Animal language

Last updated
Parrots (Australian ringneck) Mallee ringneck 42 - Patchewollock.jpg
Parrots (Australian ringneck)

Animal languages are forms of non-human animal communication that show similarities to human language. [1] Animals communicate through a variety of signs, such as sounds and movements. Signing among animals may be considered a form of language if the inventory of signs is large enough. The signs are relatively arbitrary, and the animals seem to produce them with a degree of volition (as opposed to relatively automatic conditioned behaviors or unconditioned instincts, usually including facial expressions). In experimental tests, animal communication may also be evidenced through the use of lexigrams by chimpanzees and bonobos. [2] [3]

Contents

Many researchers argue that animal communication lacks a key aspect of human language, the creation of new patterns of signs under varied circumstances. Humans, by contrast, routinely produce entirely new combinations of words. Some researchers, including the linguist Charles Hockett, argue that human language and animal communication differ so much that the underlying principles are unrelated. [4] Accordingly, linguist Thomas A. Sebeok has proposed to not use the term "language" for animal sign systems. [5] However, other linguists and biologists, including Marc Hauser, Noam Chomsky, and W. Tecumseh Fitch, assert an evolutionary continuum exists between the communication methods of animal and human language. [6]

Aspects of human language

Human (Claudine Andre) and bonobo. Claudine andre.jpg
Human (Claudine André) and bonobo.

Human language contains the following properties. Some experts argue these properties separate human language from animal communication: [7]

Research with apes, like that of Francine Patterson with Koko [9] (gorilla) or Allen and Beatrix Gardner with Washoe [10] [11] (chimpanzee), suggested that apes are capable of using language that meets some of these requirements, including arbitrariness, discreteness, and productivity. [12]

In the wild, chimpanzees have been seen "talking" to each other when warning about approaching danger. For example, if one chimpanzee sees a snake, said chimpanzee may make a low, rumbling noise, signaling for all the other chimps to climb into nearby trees. [13] In this case, the chimpanzees' communication does not indicate displacement, as it is entirely contained to an observable event.

Arbitrariness has been noted in meerkat calls; bee dances demonstrate elements of spatial displacement; and cultural transmission has possibly occurred through language between the bonobos named Kanzi and Panbanisha. [14]

Human language may also not be completely "arbitrary." Research has shown that almost all humans naturally demonstrate limited Crossmodal perception (e.g. synesthesia) and multisensory integration, as illustrated by the Kiki and Bouba study. [15] [16] Other recent research has tried to explain how the structure of human language emerged, comparing two different aspects of hierarchical structure present in animal communication and proposing that human language arose out of these two separate systems. [17]

Claims that animals have language skills akin to humans, however, are extremely controversial. In his book The Language Instinct , [18] Steven Pinker illustrates that claims of chimpanzees acquiring language are exaggerated and rest on very limited or specious evidence. [18]

The American linguist Charles Hockett theorized that there are sixteen features of human language that distinguish human communication from that of animals. He called these the design features of language. The features mentioned below have so far been found in all spoken human languages, and at least one is missing from any other animal communication system.

Primates

Humans are able to distinguish real words from fake words based on the phonological order of the word itself. In a 2013 study, baboons were shown to have this skill as well. The discovery has led researchers to believe that reading is not as advanced a skill as previously believed, but instead based on the ability to recognize and distinguish letters from one another. The experimental setup consisted of six young adult baboons, and results were measured by allowing the animals to use a touch screen and select whether or not the displayed word was a real word, or a non-word such as "dran" or "telk". The study lasted for six weeks, with approximately 50,000 tests completed in that time. The researchers minimized common bigrams, or combinations of two letters, in non-words, and maximized them in real words. Further studies will attempt to teach baboons how to use an artificial alphabet. [22]

In a 2016 study, a team of biologists from several universities concluded that macaques possess vocal tracts physically capable of speech, "but lack a speech-ready brain to control it". [23] [24]

Non-primates

Among the most studied examples of non-primate languages are:

Birds

Insects

Mammals

Aquatic mammals

  • Bottlenose dolphins: Dolphins can hear one another up to 6 miles apart underwater. [30] Researchers observed a mother dolphin successfully communicating with her baby using a telephone. It appeared that both dolphins knew who they were speaking with and what they were speaking about. Not only do dolphins communicate via nonverbal cues, they also seem to chatter and respond to other dolphins' vocalizations. [31]
Spectrogram of humpback whale vocalizations. Detail is shown for the first 24 seconds of the 37 second humpback whale "song" recording. The whale songs and echolocation "clicks" are visible as horizontal striations and vertical sweeps respectively. HumBack2.jpg
Spectrogram of humpback whale vocalizations. Detail is shown for the first 24 seconds of the 37 second humpback whale "song" recording. The whale songs and echolocation "clicks" are visible as horizontal striations and vertical sweeps respectively.
  • Whales: Two groups of whales, the humpback whale and a subspecies of blue whale found in the Indian Ocean, are known to produce repeated sounds at varying frequencies, known as whale songs. Male humpback whales perform these vocalizations only during the mating season, and so it is surmised the purpose of songs is to aid sexual selection. Humpbacks also make a sound called a feeding call, which is five to ten seconds in length at a nearly constant frequency. Humpbacks generally feed cooperatively by gathering in groups, swimming underneath shoals of fish and lunging up vertically through the fish and out of the water together. Prior to these lunges, whales make their feeding call. The exact purpose of the call is not known, but research suggests that fish react to it. When the sound was played back to them, a group of herring responded to the sound by moving away from the call, even though no whale was present.[ citation needed ]
  • Sea lions: Since 1971, Ronald J. Schusterman and his research associates have studied sea lions' cognitive ability. They have discovered that sea lions are able to recognize relationships between stimuli based on similar functions or connections made with their peers, rather than only the stimuli's common features. This is called equivalence classification. This ability to recognize equivalence may be a precursor to language. [32] Research is currently being conducted at the Pinniped Cognition & Sensory Systems Laboratory to determine how sea lions form these equivalence relations. Sea lions have also been proven to understand simple syntax and commands when taught an artificial sign language similar to one used with primates. [33] The sea lions studied were able to learn and use a number of syntactic relations between the signs they were taught, such as how the signs should be arranged in relation to each other. However, the sea lions rarely used the signs semantically or logically. [34] In the wild it is thought that sea lions use reasoning skills associated with equivalence relations in order to make important decisions that can affect their survival, e.g. recognizing friends and relatives or avoiding enemies and predators. [32] Sea lions use various postural positions and a range of barks, chirps, clicks, moans, growls and squeaks to communicate. [35] It has yet to be proven that sea lions use echolocation as a means of communication. [36]

The effects of learning on auditory signaling in these animals is of interest to researchers. Several investigators have pointed out that some marine mammals appear to have the capacity to alter both the contextual and structural features of their vocalizations as a result of experience. Janik and Slater have stated that learning can modify vocalizations in one of two ways, by influencing the context in which a particular call is used, or by altering the acoustic structure of the call itself. [37] Male California sea lions can learn to inhibit their barking in the presence of any male dominant to them, but vocalize normally when dominant males are absent. [38] The different call types of gray seals can be selectively conditioned and controlled by different cues, [39] and the use of food reinforcement can also modify vocal emissions. A captive male harbor seal named Hoover demonstrated a case of vocal mimicry, but similar observations have not been reported since. Still shows that under the right circumstances pinnipeds may use auditory experience in addition to environmental consequences such as food reinforcement and social feedback to modify their vocal emissions.[ citation needed ]

In a 1992 study, Robert Gisiner and Schusterman conducted experiments in which they attempted to teach syntax to a female California sea lion named Rocky. [34] Rocky was taught signed words, then she was asked to perform various tasks dependent on word order after viewing a signed instruction. It was found that Rocky was able to determine relations between signs and words, and form basic syntax. [34] A 1993 study by Schusterman and David Kastak found that the California sea lion was capable of understanding abstract concepts such as symmetry, sameness and transitivity. This suggests that equivalence relations can form without language.

The distinctive sounds of sea lions are produced both above and below water. To mark territory, sea lions "bark", with non-alpha males making more noise than alphas. Although females also bark, they do so less frequently and most often in connection with birthing pups or caring for their young. Females produce a highly directional bawling vocalization, the pup attraction call, which helps mothers and pups locate one another. As noted in Animal Behavior, their amphibious lifestyle has made them need acoustic communication for social organization while on land.

Sea lions can hear frequencies between 100  Hz and 40,000 Hz, and vocalize from 100 to 10,000 Hz. [40]

Mollusks

Fish

Comparison of "animal language" and "animal communication"

It is worth distinguishing "animal language" from "animal communication", although there is some comparative interchange in certain cases (e.g. Cheney & Seyfarth's vervet monkey call studies). [47] Animal language typically does not include bee dancing, bird song, whale song, dolphin signature whistles, prairie dogs, nor the communicative systems found in most social mammals.[ citation needed ] The features of language as listed above are a dated formulation by Hockett in 1960. Through this formulation Hockett made one of the earliest attempts to break down features of human language for the purpose of applying Darwinian gradualism. Although an influence on early animal language efforts (see below), it is no longer considered the key architecture at the core of animal language research.[ citation needed ]

Clever Hans, an Orlov Trotter horse that was claimed to have been able to perform arithmetic and other intellectual tasks CleverHans.jpg
Clever Hans, an Orlov Trotter horse that was claimed to have been able to perform arithmetic and other intellectual tasks

Animal language results are controversial for several reasons (for a related controversy, see also Clever Hans). Early chimpanzee work was executed using chimpanzee infants raised as if they were human; a test of the nature vs. nurture hypothesis.[ citation needed ] Chimpanzees have a laryngeal structure very different from that of humans, and it has been suggested that chimpanzees are not capable of voluntary control of their breathing, although better studies are needed to accurately confirm this. This combination is thought to make it very difficult for the chimpanzees to reproduce the vocal intonations required for human language. Researchers eventually moved towards a gestural (sign language) modality, as well as keyboard devices with buttons with symbols (known as "lexigrams") that the animals could press to produce artificial language. Other chimpanzees learned by observing human subjects performing the task.[ citation needed ] This latter group of researchers studying chimpanzee communication through symbol recognition (keyboard) as well as through the use of sign language (gestural), are on the forefront of communicative breakthroughs in the study of animal language, and they are familiar with their subjects on a first name basis: Sarah, Lana, Kanzi, Koko, Sherman, Austin and Chantek.[ citation needed ]

Perhaps the best known critic of animal language is Herbert Terrace. Terrace's 1979 criticism using his own research with the chimpanzee Nim Chimpsky [48] [49] was scathing and spelled the end of animal language research in that era, most of which emphasized the production of language by animals. In short, he accused researchers of over-interpreting their results, especially as it is rarely parsimonious to ascribe true intentional "language production" when other simpler explanations for the behaviors (gestural hand signs) could be put forth. Additionally, his animals failed to show generalization of the concept of reference between the modalities of comprehension and production; this generalization is one of many fundamental ones that are trivial for human language use. The simpler explanation according to Terrace was that the animals had learned a sophisticated series of context-based behavioral strategies to obtain either primary (food) or social reinforcement, behaviors that could be over-interpreted as language use.

In 1984 Louis Herman published an account of artificial language found in the bottlenose dolphin in the journal Cognition . [50] A major difference between Herman's work and previous research was his emphasis on a method of studying language comprehension only (rather than language comprehension and production by the animal(s)), which enabled rigorous controls and statistical tests, largely because he was limiting his researchers to evaluating the animals' physical behaviors (in response to sentences) with blinded observers, rather than attempting to interpret possible language utterances or productions. The dolphins' names here were Akeakamai and Phoenix. [50] Irene Pepperberg used the vocal modality for language production and comprehension in a grey parrot named Alex in the verbal mode, [51] [52] [53] [54] and Sue Savage-Rumbaugh continues to study bonobos [55] [56] such as Kanzi and Panbanisha. R. Schusterman duplicated many of the dolphin results in his California sea lions ("Rocky"), and came from a more behaviorist tradition than Herman's cognitive approach. Schusterman's emphasis is on the importance on a learning structure known as equivalence classes. [57] [58]

However, overall, there has not been any meaningful dialog between the linguistics and animal language spheres, despite capturing the public's imagination in the popular press. Furthermore, the growing field of language evolution is another source of future interchange between these disciplines. Most primate researchers tend to show a bias toward a shared pre-linguistic ability between humans and chimpanzees, dating back to a common ancestor, while dolphin and parrot researchers stress the general cognitive principles underlying these abilities. More recent related controversies regarding animal abilities include the closely linked areas of theory of mind, Imitation (e.g. Nehaniv & Dautenhahn, 2002), [59] Animal Culture (e.g. Rendell & Whitehead, 2001), [60] and Language Evolution (e.g. Christiansen & Kirby, 2003). [61]

There has been a recent emergence in animal language research which has contested the idea that animal communication is less sophisticated than human communication. Denise Herzing has done research on dolphins in the Bahamas whereby she created a two-way conversation via a submerged keyboard. [62] The keyboard allows divers to communicate with wild dolphins. By using sounds and symbols on each key the dolphins could either press the key with their nose or mimic the whistling sound emitted in order to ask humans for a specific prop. This ongoing experiment has shown that in non-linguistic creatures sophisticated and rapid thinking does occur despite our previous conceptions of animal communication. Further research done with Kanzi using lexigrams has strengthened the idea that animal communication is much more complex than once thought. [63]

See also

Researchers

Animals

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Washoe (chimpanzee)</span> Chimpanzee research subject

Washoe was a female common chimpanzee who was the first non-human to learn to communicate using American Sign Language (ASL) as part of an animal research experiment on animal language acquisition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cetacean intelligence</span> Intellectual capacity of cetaceans

Cetacean intelligence is the overall intelligence and derived cognitive ability of aquatic mammals belonging in the infraorder Cetacea (cetaceans), including baleen whales, porpoises, and dolphins. In 2014, a study found for first time that the long-finned pilot whale has more neocortical neurons than any other mammal, including humans, examined to date.

Yerkish is an artificial language developed for use by non-human primates. It employs a keyboard whose keys contain lexigrams, symbols corresponding to objects or ideas.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal cognition</span> Intelligence of non-human animals

Animal cognition encompasses the mental capacities of non-human animals including insect cognition. The study of animal conditioning and learning used in this field was developed from comparative psychology. It has also been strongly influenced by research in ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology; the alternative name cognitive ethology is sometimes used. Many behaviors associated with the term animal intelligence are also subsumed within animal cognition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Kanzi</span> Bonobo research subject

Kanzi, also known by the lexigram , is a male bonobo who has been the subject of several studies on great ape language. According to Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, a primatologist who has studied the bonobo throughout his life, Kanzi has exhibited advanced linguistic aptitude.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Great ape language</span> Efforts to teach non-human primates to communicate with humans

Research into great ape language has involved teaching chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas and orangutans to communicate with humans and each other using sign language, physical tokens, lexigrams, and imitative human speech. Some primatologists argue that the use of these communication methods indicate primate "language" ability, though this depends on one's definition of language. The cognitive tradeoff hypothesis suggests that human language skills evolved at the expense of the short-term and working memory capabilities observed in other hominids.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mirror test</span> Animal self-awareness test to determine self-recognition in a mirror

The mirror test—sometimes called the mark test, mirror self-recognition (MSR) test, red spot technique, or rouge test—is a behavioral technique developed in 1970 by American psychologist Gordon Gallup Jr. as an attempt to determine whether an animal possesses the ability of visual self-recognition. The MSR test is the traditional method for attempting to measure physiological and cognitive self-awareness. However, agreement has been reached that animals can be self-aware in ways not measured by the mirror test, such as distinguishing between their own and others' songs and scents, and being aware of their own bodies, while humans have abnormally good vision, and thus intelligence that is highly visual.

The Mind of an Ape is a 1983 book by David Premack and his wife Ann James Premack. The authors argue that it is possible to teach language to (non-human) great apes. They write: "We now know that someone who comprehends speech must know language, even if he or she cannot produce it."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human–animal communication</span> Verbal and non-verbal interspecies communication

Human–animal communication is the communication observed between humans and other animals, ranging from non-verbal cues and vocalizations to the use of language.

Animal culture can be defined as the ability of non-human animals to learn and transmit behaviors through processes of social or cultural learning. Culture is increasingly seen as a process, involving the social transmittance of behavior among peers and between generations. It can involve the transmission of novel behaviors or regional variations that are independent of genetic or ecological factors.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Elephant cognition</span> Intelligence and awareness in elephants

Elephant cognition is animal cognition as present in elephants. Most contemporary ethologists view the elephant as one of the world's most intelligent animals. With a mass of just over 5 kg (11 lb), an elephant's brain has more mass than that of any other land animal, and although the largest whales have body masses twenty times those of a typical elephant, a whale's brain is barely twice the mass of an elephant's brain. In addition, elephants have around 257 billion neurons. Elephant brains are similar to those of humans and many other mammals in terms of general connectivity and functional areas, with several unique structural differences. Although initially estimated to have as many neurons as a human brain, the elephant's brain has about three times the amount of neurons as a human brain. However, the elephant's cerebral cortex has about one-third of the number of neurons as a human's cerebral cortex.

Vocal learning is the ability to modify acoustic and syntactic sounds, acquire new sounds via imitation, and produce vocalizations. "Vocalizations" in this case refers only to sounds generated by the vocal organ as opposed to by the lips, teeth, and tongue, which require substantially less motor control. A rare trait, vocal learning is a critical substrate for spoken language and has only been detected in eight animal groups despite the wide array of vocalizing species; these include humans, bats, cetaceans, pinnipeds, elephants, and three distantly related bird groups including songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. Vocal learning is distinct from auditory learning, or the ability to form memories of sounds heard, a relatively common trait which is present in all vertebrates tested. For example, dogs can be trained to understand the word "sit" even though the human word is not in its innate auditory repertoire. However, the dog cannot imitate and produce the word "sit" itself as vocal learners can.

A talking animal or speaking animal is any non-human animal that can produce sounds or gestures resembling those of a human language. Several species or groups of animals have developed forms of communication which superficially resemble verbal language, however, these usually are not considered a language because they lack one or more of the defining characteristics, e.g. grammar, syntax, recursion, and displacement. Researchers have been successful in teaching some animals to make gestures similar to sign language, although whether this should be considered a language has been disputed.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Primate cognition</span> Study of non-human primate intellect

Primate cognition is the study of the intellectual and behavioral skills of non-human primates, particularly in the fields of psychology, behavioral biology, primatology, and anthropology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Laughter in animals</span> Overview of humor in animals

Laughter in animals other than humans describes animal behavior which resembles human laughter.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Panbanisha</span> Bonobo research subject

Panbanisha, also known by the lexigram , was a female bonobo that featured in studies on great ape language by Professor Sue Savage-Rumbaugh. Her name is Swahili for "to cleave together for the purpose of contrast."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cooperative pulling paradigm</span> Experimental design

The cooperative pulling paradigm is an experimental design in which two or more animals pull rewards toward themselves via an apparatus that they cannot successfully operate alone. Researchers use cooperative pulling experiments to try to understand how cooperation works and how and when it may have evolved.

Panpanzee the chimpanzee, was born at the Language Research Center at Georgia State University in Atlanta, Georgia. She lived the rest of her life at the Great Ape Trust in Iowa. Her half-brother is Kanzi, a famous bonobo. Kanzi learned 348 lexigram symbols and over 3,000 words from the English language over her lifespan.

References

  1. Shah, Sonia (20 September 2023). "The Animals Are Talking. What Does It Mean? – Language was long understood as a human-only affair. New research suggests that isn't so. + comment". The New York Times . Archived from the original on 21 September 2023. Retrieved 21 September 2023.
  2. Yamamoto, Hare & (2016). Bonobos. Oxford University Press. ISBN   978-0-19-872851-1.
  3. Lyn, Heidi; Greenfield, Patricia M.; Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue (2011). "Semiotic combinations in Pan: A comparison of communication in a chimpanzee and two bonobos". First Language. 31 (3): 300–325. doi:10.1177/0142723710391872. ISSN   0142-7237.
  4. Hockett, Charles F. (1960). "Logical considerations in the study of animal communication". In Lanyon, W.E.; Tavolga, W.N. (eds.). Animals sounds and animal communication. American Institute of Biological Sciences. pp. 392–430.
  5. Martinelli, Dario (2010). "Introduction to Zoosemiotics". A Critical Companion to Zoosemiotics: People, Paths, Ideas. Biosemiotics. Vol. 5. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. pp. 1–64. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-9249-6_1. ISBN   978-90-481-9249-6.
  6. Hauser, Marc D.; Chomsky, Noam; Fitch, W. Tecumseh (22 November 2002). "The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve?" (PDF). Science. American Association for the Advancement of Science. pp. 1569–1579. Archived from the original (PDF) on 28 December 2013. Retrieved 28 March 2014. We argue that an understanding of the faculty of language requires substantial interdisciplinary cooperation. We suggest how current developments in linguistics can be profitably wedded to work in evolutionary biology, anthropology, psychology, and neuroscience. We submit that a distinction should be made between the faculty of language in the broad sense (FLB) and in the narrow sense (FLN). FLB includes a sensory-motor system, a conceptual-intentional system, and the computational mechanisms for recursion, providing the capacity to generate an infinite range of expressions from a finite set of elements. We hypothesize that FLN only includes recursion and is the only uniquely human component of the faculty of language. We further argue that FLN may have evolved for reasons other than language, hence comparative studies might look for evidence of such computations outside of the domain of communication (for example, number, navigation, and social relations).
  7. Denham, Kristin; Lobeck, Anne (2010). Linguistics for Everyone: An Introduction (Instructor's ed.). Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. pp. 4–5. ISBN   9781428205833.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Fitch, WT. (Feb 2011). "Unity and diversity in human language". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366 (1563): 376–88. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0223. PMC   3013471 . PMID   21199842.
  9. Patterson, Francine; Linden, Eugene (1981). The education of Kok. New York: Holt. ISBN   978-0-03-046101-9. OCLC   7283799.
  10. Gardner, R. A.; Gardner, B. T. (1969). "Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee". Science. 165 (3894): 664–672. Bibcode:1969Sci...165..664G. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.384.4164 . doi:10.1126/science.165.3894.664. ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   5793972.
  11. Gardner, B.T.; Gardner, R.A. (1975). "Evidence for sentence constituents in the early utterances of child and chimpanzee". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 104 (3): 244–267. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.244.
  12. Fernández, Eva M.; Cairns, Helen Smith (2011). Fundamentals of psycholinguistic. Chichester, West Sussex [England] ; Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN   978-1-4051-9147-0. OCLC   667883441.
  13. Crockford, Catherine; Wittig, Roman M.; Mundry, Roger; Zuberbühler, Klaus (January 2012). "Wild Chimpanzees Inform Ignorant Group Members of Danger". Current Biology. 22 (2): 142–146. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053. hdl: 10023/4314 . ISSN   0960-9822. PMID   22209531. S2CID   3257708.
  14. Raffaele, P (November 2006). "Speaking Bonobo". Smithsonian . Retrieved 2008-03-18.
  15. Maurer, D.; Pathman, T.; Mondloch, CJ. (May 2006). "The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults". Dev Sci. 9 (3): 316–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x . PMID   16669803. S2CID   7297731.
  16. Kubovy, M.; Yu, M. (Apr 2012). "Multistability, cross-modal binding and the additivity of conjoined grouping principles". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 367 (1591): 954–64. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0365. PMC   3282311 . PMID   22371617.
  17. Miyagawa, Shigeru (2013). "The emergence of hierarchical structure in human language". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 71. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00071 . PMC   3577014 . PMID   23431042.
  18. 1 2 Pinker, Steven (2000). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. HarperCollins. ISBN   978-0-06-095833-6.
  19. Cobra Master (2003-12-12). "Introduction". Cobras.org. Retrieved 2013-05-19.
  20. "Skunks, Skunk Pictures, Skunk Facts – National Geographic". Animals.nationalgeographic.com. 2013-05-15. Archived from the original on January 14, 2010. Retrieved 2013-05-19.
  21. Almaas, Torj.; Christensen, Thomasa.; Mustaparta, Hanna (1991-09-01). "Chemical communication in heliothine moths". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 169 (3). doi:10.1007/BF00206989. S2CID   20567271.
  22. Haghighat, Leila (2012). "Baboons Can Learn to Recognize Words". Nature News. doi: 10.1038/nature.2012.10432 . S2CID   178872255 . Retrieved 15 April 2013.
  23. Fitch, W. T.; de Boer, B.; Mathur, N.; Ghazanfar, A. A. (December 2016). "Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready". Science Advances . 2 (12): e1600723. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E0723F. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600723. PMC   5148209 . PMID   27957536.
  24. "Why can't monkeys talk? Their anatomy is 'speech-ready' but their brains aren't wired for it: neuroscientist". National Post . Retrieved 10 December 2016.
  25. 1 2 Dong, Shihao; Lin, Tao; Nieh, James C.; Tan, Ken (2023-03-10). "Social signal learning of the waggle dance in honey bees". Science. 379 (6636): 1015–1018. doi:10.1126/science.ade1702. ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   36893231. S2CID   257430009.
  26. 1 2 "The Idea". Elephant Listening Project. Cornell University. Archived from the original on 2023-01-28. Retrieved 28 December 2013.
  27. "The Secret Language of Elephants". CBS News 60 Minutes. Retrieved 2013-02-24.
  28. Kanwal, J. S.; Matsumura, S.; Ohlemiller, K.; Suga, N. (1994). "Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 94 (3): 1229–1254. Bibcode:1994ASAJ...96.1229K. doi:10.1121/1.410273. PMID   7962992.
  29. "Con Slobodchikoff". nau.edu. Retrieved 10 December 2016.
  30. "Dolphins' Secret Language". Young People's Trust for the Environment. 29 February 2012. Archived from the original on 27 August 2012.
  31. "The Secret Language of Dolphins". National Geographic Kids. Archived from the original on 2013-03-08. Retrieved 2013-03-31.
  32. 1 2 "NOVA scienceNOW: Smart Marine Mammals – Smart Sea Lions". teachersdomain.org. Retrieved 10 December 2016.
  33. http://www.pinnipedlab.org/ Archived 2012-01-18 at the Wayback Machine The Pinniped Cognition & Sensory Systems Laboratory
  34. 1 2 3 Gisiner, Robert; Schusterman, Ronald J. (1992). "Sequence, syntax, and semantics: Responses of a language-trained sea lion (Zalophus californianus) to novel sign combinations" (PDF). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 106 (1): 78–91. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.106.1.78. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-11-19. Retrieved 2013-12-28.
  35. "California Sea Lion – Communication". seaworld.org. Retrieved 2013-05-19.
  36. "Sea Lion Info – Dolphin Research Center". dolphins.org. Retrieved 10 December 2016.
  37. Janik, VM.; Slater, PJ. (Jul 2000). "The different roles of social learning in vocal communication". Anim Behav. 60 (1): 1–11. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1410. PMID   10924198. S2CID   1839031.
  38. Schusterman, RJ.; Dawson, RG. (Apr 1968). "Barking, dominance, and territoriality in male sea lions". Science. 160 (3826): 434–6. Bibcode:1968Sci...160..434S. doi:10.1126/science.160.3826.434. PMID   5689412. S2CID   28586877.
  39. Shapiro, AD.; Slater, PJ.; Janik, VM. (Dec 2004). "Call usage learning in gray seals (Halichoerus grypus)". J Comp Psychol. 118 (4): 447–54. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.447. PMID   15584781.
  40. "Oceanography". Onr.navy.mil. Archived from the original on 2013-12-02. Retrieved 2013-05-19.
  41. Cloney, RA; Florey, E (1968). "Ultrastructure of cephalopod chromatophore organs". Zeitschrift für Zellforschung und Mikroskopische Anatomie. 89 (2): 250–80. doi:10.1007/BF00347297. PMID   5700268. S2CID   26566732.
  42. "Sepioteuthis sepioidea; Caribbean Reef squid". The Cephalopod Page. Retrieved 2013-05-19.
  43. Byrne, R.A., U. Griebel, J.B. Wood & J.A. Mather 2003. "Squids say it with skin: a graphic model for skin displays in Caribbean Reef Squid" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-07-04. (3.86 MB)Berliner Geowissenschaftliche Abhandlungen3: 29–35.
  44. "Pauses during communication release behavioral habituation through recovery from synaptic depression: Current Biology".
  45. "Extreme Enlargement of the Cerebellum in a Clade of Teleost Fishes that Evolved a Novel Active Sensory System: Current Biology".
  46. "Blind Fish Have Regional Accents Just Like Humans". Nerdist.
  47. Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D.L. (1990). "The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and other species' alarm calls". Animal Behaviour. 40 (4): 754–764. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80704-3. S2CID   33689834.
  48. Terrace, Herbert S. (1979). Nim. New York: Knopf : distributed by Random House. ISBN   978-0-394-40250-5. OCLC   5102119.
  49. Terrace, H.S.; Petitto, L.A.; Sanders, R.J.; Bever, T.G. (1979). "Can an ape create a sentence?". Science. 206 (4421): 891–902. Bibcode:1979Sci...206..891T. doi:10.1126/science.504995. PMID   504995. S2CID   7517074.
  50. 1 2 Herman, L. M.; Richards, D. G.; Wolz, J. P. (1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins". Cognition. 16 (2): 129–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9. PMID   6540652. S2CID   43237011.
  51. Pepperberg, Irene M. (1999). The Alex studies : cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrot. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. ISBN   978-0-674-00051-3. OCLC   807730081.
  52. Pepperberg, IM. (Oct 2010). "Vocal learning in Grey parrots: A brief review of perception, production, and cross-species comparisons". Brain Lang. 115 (1): 81–91. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.002. PMID   20199805. S2CID   1744169.
  53. Pepperberg, IM.; Carey, S. (Nov 2012). "Grey parrot number acquisition: the inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list". Cognition. 125 (2): 219–32. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.003. PMC   3434310 . PMID   22878117.
  54. Pepperberg, IM. (Feb 2013). "Abstract concepts: data from a Grey parrot". Behav Processes. 93: 82–90. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2012.09.016. PMID   23089384. S2CID   33278680.
  55. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). "Language Acquisition in a Nonhuman Species: Implications for the innateness debate". Developmental Psychobiology . 23 (7): 599–620. doi:10.1002/dev.420230706. PMID   2286294.
  56. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S.; Fields, W. M. (2000). "Linguistic, cultural and cognitive capacities of bonobos (Pan paniscus)". Culture and Psychology. 6 (2): 131–154. doi:10.1177/1354067X0062003. S2CID   145714904.
  57. Schusterman, RJ.; Kastak, D. (May 1998). "Functional equivalence in a California sea lion: relevance to animal social and communicative interactions". Anim Behav. 55 (5): 1087–95. doi:10.1006/anbe.1997.0654. PMID   9632496. S2CID   25316126.
  58. Kastak, CR.; Schusterman, RJ.; Kastak, D. (Sep 2001). "Equivalence classification by California sea lions using class-specific reinforcers". J Exp Anal Behav. 76 (2): 131–58. doi:10.1901/jeab.2001.76-131. PMC   1284831 . PMID   11599636.
  59. Nehaniv, Chrystopher; Dautenhahn, Kerstin (2002). Imitation in animals and artifacts. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN   9780262271219. OCLC   51938434.
  60. Rendell, L.; Whitehead, H. (2001). "Culture in whales and dolphins". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 309–382. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0100396X. PMID   11530544. S2CID   24052064.
  61. Christiansen, Morten H.; Kirby, Simon (2003). Language evolution. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN   978-0-19-924484-3. OCLC   51235137.
  62. Herzing, Denise L.; Delfour, Fabienne; Pack, Adam A. (2012). "Responses of human-habituated wild Atlantic Spotted Dolphins to play behaviours using a two-way human/dolphin interface" (PDF). International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 25 (2): 137–165. doi:10.46867/IJCP.2012.25.02.02.
  63. Savage-Rumbaugh, S.; Rumbaugh, D.; Fields, W. "Empirical kanzi: The ape language controversy revisited. (2009)". Skeptic. 15 (1): 25–33. Archived from the original on 2013-05-18.

Further reading