Reward dependence

Last updated

Reward dependence (RD) is characterized as a tendency to respond markedly to signals of reward, particularly to verbal signals of social approval, social support, and sentiment. [1] When reward dependence levels deviate from normal we see the rise of several personality and addictive disorders.

Contents

RD and gambling disorder Gambling chips.jpg
RD and gambling disorder

In psychology, reward dependence is considered a moderately heritable personality trait which is stable throughout our lives. It is an inherited neurophysiological mechanism that drives our perception of our society and the environment. Even though we are born with these personality traits, their expression during our life span can be modulated throughout our development.

Origin and definitions

Reward dependence is one of the temperament dimensions from the "tridimensional personality theory", which was proposed by C. Robert Cloninger as part of his "unified bio-social theory of personality". [2] His personality theory suggested the hypothesis that specific neurochemical transmitters in our brain determine how we respond to a specific stimulus we may experience.

These innate personality traits can play a significant role not only in an individual's predisposition to certain disorders, but also in their maintenance of those disorders.

By understanding the specific temperamental traits that are common among individuals with specific disorders, clinicians can form a more targeted, informed approach to treatment and look to newer psychotherapies for guidance. The relationship between temperament and individual clusters of DSM personality disorders is often investigated. According to the DSM-IV, [3] the tridimensional structure allows up to nine major personality disorders to be identified, the theory thereby making a great contribution to the understanding of psychiatric disorders.

Cloninger's Tridimension Personality Theory and reward dependence

C. Robert Cloninger CR Cloninger.jpg
C. Robert Cloninger

Cloninger's tridimensional personality theory offers three independent "temperament" dimensions which aid in measuring how different individuals feel or behave. Reward Dependence (RD) is one of the three temperament dimensions, the other two being "Harm Avoidance (HA)" and "Novelty Seeking (NS)". A temperament, according to Cloninger, is the automatic emotional response to experience. [4]

Cloninger suggested that RD is influenced by a single monoamine neurotransmitter system: the noradrenergic system [2] and RD is correlated with low basal noradrenergic activity. This suggestion has led to several research experiments that have investigated and supported his hypothesis, that RD traits are indeed in part determined by norepinephrine. [5] [6] [7]

Measure of the RD dimension, as suggested by Cloninger, is used to assess the Altruistic-warm versus detached-tough mindedness components of our personalities.

According to Cloninger's theory, [8] individuals high in reward dependence and low in norepinephrine levels are ambitious, warm, sentimental, pleasant, sociable, sensitive, sympathetic and socially dependent. Individuals with high RD personalities have a disposition to recognize salient social cues which in turn facilitates effective communication, warm social relations, and their genuine care for others, but these individuals are then disadvantaged in being excessively socially dependent. High reward dependent individuals also exhibit persistent behaviors and are easily influenced by emotional appeals.

Individuals low in reward dependence and high in norepinephrine levels are then hypothesized to be typically independent, non-conformist, practical, tough-minded, cynical, unwilling to share their intimate feelings with others, socially detached, irresolute, insensitive to social cues and pressures, and content to be alone. They are also minimally motivated to please others and act for immediate gratification. So being low in reward dependence is related to social withdrawal, with aggressive anti-social behavior, [9] [10] detachment, and coldness in social attitudes. [11]

The RD temperament interacts with other temperaments and characters inherent in us enabling us to adapt to life experiences and influence susceptibility to emotional and behavioral disorders [11]

Quantifying reward dependence

Two questionnaires were devised by Cloninger to measure the temperaments and characters of individuals. RD can be measured using both the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) personality test and by the newer and refined version of the personality test called Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and its revised version (TCI-R). Owing to the limitations encountered in the TPQ, in that the three dimensions' clinical utility was not readily apparent to many clinicians, [12] Cloninger revamped the questionnaire and produced the TCI scale, which incorporates four dimensions of "temperament" and three dimensions of "character".

The so-called subscales of RD in TCI-R are

  1. Sentimentality (RD1)
  2. Openness to warm communication or social sensitivity (RD2)
  3. Attachment (RD3)
  4. Dependence on approval by others (RD4)

A study comparing the TCI to the five factor model of personality found that reward dependence was substantially positively associated with extraversion and to a lesser extent openness to experience. [13]

Brain physiology

Cloninger describes reward dependence as being a prime component of the Behavior maintenance System (BMS). [6] Norepinephrine (NE), apart from producing alertness and arousal, is seen as influencing the brain reward system [14] by aiding in the learning of new paired associations. [15]

According to Cloninger, the norepinephrine neurotransmitter has its major ascending pathways arising in the locus coeruleus in the pons, projecting onward to the hypothalamic and limbic structures, and then branching upwards to the neocortex. [16]

High reward dependence is characterized by learning from reward signals, persisting repetition of actions that are associated with rewards, increased sociability and a need for social approval. The striatum, especially the dorsal regions, is necessary to carry out these functions. [17] [18]
MRI MRI head side.jpg
MRI
Primary reward processing has also been associated with the orbitofrontal cortex and the grey matter density in the ventral striatum, whereas higher RD scores were associated with the interactions between dopamine projections, neuropeptides and opiates in the ventral striatum. It is demonstrated here, then, that there is evidence for a structural disposition of the brain towards social interactions, and that both sensitivity to salient social reward cues and primary reward processing share the same brain systems. [18]
In addition to these findings, the bilateral temporal poles were also identified in which gray matter density correlated with reward dependence. The medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, along with other temporal structures, project to the temporal pole, enabling reward stimuli information between the two regions to be integrated. In an fMRI study, the temporal poles and the ventral striatal areas have been shown to be activated in response to social rewards (such as humor) and also in looking forward to simpler rewards (such as money). [21]

Relationship to clinical disorders

Cloninger's theory suggests that over expression of the RD temperament could cause psychiatric illnesses, such as addictive behaviors, sociopathies, and personality disorders. [22]

Low levels of norepinephrine cause an increase in reward dependence. When produced in normal levels, norepinephrine creates a sense of well-being, but low levels of norepinephrine cause symptoms of depression, lack of arousal and lack of motivation. In humans, this leads to then a negative feedback mechanism whereby we seek out pleasurable activities to remove the negative affect caused by the low levels of norepinephrine, therefore increasing our reward dependence.

An increase in the RD temperament leads us to seek out those behaviors or substances that will allow us to remain in a pleasant physical and/or mental state, attributing to the fact that we humans are hedonistic individuals, seeking to avoid pain and embracing pleasurable stimuli. [23] Our pleasure and reward systems in the brain are hyper-activated, which makes us display continuous approach behaviors to the reward in question. Our neuro-circuitry is as such that when we stop having access to such pleasurable objects of desire, we then experience negative consequences (withdrawal symptoms). Addictive behaviors then arise to alleviate such negative consequences and the cycle continues. [24]

Relationship to addictive behaviors

People who have high reward dependent personalities but find themselves in situations where they are unable to find rewards and approval through family support or other types, will seek reward through other means e.g. substance abuse or over-eating. It is then suggested that individuals with such predispositions should adopt preventive strategies or avoid such situations, where they seek such rewards that could potentially have adverse consequences. [20] Reward dependence drives behavior maintenance systems, and in addictive behaviors, maintenance of behaviors occurs despite adverse consequences.

The brain on substance dependence. Addictiondependence1.png
The brain on substance dependence.

According to researchers the reward circuit is continuously involved in initiating behaviors that are essential for the sustenance of the individual (such as eating) or of the preservation of the species (such as sexual reproduction). Food intake or sexual stimuli then lead to endorphin regulated release of dopamine in core substrates of the reward processing system, which subsequently activate craving sensations. The development of addictions is then directly related to the acquired or genetic abuse of the reward circuit. This finding then suggests that individuals with increased reward dependence have a relative deficit of endorphins and are possibly at higher risk for developing addictive behaviors. [19]

Adolescents low in reward dependence may find conventional sources of reward unfulfilling (e.g., academic achievement or activities involving social affiliation), and eventually turn to unconventional reward sources (e.g., smoking and other forms of substance misuse) to derive satisfaction and pleasure. [25] A study conducted using adolescents with Excessive Internet video Game Play (EIGP) showed that such addictive behaviors were correlated with higher RD scores on the TCI, [26] whereas both low and high scores of RD were implicated in specific alcoholic and drug addictions.

Low reward dependence is then hypothesized to fit the psychological profile of Type 2 Alcoholism (predominantly in males). Type 2 Alcoholism is clinically characterized by an early onset of alcohol-related problems before the age of 25 (independent of external circumstances), drinking often associated with antisocial behavior, spontaneous alcohol-seeking behavior (inability to abstain), low associated guilt and fear, and frequent fighting and arrests after drinking. This combination of traits also describes people with Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) and is consistent with findings that type 2 alcoholics frequently suffer from ASPD. Since the inception of these theories they have been tested in several investigations and the results have been consistent with the theories. [16]
Type 2 Alcoholism The Drunkard's Progress 1846.jpg
Type 2 Alcoholism
fMRI studies also show that brain systems involved in the processing of primary rewards, such as the striatum, may sub serve social rewards processing. Since higher RD scores are also correlated to the basal ganglia of the ventral striatum, this explains why Parkinson's disease (PD) patients on medications indulge in impulsive gambling behaviors. [31] Pathologic gamblers were shown to experience stronger cravings than did Alcohol-Dependent Subjects (ADS). [29] This may be a disturbing experience for pathologic gamblers and a potential cause for relapse.

Relationship to personality disorders

Social reward dependence is hypothesized to be related to behaviors that represent the RD personality dimension sub-scales of social sensitivity and attachment. It is suggested that the brain substrates that are involved in the response to simple primary rewards stimuli are also implicated in the response to complex social rewards stimuli. The RD sub-scale from the TCI inventory measures how sensitive individuals are to social rewards. High RD scores on the test correlate to increased attachment and an increased need for social relationships. Low RD scores show a movement towards social detachment and insensitivity. [21]

When reward dependence goes wrong OxyContin setup.jpg
When reward dependence goes wrong
Also a significant positive correlation between plasma oxytocin levels and RD personality dimension was found by researchers in New Zealand. Considerable evidence from animal studies has shown oxytocin to be involved in the processing of social information and the regulation of social affiliative behavior. Results from studies measuring plasma oxytocin levels in patients with a diagnosis of major depressive episode according to DSM III-R has shown decreased oxytocin levels in these patients and lower RD scores on the TCI, leading to an assumption that lower reward dependence leads to depression. [37]
Lower RD scores and decreased plasma oxytocin levels have also typically been associated with paranoid, schizoid and schizotypal personality disorders. [37]
Research has also found a low expression of reward dependence in suicide attempters. [39] These findings and the negative correlation between reward dependence and the number of suicide attempts may suggest the implication of the noradrenergic pathway in suicide behaviors. Low scores of RD have also been implicated in showing criminality in adult age.

At the 2010 Annual meeting of the American Psychological Association (APA), a study looking at treatment methods of personality disorders was presented in which it was found, that panic disorder patients with higher RD scores were more resistant to Escitalopram treatment. Since long-term pharmacotherapy is needed for treatment of panic disorder, the present results suggest that development of therapeutic strategy for panic patients with high reward dependence is needed. [40]

Other clinical disorders

In response to a lack of social reward, individuals with high reward dependence are more likely to have increased noradrenergic activity. These individuals experience feelings of depression, agitation and extreme discontent, leading them to indulge in habits that reinforce reward-seeking, such as increased sexual activity or overeating. [2]

Individuals with higher reward dependence also look for more social approval, and are more inclined to succumb to peer-pressure. They often become overly concerned with their body image and maybe prone to eating disorders, such as Bulimia nervosa. [41] Whereas restricting anorexia, in particular, tends to reflect low reward dependence. [42]

Reward dependence is not consistently associated with diagnosis but can also significantly affect treatment issues, such as Therapeutic alliance. Persistence in completing weight-loss programs were related to high RD scores during pre-treatment. This demonstrates that, having higher reward dependence enhances an individual's disposition to being more dedicated and sociable, making them increasingly respond to societal pressures, thereby reducing their risk of dropping out from weight-loss programs.[ medical citation needed ]

Other research

A study of norepinephrine levels in gamblers found high cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels of the norepinephrine (NE) metabolite 3-methoq-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MI-IPG). [43] This could suggest a relationship between the noradrenergic system and gambling, and gambling could be seen as a reward-dependent behavior, but according to Cloninger's theory these gamblers should exhibit low levels of NE, not high. Cloninger suggests that had these gamblers been retested after they had refrained from gambling for a determined period, their CSF MHPG levels might have been low. [12] A study of norepinephrine levels in alcoholics with high reward dependence also showed a significant decrease of MHPG in their CSF. [44] This finding demonstrates a significant relationship between reward dependence and norepinephrine.

Several genes have also been found to express the RD temperament dimension. Specifically, the gene MAOA-uVNTR has been highly implicated in evoking the RD personality trait. [45]

See also

Related Research Articles

In psychology, schizotypy is a theoretical concept that posits a continuum of personality characteristics and experiences, ranging from normal dissociative, imaginative states to extreme states of mind related to psychosis, especially schizophrenia. The continuum of personality proposed in schizotypy is in contrast to a categorical view of psychosis, wherein psychosis is considered a particular state of mind, which the person either has or does not have.

Motivational salience is a cognitive process and a form of attention that motivates or propels an individual's behavior towards or away from a particular object, perceived event or outcome. Motivational salience regulates the intensity of behaviors that facilitate the attainment of a particular goal, the amount of time and energy that an individual is willing to expend to attain a particular goal, and the amount of risk that an individual is willing to accept while working to attain a particular goal.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Problem gambling</span> Repetitive gambling despite demonstrable harm and adverse consequences

Problem gambling or ludomania is repetitive gambling behavior despite harm and negative consequences. Problem gambling may be diagnosed as a mental disorder according to DSM-5 if certain diagnostic criteria are met. Pathological gambling is a common disorder associated with social and family costs.

According to proponents of the concept, sexual addiction, also known as sex addiction, is a state characterized by compulsive participation or engagement in sexual activity, particularly sexual intercourse, despite negative consequences. The concept is contentious; neither of the two major mainstream medical categorization systems recognise sex addiction as a real medical condition, instead categorizing such behavior under labels such as compulsive sexual behavior.

An addictive behavior is a behavior, or a stimulus related to a behavior, that is both rewarding and reinforcing, and is associated with the development of an addiction. There are two main forms of addiction: substance use disorders and behavioral addiction. The parallels and distinctions between behavioral addictions and other compulsive behavior disorders like bulimia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are still being researched by behavioral scientists.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Impulsivity</span> Tendency to act on a whim without considering consequences

In psychology, impulsivity is a tendency to act on a whim, displaying behavior characterized by little or no forethought, reflection, or consideration of the consequences. Impulsive actions are typically "poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation that often result in undesirable consequences," which imperil long-term goals and strategies for success. Impulsivity can be classified as a multifactorial construct. A functional variety of impulsivity has also been suggested, which involves action without much forethought in appropriate situations that can and does result in desirable consequences. "When such actions have positive outcomes, they tend not to be seen as signs of impulsivity, but as indicators of boldness, quickness, spontaneity, courageousness, or unconventionality." Thus, the construct of impulsivity includes at least two independent components: first, acting without an appropriate amount of deliberation, which may or may not be functional; and second, choosing short-term gains over long-term ones.

The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) is an inventory for personality traits devised by Cloninger et al. It is closely related to and an outgrowth of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ), and it has also been related to the dimensions of personality in Zuckerman's alternative five and Eysenck's models and those of the five factor model.

Behavioral addiction, process addiction, or non-substance-related disorder is a form of addiction that involves a compulsion to engage in a rewarding non-substance-related behavior – sometimes called a natural reward – despite any negative consequences to the person's physical, mental, social or financial well-being. In the brain's reward system, a gene transcription factor known as ΔFosB has been identified as a necessary common factor involved in both behavioral and drug addictions, which are associated with the same set of neural adaptations.

Harm avoidance (HA) is a personality trait characterized by excessive worrying; pessimism; shyness; and being fearful, doubtful, and easily fatigued. In MRI studies HA was correlated with reduced grey matter volume in the orbito-frontal, occipital and parietal regions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">C. Robert Cloninger</span>

Claude Robert Cloninger is an American psychiatrist and geneticist noted for his research on the biological, psychological, social, and spiritual foundation of both mental health and mental illness. He previously held the Wallace Renard Professorship of Psychiatry, and served as professor of psychology and genetics, as well as director of the Sansone Family Center for Well-Being at Washington University in St. Louis. Cloninger is a member of the evolutionary, neuroscience, and statistical genetics programs of the Division of Biology and Biomedical Sciences at Washington University, and is recognized as an expert clinician in the treatment of general psychopathology, substance dependence, and personality disorders. Dr. Cloninger is currently professor emeritus.

Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) is a personality test. It was devised by C. Robert Cloninger. A newer version of the questionnaire is called Temperament and Character Inventory.

In psychology, novelty seeking (NS) is a personality trait associated with exploratory activity in response to novel stimulation, impulsive decision making, extravagance in approach to reward cues, quick loss of temper, and avoidance of frustration. That is, Novelty seeking refers to the tendency to pursue new experiences with intense emotional sensations. It is a multifaceted behavioral construct that includes thrill seeking, novelty preference, risk taking, harm avoidance, and reward dependence. The novelty-seeking trait is considered a heritable tendency of individuals to take risks for the purpose of achieving stimulation and seeking new environments and situations that make their experiences more intense. This trait has been associated with the level of motive and excitement in response to novelty. Persons with high levels of novelty seeking have been described as more impulsive and disorderly than low novelty seekers and have a higher propensity to get involved in risky activities, such as starting to misuse drugs, engaging in risky sexual activities, and suffering accidental injuries. It is measured in the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire as well as the later version Temperament and Character Inventory and is considered one of the temperament dimensions of personality. Like the other temperament dimensions, it has been found to be highly heritable. Another related term, Variety seeking or variety-seeking buying behavior describes a consumer's desire to search for alternative products even if she or he is satisfied with a current product. For example, someone may drink tea with lunch one day but choose orange juice the next day specifically to get something different. High NS has been suggested to be related to low dopaminergic activity.

An addictive personality refers to a hypothesized set of personality traits that make an individual predisposed to developing addictions. This hypothesis states that there may be common personality traits observable in people suffering from addiction; however, the lack of a universally agreed upon definition has marked the research surrounding addictive personality. Addiction is a fairly broad term; it is most often associated with substance use disorders, but it can also be extended to cover a number of other compulsive behaviors, including sex, internet, television, gambling, food, and shopping. Within these categories of addiction a common diagnostic scale involves tolerance, withdrawal, and cravings. This is a fairly contentious topic, with many experts suggesting the term be retired due to a lack of cumulative evidence supporting the existence of addictive personality. It has been claimed that characteristics of personality attributed to addictive personality do not predict addiction, but rather can be the result of addiction. However, different personality traits have been linked to various types of addictive behaviors, suggesting that individual addictions may be associated with different personality profiles. The strongest consensus is that genetic factors play the largest role in determining a predisposition for addictive behaviors. Even then, however, genes play different roles in different types of addictions. Forty to seventy percent of the population variance in the expression of addictions can be explained by genetic factors.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Addiction</span> Disorder resulting in compulsive behaviours

Addiction is a neuropsychological disorder characterized by a persistent and intense urge to use a drug or engage in a behaviour that produces natural reward, despite substantial harm and other negative consequences. Repetitive drug use often alters brain function in ways that perpetuate craving, and weakens self-control. This phenomenon – drugs reshaping brain function – has led to an understanding of addiction as a brain disorder with a complex variety of psychosocial as well as neurobiological factors that are implicated in addiction's development. Classic signs of addiction include compulsive engagement in rewarding stimuli, preoccupation with substances or behavior, and continued use despite negative consequences. Habits and patterns associated with addiction are typically characterized by immediate gratification, coupled with delayed deleterious effects.

Reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) proposes three brain-behavioral systems that underlie individual differences in sensitivity to reward, punishment, and motivation. While not originally defined as a theory of personality, the RST has been used to study and predict anxiety, impulsivity, and extraversion. The theory evolved from Gray's biopsychological theory of personality to incorporate findings from a number of areas in psychology and neuroscience, culminating in a major revision in 2000. The revised theory distinguishes between fear and anxiety and proposes functionally related subsystems. Measures of RST have not been widely adapted to reflect the revised theory due to disagreement over related versus independent subsystems. Despite this controversy, RST informed the study of anxiety disorders in clinical settings and continues to be used today to study and predict work performance. RST, built upon Gray's behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and behavioral activation system (BAS) understanding, also may help to suggest predispositions to and predict alcohol and drug abuse. RST, a continuously evolving paradigm, is the subject of multiple areas of contemporary psychological enquiry.

Self-transcendence is a personality trait that involves the expansion or evaporation of personal boundaries. This may potentially include spiritual experiences such as considering oneself an integral part of the universe. Several psychologists, including Viktor Frankl, Abraham Maslow, Pamela G. Reed, C. Robert Cloninger, Lars Tornstam, and Scott Barry Kaufman have made contributions to the theory of self-transcendence.

Self-directedness is a personality trait held by someone with characteristic self-determination, that is, the ability to regulate and adapt behavior to the demands of a situation in order to achieve personally chosen goals and values.

Cooperativeness is a personality trait that concerns how much a person is generally agreeable in their relations with other people as opposed to aggressively self-centered and hostile.

Personality theories of addiction are psychological models that associate personality traits or modes of thinking with an individual's proclivity for developing an addiction. Models of addiction risk that have been proposed in psychology literature include an affect dysregulation model of positive and negative psychological affects, the reinforcement sensitivity theory model of impulsiveness and behavioral inhibition, and an impulsivity model of reward sensitization and impulsiveness.

Nancy M. Petry was a psychologist known for her research on behavioral treatments for addictive disorders, behavioral pharmacology, impulsivity and compulsive gambling. She was Professor of Medicine at the University of Connecticut Health Center. Petry served as a member of the American Psychiatric Association Workgroup on Substance Use Disorders for the DSM-5 and chaired the Subcommittee on Non-Substance Behavioral Addictions. The latter category includes Internet addiction disorder and problem gambling. She also served as a member of the Board of Advisors of Children and Screens: Institute of Digital Media and Child Development.

References

  1. Theodore Millon; Melvin J. Lerner; Irving Bernard Weiner (January 3, 2003). Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology. Wiley. p. 688. ISBN   978-0-471-38404-5.
  2. 1 2 3 4 Cloninger, C. R. (1986). "A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states". Psychiatric Developments. 4 (3): 167–226. PMID   3809156.
  3. W. John Livesley (19 May 1995). The DSM-IV personality disorders . Guilford Press. ISBN   978-0-89862-257-7 . Retrieved 16 November 2011.
  4. Nixon, S.; Parsons, O. (1989). "Cloninger's tridimensional theory of personality: Construct validity in a sample of college students". Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (12): 1261. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90238-9.
  5. Ham, B. J.; Choi, M. J.; Lee, H. J.; Kang, R. H.; Lee, M. S. (2005). "Reward dependence is related to norepinephrine transporter T-182C gene polymorphism in a Korean population". Psychiatric Genetics. 15 (2): 145–147. doi:10.1097/00041444-200506000-00012. PMID   15900230. S2CID   13270324.
  6. 1 2 Garvey, M. J.; Noyes Jr, R.; Cook, B.; Blum, N. (1996). "Preliminary confirmation of the proposed link between reward-dependence traits and norepinephrine". Psychiatry Research. 65 (1): 61–64. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(96)02954-X. PMID   8953662. S2CID   35689266.
  7. Gerra, G.; Zaimovic, A.; Timpano, M.; Zambelli, U.; Delsignore, R.; Brambilla, F. (2000). "Neuroendocrine correlates of temperamental traits in humans". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 25 (5): 479–496. doi:10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00004-4. PMID   10818282. S2CID   30906909.
  8. Pud, D.; Eisenberg, E.; Sprecher, E.; Rogowski, Z.; Yarnitsky, D. (2004). "The tridimensional personality theory and pain: Harm avoidance and reward dependence traits correlate with pain perception in healthy volunteers". European Journal of Pain. 8 (1): 31–38. doi:10.1016/S1090-3801(03)00065-X. PMID   14690672. S2CID   6872020.
  9. 1 2 3 Mario Maj (4 April 2005). Personality disorders. John Wiley and Sons. ISBN   978-0-470-09036-7 . Retrieved 17 November 2011.
  10. 1 2 Joan McCord (1992). Facts, frameworks, and forecasts. Transaction Publishers. ISBN   978-0-88738-363-2 . Retrieved 17 November 2011.
  11. 1 2 "TCI, A comprehensive assessment of Personality". Archived from the original on 2012-04-25. Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  12. 1 2 Cloninger, C. R. (1988). "A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states: A reply to commentaries". Psychiatric Developments. 6 (2): 83–120. PMID   2907136.
  13. De Fruyt, F.; Van De Wiele, L.; Van Heeringen, C. (2000). "Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five-Factor Model of Personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 29 (3): 441–452. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4.
  14. Weinshenker, D.; Schroeder, J. P. (2006). "There and Back Again: A Tale of Norepinephrine and Drug Addiction". Neuropsychopharmacology. 32 (7): 1433–1451. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301263 . PMID   17164822.
  15. 1 2 3 Pomerleau, C. S.; Pomerleau, O. F.; Flessland, K. A.; Basson, S. M. (1992). "Relationship of Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire scores and smoking variables in female and male smokers". Journal of Substance Abuse. 4 (2): 143–154. doi:10.1016/0899-3289(92)90014-o. hdl: 2027.42/30311 . PMID   1504639.
  16. 1 2 Cloninger, C. R. (1987). "Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism". Science. 236 (4800): 410–416. Bibcode:1987Sci...236..410C. doi:10.1126/science.2882604. PMID   2882604.
  17. 1 2 3 4 Cohen, M. X.; Schoene-Bake, J. C.; Elger, C. E.; Weber, B. (2008). "Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics". Nature Neuroscience. 12 (1): 32–34. doi:10.1038/nn.2228. PMID   19029888. S2CID   148747313.
  18. 1 2 3 4 Lebreton, M. L.; Barnes, A.; Miettunen, J.; Peltonen, L.; Ridler, K.; Veijola, J.; Tanskanen, P. I.; Suckling, J.; Jarvelin, M. R.; Jones, P. B.; Isohanni, M.; Bullmore, E. T.; Murray, G. K. (2009). "The brain structural disposition to social interaction". European Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (11): 2247–2252. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06782.x. PMID   19490022. S2CID   17843868.
  19. 1 2 Schreckenberger, M.; Klega, A.; Gründer, G.; Buchholz, H. -G.; Scheurich, A.; Schirrmacher, R.; Schirrmacher, E.; Müller, C.; Henriksen, G.; Bartenstein, P. (2008). "Opioid Receptor PET Reveals the Psychobiologic Correlates of Reward Processing". Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 49 (8): 1257–1261. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.108.050849 . PMID   18632824.
  20. 1 2 "Personality traits reflect individual structural differences in the brain" . Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  21. 1 2 Kondo, H.; Saleem, K. S.; Price, J. L. (2003). "Differential connections of the temporal pole with the orbital and medial prefrontal networks in macaque monkeys". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 465 (4): 499–523. doi:10.1002/cne.10842. PMID   12975812. S2CID   9346736.
  22. Yamano, E.; Isowa, T.; Nakano, Y.; Matsuda, F.; Hashimoto-Tamaoki, T.; Ohira, H.; Kosugi, S. (2008). "Association study between reward dependence temperament and a polymorphism in the phenylethanolamine N-methyltransferase gene in a Japanese female population". Comprehensive Psychiatry. 49 (5): 503–507. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.03.001. PMID   18702937.
  23. "Pleasure Systems in the Brain" . Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  24. "Drug Dependency" . Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  25. 1 2 Tercyak, K. P.; Audrain-Mcgovern, J. (2003). "Personality differences associated with smoking experimentation among adolescents with and without comorbid symptoms of ADHD". Substance Use & Misuse. 38 (14): 1953–1970. doi:10.1081/ja-120025121. PMID   14677777. S2CID   8348331.
  26. Han, D. H.; Lee, Y. S.; Yang, K. C.; Kim, E. Y.; Lyoo, I. K.; Renshaw, P. F. (2007). "Dopamine Genes and Reward Dependence in Adolescents with Excessive Internet Video Game Play". Journal of Addiction Medicine. 1 (3): 133–138. doi:10.1097/ADM.0b013e31811f465f. PMID   21768948. S2CID   205450169.
  27. Varma, V. K.; Basu, D.; Malhotra, A.; Sharma, A.; Mattoo, S. K. (1994). "Correlates of early- and late-onset alcohol dependence". Addictive Behaviors. 19 (6): 609–619. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)90016-7. PMID   7701972.
  28. Cunningham-Williams, R. M.; Grucza, R. A.; Cottler, L. B.; Womack, S. B.; Books, S. J.; Przybeck, T. R.; Spitznagel, E. L.; Cloninger, C. R. (2005). "Prevalence and predictors of pathological gambling: Results from the St. Louis personality, health and lifestyle (SLPHL) study". Journal of Psychiatric Research. 39 (4): 377–390. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.09.002. PMC   1618765 . PMID   15804388.
  29. 1 2 Tavares, H.; Zilberman, M. L.; Hodgins, D. C.; El-Guebaly, N. (2005). "Comparison of craving between pathological gamblers and alcoholics". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 29 (8): 1427–1431. doi:10.1097/01.alc.0000175071.22872.98. PMID   16131850.
  30. Kim, S. W.; Grant, J. E. (2001). "Personality dimensions in pathological gambling disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder". Psychiatry Research. 104 (3): 205–212. doi:10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00327-4. PMID   11728609. S2CID   42180954.
  31. "Gambling among Parkinson's Patients" . Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  32. Anckarsater, H.; Stahlberg, O.; Larson, T.; Hakansson, C.; Jutblad, S. -B.; Niklasson, L.; Nydén, A.; Wentz, E.; Westergren, S.; Cloninger, C. R.; Gillberg, C.; Rastam, M. (2006). "The Impact of ADHD and Autism Spectrum Disorders on Temperament, Character, and Personality Development". American Journal of Psychiatry. 163 (7): 1239–1244. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.7.1239. PMID   16816230.
  33. Modahl, C.; Green, L.; Fein, D.; Morris, M.; Waterhouse, L.; Feinstein, C.; Levin, H. (1998). "Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children". Biological Psychiatry. 43 (4): 270–277. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00439-3 . PMID   9513736. S2CID   922825.
  34. Green, L.; Fein, D.; Modahl, C.; Feinstein, C.; Waterhouse, L.; Morris, M. (2001). "Oxytocin and autistic disorder: Alterations in peptide forms". Biological Psychiatry. 50 (8): 609–613. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01139-8. PMID   11690596. S2CID   776555.
  35. Insel, T. R.; O'Brien, D. J.; Leckman, J. F. (1999). "Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: Is there a connection?". Biological Psychiatry. 45 (2): 145–157. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00142-5. PMID   9951561. S2CID   34384852.
  36. Kosfeld, M.; Heinrichs, M.; Zak, P. J.; Fischbacher, U.; Fehr, E. (2005). "Oxytocin increases trust in humans". Nature. 435 (7042): 673–676. Bibcode:2005Natur.435..673K. doi:10.1038/nature03701. PMID   15931222. S2CID   1234727.
  37. 1 2 Bell, C. J.; Nicholson, H.; Mulder, R. T.; Luty, S. E.; Joyce, P. R. (2006). "Plasma oxytocin levels in depression and their correlation with the temperament dimension of reward dependence". Journal of Psychopharmacology. 20 (5): 656–660. doi:10.1177/0269881106060512. PMID   16401658. S2CID   22816209.
  38. Goekoop, J.; De Winter, R.; Wolterbeek, R.; Spinhoven, P.; Zitman, F.; Wiegant, V. (2008). "Reduced cooperativeness and reward-dependence in depression with above-normal plasma vasopressin concentration". Journal of Psychopharmacology. 23 (8): 891–897. doi:10.1177/0269881108093584. PMID   18583437. S2CID   22298032.
  39. "Suicide and Personality" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2009-04-10. Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  40. "The Effect of Temperament and Character on the Treatment Outcome After 24-Week Pharmacotherapy with Eecitalopram in Patients with Panic Disorder" (PDF). p. 9. Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  41. "Personality Traits Associated with Bulimia Nervosa". Archived from the original on 2012-04-25. Retrieved 2011-11-17.
  42. Aimee Liu (22 February 2007). Gaining: the truth about life after eating disorders. Hachette Digital, Inc. ISBN   978-0-446-57766-3 . Retrieved 18 November 2011.
  43. Roy, A.; Adinoff, B.; Roehrich, L.; Lamparski, D.; Custer, R.; Lorenz, V.; Barbaccia, M.; Guidotti, A.; Costa, E.; Linnoila, M. (1988). "Pathological gambling. A psychobiological study". Archives of General Psychiatry. 45 (4): 369–373. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800280085011. PMID   2451490.
  44. Borg, S.; Kvande, H.; Mossberg, D.; Valverius, P.; Sedvall, G. (1983). "Central nervous system noradrenaline metabolism and alcohol consumption in man". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 18 Suppl 1: 375–378. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(83)90202-2. PMID   6634849. S2CID   40483709.
  45. "Brede Wiki for Personality Genetics" . Retrieved 2011-11-17.