachment behaviour anticipates a response by the attachment figure which will remove threat or discomfort". Attachment also describes the function of availability, which is the degree to which the authoritative figure is responsive to the child's needs and shares communication with them. Childhood attachment can define characteristics that will shape the child's sense of self, their forms of emotion-regulation, and how they carry out relationships with others. Attachment is found in all mammals to some degree, especially primates.

Attachment measures refer to the various procedures used to assess the attachment system in children and adults. Researchers have developed various ways of assessing self-protective strategies and patterns of attachment. Some methods work across the several models of attachment and some are model-specific.

Co-regulation is a term used in psychology. It is defined most broadly as a "continuous unfolding of individual action that is susceptible to being continuously modified by the continuously changing actions of the partner". An important aspect of this idea is that co-regulation cannot be reduced down to the behaviors or experiences of the individuals involved in the interaction. The interaction is a result of each participant repeatedly regulating the behavior of the other. It is a continuous and dynamic process, rather than the exchange of discrete information.

Cupboard love is a popular learning theory of the 1950s and 1960s based on the research of Sigmund Freud, Anna Freud, Melanie Klein and Mary Ainsworth. Rooted in psychoanalysis, the theory speculates that attachment develops in the early stages of infancy. This process involves the mother satisfying her infant's instinctual needs, exclusively. Cupboard love theorists conclude that during infancy, our primary drive is food which leads to a secondary drive for attachment.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Maternal deprivation</span> Work on the effects of separating infants/young children from their mother

Maternal deprivation is a scientific term summarising the early work of psychiatrist and psychoanalyst John Bowlby on the effects of separating infants and young children from their mother. Although the effect of loss of the mother on the developing child had been considered earlier by Freud and other theorists, Bowlby's work on delinquent and affectionless children and the effects of hospital and institutional care led to his being commissioned to write the World Health Organization's report on the mental health of homeless children in post-war Europe whilst he was head of the Department for Children and Parents at the Tavistock Clinic in London after World War II. The result was the monograph Maternal Care and Mental Health published in 1951, which sets out the maternal deprivation hypothesis.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">History of attachment theory</span>

Attachment theory, originating in the work of John Bowlby, is a psychological, evolutionary and ethological theory that provides a descriptive and explanatory framework for understanding interpersonal relationships between human beings.

Attachment-based therapy applies to interventions or approaches based on attachment theory, originated by John Bowlby. These range from individual therapeutic approaches to public health programs to interventions specifically designed for foster carers. Although attachment theory has become a major scientific theory of socioemotional development with one of the broadest, deepest research lines in modern psychology, attachment theory has, until recently, been less clinically applied than theories with far less empirical support. This may be partly due to lack of attention paid to clinical application by Bowlby himself and partly due to broader meanings of the word 'attachment' used amongst practitioners. It may also be partly due to the mistaken association of attachment theory with the pseudo-scientific interventions misleadingly known as attachment therapy. The approaches set out below are examples of recent clinical applications of attachment theory by mainstream attachment theorists and clinicians and are aimed at infants or children who have developed or are at risk of developing less desirable, insecure attachment styles or an attachment disorder.

The strange situation is a procedure devised by Mary Ainsworth in the 1970s to observe attachment in children, that is relationships between a caregiver and child. It applies to children between the age of nine and 30 months. Broadly speaking, the attachment styles were (1) secure and (2) insecure. Later, Mary Main and her husband Erik Hesse introduced the 3rd category, disorganized. The procedure played an important role in the development of attachment theory.

Mary Main is an American psychologist notable for her work in the field of attachment. A Professor at the University of California Berkeley, Main is particularly known for her introduction of the 'disorganized' infant attachment classification and for development of the Adult Attachment Interview and coding system for assessing states of mind regarding attachment. This work has been described as 'revolutionary' and Main has been described as having 'unprecedented resonance and influence' in the field of psychology.

Patricia McKinsey Crittenden is an American psychologist known for her work in the development of attachment theory and science, her work in the field of developmental psychopathology, and for creation of the Dynamic-Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM).

Separation anxiety disorder (SAD) is an anxiety disorder in which an individual experiences excessive anxiety regarding separation from home and/or from people to whom the individual has a strong emotional attachment. Separation anxiety is a natural part of the developmental process. It is most common in infants and little children, typically between the ages of six to seven months to three years, although it may pathologically manifest itself in older children, adolescents and adults. Unlike SAD, normal separation anxiety indicates healthy advancements in a child's cognitive maturation and should not be considered a developing behavioral problem.

Studies have found that the father is a child's preferred attachment figure in approximately 5–20% of cases. Fathers and mothers may react differently to the same behaviour in an infant, and the infant may react to the parents' behaviour differently depending on which parent performs it.

There are multiple consequences of different attachment patterns that are formed in childhood development. This article will explore the way attachment patterns are formed, how parents pass on their attachment styles, long-term consequences of attachment patterns, and cross cultural attachment patterns.

Internal working model of attachment is a psychological approach that attempts to describe the development of mental representations, specifically the worthiness of the self and expectations of others' reactions to the self. This model is a result of interactions with primary caregivers which become internalized, and is therefore an automatic process. John Bowlby implemented this model in his attachment theory in order to explain how infants act in accordance with these mental representations. It is an important aspect of general attachment theory.

Social emotional development represents a specific domain of child development. It is a gradual, integrative process through which children acquire the capacity to understand, experience, express, and manage emotions and to develop meaningful relationships with others. As such, social emotional development encompasses a large range of skills and constructs, including, but not limited to: self-awareness, joint attention, play, theory of mind, self-esteem, emotion regulation, friendships, and identity development.

References

  1. 1 2 3 Wu, Tiejian; Wallace E. Dixon; William T. Dalton; Fred Tudiver; Xuefeng Liu (2011). "Joint Effects of Child Temperament and Maternal Sensitivity on the Development of Childhood Obesity". Maternal and Child Health Journal. 15 (4): 469–477. doi:10.1007/s10995-010-0601-z. PMID   20358395. S2CID   24929495.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 Beckwith, Leila; et al. (May 1999). "Maternal Sensitivity During Infancy and Subsequent Life Events Relate to Attachment Representation at Early Adulthood". Developmental Psychology. 35 (3): 693–700. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.693. PMID   10380860 . Retrieved 12 June 2012.
  3. 1 2 3 Demers, Isabelle; Bernier, Annie; et al. (July 2010). "Mind-mindedness in adult and adolescent mothers: Raltions to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 34 (529): 529–537. doi:10.1177/0165025410365802. S2CID   143670689 . Retrieved 13 June 2012.
  4. 1 2 Bento, S.; Evans, E.M.; Moran, G.; Pederson, D.R. (March 2007). "Assessing Maternal Sensitivity from Videotaped Recordings". Psychology Presentations. Retrieved June 7, 2012.
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 McLeod, B. "Mary Ainsworth – Strange Situation". SimplyPsychology. Retrieved June 17, 2012.
  6. 1 2 3 Zentall, S.R.; Boker S.M.; Braungart-Rieker, J.M. (2006). "Mother–Infant synchrony: A dynamical systems approach". Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Development and Learning. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.491.8160 .
  7. 1 2 3 Meins; et al. (2002). "Maternal Mind-Mindedness and Attachment Security as Predictors of Theory of Mind Understanding". Child Development. 73 (6): 1715–1726. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00501. PMID   12487489.
  8. 1 2 Benson, Janette B.; Haith, Marshall M., eds. (2009). Social and emotional development in infancy and early childhood (1st ed.). Amsterdam: Academic. ISBN   978-0-12-375065-5.
  9. 1 2 Moran, G.; Pederson, D. R.; Pettit, P.; Krupka, A. (1992). "Maternal sensitivity and infant-mother attachment in a developmentally delayed sample". Infant Behavior and Development. 15 (4): 427–442. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80011-i.
  10. 1 2 3 Shin, Hyunjeong (2008). "Maternal sensitivity: a concept analysis". Journal of Advanced Nursing. 64 (3): 304–314. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04814.x. PMID   18764848.
  11. Kemppinen, K. (2007). Early Maternal Sensitivity: Continuity and Related Risk Factors. Kuopio University Publications D. Medical Sciences 412. p. 19.
  12. Schechter, DS; Moser, D; Wang, Z; Marsh, R; Hao, XJ; Duan, Y; Yu, S; Gunter, B; Murphy, D; McCaw, J; Kangarlu, A; Willheim, E; Myers, M; Hofer, M; Peterson, BS (2012). "An fMRI study of the brain responses of traumatized mothers to viewing their toddlers during separation and play". Journal of Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 7 (8): 969–79. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr069. PMC   3501701 . PMID   22021653.
  13. Schechter, DS; Willheim, E; Hinojosa, C; Scholfield-Kleinman, K; Turner, JB; McCaw, J; Zeanah, CH; Myers, MM (2010). "Subjective and objective measures of parent-child relationship dysfunction, child separation distress, and joint attention". Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes. 73 (2): 130–144. doi:10.1521/psyc.2010.73.2.130. PMID   20557225. S2CID   5132495.
  14. 1 2 Bernard, K; Dozier, M.; Lindhiem, O (2011). "Maternal Sensitivity: Within-Person Variability and the Utility of Multiple Assessments". Child Maltreatment. 16 (1): 41–50. doi:10.1177/1077559510387662. PMC   3210187 . PMID   21131634.
  15. Atkinson, L.; Goldberg, S.; Raval, V.; Pederson, D.; Benoit, D.; Moran, G.; Leung, E. (2005). "On the relation between maternal state of mind and sensitivity in the prediction of infant attachment security". Developmental Psychology. 41 (1): 42–53. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.42. PMID   15656736.
  16. Pickler, R. H; Reyna, A.B (2009). "Mother–Infant Synchrony". Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing. 38 (4): 470–477. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6909.2009.01044.x. PMID   19614883.
  17. 1 2 Ainsworth, M.S (1979). "Infant-Mother Attachment". American Psychologist. 34 (10): 932–937. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.932. PMID   517843.
  18. Susman-Stillman, A.; et al. (1996). "Infant Temperament and Maternal Sensitivity as Predictors of Attachment Security". Infant Behavior and Development. 19 (1): 33–47. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90042-9.
  19. 1 2 Belsky, Jay; R. M. Pasco Fearon (2002). "Early attachment security, subsequent maternal sensitivity, and later child development: Does continuity in development depend upon continuity of caregiving?". Attachment & Human Development. 4 (3): 361–387. doi:10.1080/14616730210167267. PMID   12537851. S2CID   36800210.
  20. Downer, Jason T.; Robert C. Pianta (2006). "Academic and Cognitive Functioning in First Grade: Associations with Earlier Home and Child Care Predictors and with Concurrent Home and Classroom Experiences" (PDF). School Psychology Review. 35 (1): 11–30. doi:10.1080/02796015.2006.12087999. S2CID   141389189. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 October 2011. Retrieved 4 July 2012.
  21. Bell, Martha Ann; Christy D. Wolfe (March–April 2004). "Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process". Child Development. 75 (2): 366–370. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.502.798 . doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00679.x. PMID   15056192.
  22. Booting, J; Macfarlane, A.; Price, F (1990). "Three, Four and More. A Study of Triplets and Higher Order Births". London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.
  23. Feldman, Ruth; Arthur I. Eidelman; Noa Rotenberg (November–December 2004). "Parenting Stress, Infant Emotion Regulation, Maternal Sensitivity, and the Cognitive Development of Triplets: A Model for Parent and Child Influences in a Unique Ecology". Child Development. 75 (6): 1774–1791. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00816.x. PMID   15566379.
  24. Spinrad, T.L.; Eisenberg, N.; Gaertner, B.; Popp, T.; Smith, C. L.; Kupfer, A.; Greving, K.; Liew, J.; Hofer, C. (2007). "Relations of Maternal Socialization and Toddlers' Effortful Control to Children's Adjustment and Social Competence". Developmental Psychology. 43 (5): 1170–1186. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1170. PMC   2096418 . PMID   17723043.
  25. Kiang, L.; Moreno, A.J.; Robinson, J. L. (2004). "Maternal Preconceptions About Parenting Predict Child Temperament, Maternal Sensitivity, and Children's Empathy". Developmental Psychology. 40 (6): 1081–1092. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1081. PMID   15535758.
  26. 1 2 3 Waters, Everett; Hamiltion, Claire E.; et al. (May–June 2000). "The Stability of Attachment Security from Infancy to Adolescence and early Adulthood: General Introduction". Child Development. 71 (3): 678–683. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00175. PMID   10953933.
  27. 1 2 Ward, Mary J.; Elizabeth A. Carlson (1995). "Associations among Adult Attachment Representations, Maternal Sensitivity, and Infant-Mother Attachment in a Sample of Adolescent Mothers". Child Development. 66 (1): 69–79. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00856.x. PMID   7497830.
  28. Lobi, M; Welcher DW; Mellitz ED (1971). "Maternal age and intellectual functioning of offspring". Johns Hopkins Med J. 128 (6): 347–361. PMID   5559291.
  29. Elster, Arthur B.; Elizabeth R. McAnarney; Michael E. Lamb (April 1983). "Parental Behavior of Adolescent Mothers". Pediatrics. 71 (4): 494–503. doi:10.1542/peds.71.4.494. PMID   6340043. S2CID   6262717 . Retrieved 13 June 2012.
  30. Prior, Vivien; Glaser, Danya (2006). Understanding attachment and attachment disorders : theory, evidence and practice (1. publ. ed.). London; Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. ISBN   978-1-84310-245-8.
  31. Moran, G. (2009). The Maternal Behavior Q-Sort (MBQS) – Overview, Available Materials and Support. SelectedWorks of Greg Moran.
  32. Pederson & Moran (1995). A categorical description of infant-mother relationships in the home and its relation to the Q-sort measures of infant-mother interaction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60 (2/3), 111-132.
  33. Lyons-Ruth K (2008). "Contributions of the mother-infant relationship to dissociative, borderline, and conduct symptoms in young adulthood". Infant Mental Health Journal. 29 (3): 203–218. doi:10.1002/imhj.20173. PMC   2613366 . PMID   19122769.