English relative words

Last updated

The English relative words are words in English used to mark a clause, noun phrase or preposition phrase as relative. The central relative words in English include who, whom, whose, which, why, and while, as shown in the following examples, each of which has the relative clause in bold:

Contents

Most also belong to the set of English interrogative words but function differently as relative words.

The subordinator that is widely regarded as a relative word, though one with different properties from the others. [lower-alpha 1]

Semantics

Semantically speaking, relative words typically refer to some antecedent in the containing phrase or clause. For example, who within the teacher of mine who likes apples does not question the identity of a person, but rather refers to "the teacher of mine", which in turn denotes some particular person.

Individual words

All of the words may have singular or plural antecedents. For example, the customer who was cheated/the customers who were cheated.

That differs from the other relative words in that, like other subordinators, it lacks semantic content, referring or otherwise. [2] :955,990

Lexical categories and syntactic functions

Each relative word also has a syntactic function in a phrase or clause. For example, in the person who arrived, who functions as the subject of the relative clause. Different words have different functions depending on their lexical category and form. For example, while a plain [lower-alpha 4] pronoun like who may typically function as a subject or object, its genitive form functions only as a determiner (e.g., the person [whose keys] I found).

Syntax

A relative word occurs within a relative phrase, which appears in clause-initial position. A simple relative phrase consists of a relative word by itself (where within the restaurant where we dined; who within the man who you introduced me to). A complex relative phrase also has other material; it is exemplified by to whom within the man to whom you introduced me, from under which within the rock from under which it had crawled, and whose car within the man whose car you borrowed. [2] :1039

Individual words in relative clauses


Fused relatives

A fused construction is one in which a word or phrase fuses has two functions at once. A simple type of fused construction (not a relative) is exemplified by any within I didn't notice any: Although a noun phrase (such as any food) is normally headed by a noun, and although any is normally (as in any food) a dependent, within I didn't notice any it heads a noun phrase and thus functions as a fused dependent-head. [2] :56

Similarly, a fused relative is a noun phrase or preposition phrase (not a clause, but containing one) [2] :1068–1070 that is headed by a relative phrase (most commonly by a simple relative phrase, and thus by a relative word alone), and that lacks an antecedent. [2] :63,1035–1036,1068 For example, the fused relative construction who you want within Believe who you want contains the relative phrase who. This has functions within both the NP that contains the relative clause and within the relative clause itself: functions that are fused. [2] :1073

The fused relative is also called a free relative, [19] :417,431 free relative clause, [15] :200–202 [lower-alpha 6] nominal relative clause, and independent relative clause. [21] :165

Fused relatives with relative words without -ever

A fused relative may be headed by a non-compound word, such as what, by a where+preposition compound, or by a compound with -ever. The three kinds are considered in turn.

Individual non-compound words in fused relatives

  • Who, as in You can believe who you want. But this is archaic with who as the subject in the relative clause: Who steals my purse steals trash was used by Shakespeare, and survives in the expression Can I help who's next? but *You can believe who told you is not used today. [22]
  • Whom, as in You can believe whom you want.
  • Whose, as in You can believe whose version you want.
  • Which, as in You can believe which version you want. Like who, this is not used as the subject in the relative clause: *Use which is handy.
  • When, as in We arrived when it opened.
  • Where, as in My phone wasn't where I'd left it.
  • While, as in We arrived while the band was playing.
  • Whence (rather archaic), as in He returned whence he had come.
  • Whither (distinctly archaic), as in He goes whither he wants.
  • Why, as in Why I resigned was because of the chairman's intransigence. Use in fused relatives of why seems to be limited to pseudo-clefts. [2] :1077n
  • What, as in They ate what was offered. Examples with what as a determiner, such as They ate what food was offered and They considered what options were open to them, imply a small quantity or number. [2] :1077
  • How, as in You can dress how you like.

Individual where+preposition words in fused relatives

  • Whereof is widely used, particularly with the verb speak (The mayor, 35, knows whereof she speaks; [23] perhaps best known from the final proposition within Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus in its first English translation: Whereof one cannot speak, thereof one must be silent [24] ).

Fused relatives with -ever relative words

Individual -ever words in fused relatives

  • Whoever, as in I cursed whoever had taken it.
  • Whomever, as in I borrow from whomever I can (although this would be a less likely alternative to I borrow from whoever I can).
  • Whoever's, whosever, as in The gang will steal whoever's/whosever car they can. [2] :1075n
  • Whichever, as in Buy whichever you like; Buy whichever vase/vases you like. [lower-alpha 7]
  • Whenever, as in We go whenever we can.
  • Wherever, as in There were ants wherever we looked.
  • Whatever, as in Buy whatever you like; Buy whatever vase/furniture you like.
  • However, as in However it happened, fix it; However unfair it seems, fix it.

-Soever and -so relative words

As relative words, forms ending -soever and -so are old-fashioned variants of the -ever forms. [2] :356n, 1074n There are whoso(ever), whomso(ever), whichsoever, whensoever and whatso(ever); and the archaisms whencesoever and whithersoever are still occasionally found.

Etymology

Ultimately, the English interrogative words (those beginning with wh in addition to the word how), derive from the Proto-Indo-European root kwo- or kwi, [25] the former of which was reflected in Proto-Germanic as χwa- or khwa-, due to Grimm's law. [26]

These underwent further sound changes and spelling changes, notably wh-cluster reductions, resulting in the initial sound being either /w/ (in most dialects) or /h/ (how, who) and the initial spelling being either wh or h (how). This was the result of two sound changes – /hw/ > /h/ before /uː/ (how, who) and /hw/ > /w/ otherwise – and the spelling change from hw to wh in Middle English. The unusual pronunciation versus spelling of who is because the vowel was formerly /aː/, and thus it did not undergo the sound change in Old English, but in Middle English (following spelling change) the vowel changed to /uː/ and it followed the same sound change as how before it, but with the Middle English spelling unchanged. [26]

In how (Old English , from Proto-Germanic χwō), the w merged into the lave of the word, as it did in Old Frisian hū, hō (Dutch hoe "how"), but it can still be seen in Old Saxon hwō, Old High German hwuo (German wie "how"). In English, the gradual change of voiceless stops into voiceless fricatives (phase 1 of Grimm's law) during the development of Germanic languages is responsible for "wh-" of interrogatives. Although some varieties of American English and various Scottish dialects still preserve the original sound (i.e. [ʍ] rather than [w]), most have only the [w]. [26]

The words who, whom, whose, what and why, can all be considered to come from a single Old English word hwā, reflecting its masculine and feminine nominative (hwā), dative (hwām), genitive (hwæs), neuter nominative and accusative (hwæt), and instrumental (masculine and neuter singular) (hwȳ, later hwī) respectively. [27] Other interrogative words, such as which, how, where, whence, or whither, [28] derive either from compounds (which coming from a compound of hwā [what, who] and līc [like]), [29] or other words from the same root (how deriving from ). [26]

Phonology

Pronunciation of initial digraphs

The pronunciation of English relative words starting with the ⟨wh⟩ digraph involves a phonetic element historically pronounced as /hw/ and now variously realized as /w/ or /ʍ/. [30] :14 Speakers with the whine-wine merger generally use /w/, resulting in words like which, and why being pronounced with an initial /w/ sound, homophonous with witch, and wye. The /hw/ pronunciation is preserved in conservative speech in the Southern United States, [31] in certain Scottish English varieties, [32] and elsewhere. However, the merged /w/ pronunciation has been identified as having a continuous lineage in everyday spoken Southern English from Old English to the present. [30] :16 Three factors have been highlighted in enabling this phonetic evolution: spelling, word frequency, and possibly a shift in the sociolinguistic status of the northern pronunciation in some circles in the south during the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries. [30]

The initial ⟨th⟩ in that represents a voiced dental fricative /ð/, phonologically distinguishing it from other relative words.

Vowel sounds

Outside of fused relatives, relative words have unstressed pronunciation. The vowel sounds in English relative words vary, with some notable features:

  1. Diphthongs and monophthongs: Words like who and whose contain a monophthong /uː/, [26] while others like why incorporate a diphthong /aɪ/.
  2. Schwa and reduction: The vowels in some relative words like which can reduce to a schwa, /ə/. [33] :263 As a relative word, the subordinator that has only the unstressed pronunciation /ðət/.

Intonational phrasing

Supplementary relative clauses regularly form a separate prosodic unit, with a pause before the relative phrase, while integrated relatives do not. [33] :280

Relative vs interrogative and other words

There is significant overlap between the English relative words and the English interrogative words, but the relative words that and while are not interrogative words, the interrogative words whether and if are not relative words, and, in Standard English, what and how are mostly excluded from the relative words. [2] :1053 [lower-alpha 1] Most or all of the interrogative words that are now more or less archaic are also relative words. [2] :1046

The denotation of whose as an interrogative word is limited to persons, but the relative whose may denote non-persons, as in a book whose cover is missing. [2] :1049

Fused relatives are easily confused with open interrogatives, and even a careful analysis may conclude that, if taken out of context, a particular sentence can have either of two interpretations. An example in The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language illustrating this ambiguity is What she wrote is completely unclear. If we know what she wrote and are saying that it is hard to understand, then what she wrote is a fused relative; if on the other hand we are saying that the extent of her authorship is unknown, then what she wrote is an open interrogative content clause. [2] :1070

The preposition while also has other, relative-irrelevant uses: While she showered, I slept (time), While the maths exam was tough, the English exam was easy (contrast), While you're free to complain, doing so won't get you anywhere (concession). [15]

As a relative word, the subordinator that has only the unstressed pronunciation /ðət/. It is also used more generally with subordinate clauses (I know that he's lying) and is usually unstressed, but in some contexts necessarily stressed /ˈðæt/ (That he's lying is obvious). The stressed that that has the plural form those (I'll take that) is a determiner. [15] :61 [1] :1046 [lower-alpha 8]

The use in fused relatives of the -ever form should not be confused with its other, non-relative uses:

Notes

  1. 1 2 3 Huddleston et al do not consider that to be a relative word: "In [the sentence The video that I needed is unobtainable], there is no relative word: that is a subordinator in marker function." [1] :279
  2. This article uses asterisks to indicate ungrammatical examples.
  3. The examples below were found via the Corpus of News on the Web (NOW). [3] This list does not exhaust the possibilities. Concatenating yet other prepositions to where and looking up these compounds in the corpora show the recent use of several. However, these tend to be limited to legal use and rare even there. As an example, NOW currently (November 2023) has but a single token of relative wherefor (among numerous tokens of typo-damaged where for, irrelevant to relatives): the eligibility clause in relation to an exemption notification is given strict meaning wherefor the notification has to be interpreted in terms of its language. [4]
  4. 1 2 In contexts permitting inflectional distinctions among the three cases (nominative, accusative and genitive), "The plain case represents a neutralisation of the nominative–accusative opposition". [2] :458 Thus you, it, what, and (other than in formal contexts or deliberate speech/writing) who are in the plain case.
  5. While the result may be non-standard, such omission is rather common, writes Ellen Prince, [20] :247 as cited by James McCawley. [19] :451
  6. The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language provides six arguments for regarding these as phrases; [2] :1068–1070 perhaps because of limited space in his much smaller book, Aarts' argument for the reverse is very terse. [15] :201
  7. For the latter use, the head of which whichever is a dependent must be a count noun. With a non-count noun, as in *Buy whichever furniture you like, the result would be ungrammatical, or at best require coercion of the head noun. [2] :398
  8. Both Huddleston et al and Aarts use the term determinative for the word category that we call "determiner".
  9. Although ever is conventionally separated by a space, "actual usage" often attaches it. [2] :916n

Related Research Articles

In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase.

English grammar is the set of structural rules of the English language. This includes the structure of words, phrases, clauses, sentences, and whole texts.

The Finnish language is spoken by the majority of the population in Finland and by ethnic Finns elsewhere. Unlike the languages spoken in neighbouring countries, such as Swedish and Norwegian, which are North Germanic languages, or Russian, which is a Slavic language, Finnish is a Uralic language of the Finnic languages group. Typologically, Finnish is agglutinative. As in some other Uralic languages, Finnish has vowel harmony, and like other Finnic languages, it has consonant gradation.

The morphology of Irish is in some respects typical of an Indo-European language. Nouns are declined for number and case, and verbs for person and number. Nouns are classified by masculine or feminine gender. Other aspects of Irish morphology, while typical for an Insular Celtic language, are not typical for Indo-European, such as the presence of inflected prepositions and the initial consonant mutations. Irish syntax is also rather different from that of most Indo-European languages, due to its use of the verb–subject–object word order.

<i>Who</i> (pronoun) English pronoun

The pronoun who, in English, is an interrogative pronoun and a relative pronoun, used primarily to refer to persons.

An interrogative word or question word is a function word used to ask a question, such as what, which, when, where, who, whom, whose, why, whether and how. They are sometimes called wh-words, because in English most of them start with wh-. They may be used in both direct questions and in indirect questions. In English and various other languages the same forms are also used as relative pronouns in certain relative clauses and certain adverb clauses. It can also be used as a modal, since question words are more likely to appear in modal sentences, like

In linguistics, wh-movement is the formation of syntactic dependencies involving interrogative words. An example in English is the dependency formed between what and the object position of doing in "What are you doing?" Interrogative forms are sometimes known within English linguistics as wh-words, such as what, when, where, who, and why, but also include other interrogative words, such as how. This dependency has been used as a diagnostic tool in syntactic studies as it can be observed to interact with other grammatical constraints.

In linguistics, pied-piping is a phenomenon of syntax whereby a given focused expression brings along an encompassing phrase with it when it is moved.

A relative pronoun is a pronoun that marks a relative clause. An example is the word which in the sentence "This is the house which Jack built." Here the relative pronoun which introduces the relative clause, which modifies the noun house. A relative pronoun plays the role of a noun phrase within that clause. For example, in the relative clause "which Jack built," "which" is a pronoun functioning as the object of the verb "built."

Relative clauses in the English language are formed principally by means of relative words. The basic relative pronouns are who, which, and that; who also has the derived forms whom and whose. Various grammatical rules and style guides determine which relative pronouns may be suitable in various situations, especially for formal settings. In some cases the relative pronoun may be omitted and merely implied.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Spanish pronouns</span>

Spanish pronouns in some ways work quite differently from their English counterparts. Subject pronouns are often omitted, and object pronouns come in clitic and non-clitic forms. When used as clitics, object pronouns can appear as proclitics that come before the verb or as enclitics attached to the end of the verb in different linguistic environments. There is also regional variation in the use of pronouns, particularly the use of the informal second-person singular vos and the informal second-person plural vosotros.

German pronouns are German words that function as pronouns. As with pronouns in other languages, they are frequently employed as the subject or object of a clause, acting as substitutes for nouns or noun phrases, but are also used in relative clauses to relate the main clause to a subordinate one.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English prepositions</span> Prepositions in the English language

English prepositions are words – such as of, in, on, at, from, etc. – that function as the head of a prepositional phrase, and most characteristically license a noun phrase object. Semantically, they most typically denote relations in space and time. Morphologically, they are usually simple and do not inflect. They form a closed lexical category.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English pronouns</span> Category of words in English that prototypically "stand in" for other noun phrases

The English pronouns form a relatively small category of words in Modern English whose primary semantic function is that of a pro-form for a noun phrase. Traditional grammars consider them to be a distinct part of speech, while most modern grammars see them as a subcategory of noun, contrasting with common and proper nouns. Still others see them as a subcategory of determiner. In this article, they are treated as a subtype of the noun category.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English adverbs</span>

English adverbs are words such as so, just, how, well, also, very, even, only, really, and why that head adverb phrases, and whose most typical members function as modifiers in verb phrases and clauses, along with adjective and adverb phrases. The category is highly heterogeneous, but a large number of the very typical members are derived from adjectives + the suffix -ly and modify any word, phrase or clause other than a noun. Adverbs form an open lexical category in English. They do not typically license or function as complements in other phrases. Semantically, they are again highly various, denoting manner, degree, duration, frequency, domain, modality, and much more.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English possessive</span> Possessive words and phrases in the English language

In English, possessive words or phrases exist for nouns and most pronouns, as well as some noun phrases. These can play the roles of determiners or of nouns.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English clause syntax</span> Clauses in English grammar

This article describes the syntax of clauses in the English language, chiefly in Modern English. A clause is often said to be the smallest grammatical unit that can express a complete proposition. But this semantic idea of a clause leaves out much of English clause syntax. For example, clauses can be questions, but questions are not propositions. A syntactic description of an English clause is that it is a subject and a verb. But this too fails, as a clause need not have a subject, as with the imperative, and, in many theories, an English clause may be verbless. The idea of what qualifies varies between theories and has changed over time.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English adjectives</span> Adjectives in the English language

English adjectives form a large open category of words in English which, semantically, tend to denote properties such as size, colour, mood, quality, age, etc. with such members as other, big, new, good, different, Cuban, sure, important, and right. Adjectives head adjective phrases, and the most typical members function as modifiers in noun phrases. Most adjectives either inflect for grade or combine with more and most to form comparatives and superlatives. They are characteristically modifiable by very. A large number of the most typical members combine with the suffix -ly to form adverbs. Most adjectives function as complements in verb phrases, and some license complements of their own.

LFN has an analytic grammar and resembles the grammars of languages such as the Haitian Creole, Papiamento, and Afrikaans. On the other hand, it uses a vocabulary drawn from several modern romance languages – Portuguese, Spanish, Catalan, French, and Italian.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English interrogative words</span> English words that indicate a question is being asked, as a grammatical category

The English interrogative words are words in English with a central role in forming interrogative phrases and clauses and in asking questions. The main members associated with open-ended questions are how, what, when, where, which, who, whom, whose, and why, all of which also have -ever forms. Those associated with closed-ended questions are whether and if.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Huddleston, Rodney; Pullum, Geoffrey K.; Reynolds, Brett (2022). A Student's Introduction to English Grammar (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-1-009-08574-8.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 Huddleston, Rodney; Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-521-43146-0.
  3. Davies, Mark. "Corpus of News on the Web (NOW)". english-corpora.org. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  4. "IN THE CUSTOMS, EXCISE AND SERVICE TAX APPELLATE TRIBUNAL, KOLKATA; EASTERN ZONAL BENCH: KOLKATA; REGIONAL BENCH - COURT NO.2; Excise Appeal No. 674 of 2009", via livelaw.in. Accessed 22 November 2023.
  5. Rodger, James (2 October 2023). "Lloyds Bank sends £15 message to any customer with a credit card". Birmingham Live. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  6. United States Securities and Exchange Commission. "Queen's Gambit Growth Capital" . Retrieved 20 November 2023 via MarketWatch.
  7. Bentley, David (17 November 2023). "DWP announces 12 Universal Credit changes in tough new Back to Work Plan – see how you'll be affected". Birmingham Live. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  8. Sheffield, Rob (10 June 2022). "A requiem for the 'American Idol' dream: 20 years of power ballads and pitchy despair". Rolling Stone . Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  9. Pohang University of Science and Technology (16 November 2023). "Enhancing hydrogen fuel cell durability via tungsten oxide coating". TechXplore. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  10. Saraf, B. L. (17 December 2022). "Assembly election 2022: A window of hope for local parties in J&K". Daily Excelsior . Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  11. Harding, Michael (1 February 2023). "My friend said a bank holiday in honour of some holy biddy who never existed was a farce". The Irish Times . Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  12. "In contempt, Court to deal with compliance of order: HC". Daily Excelsior . 13 August 2017. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  13. Miller, Andrew (30 August 2023). "Brydon Carse makes his mark on debut as England power to seven-wicket victory". ESPN Cricinfo. Retrieved 20 November 2023.
  14. "Edgar S. Cahn, legal reformer in defense of the poor, dies" . New York Times . 27 January 2022.
  15. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Aarts, Bas (2011). Oxford Modern English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN   978-0-19-953319-0.
  16. Pullum, Geoffrey K. (21 April 2010). "An HR bureaucrat, whom cannot write". Language Log . Retrieved 16 November 2023.
  17. Hankamer, Jorge; Postal, Paul (1973). "Whose gorilla". Linguistic Inquiry . 4 (2): 261–271.
  18. "whose (pronoun & adjective, III.4)" . Oxford English Dictionary (Online ed.). Oxford University Press. 2024. Retrieved 14 March 2024.(Subscription or participating institution membership required.)
  19. 1 2 3 McCawley, James D. (1988). The Syntactic Phenomena of English. Vol. 2. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN   0-226-55624-7.
  20. Prince, Ellen (1981). "Toward a taxonomy of given–new information". In Cole, Peter (ed.). Radical Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press. pp. 223–255. ISBN   9780121796600.
  21. Aarts, Bas; Chalker, Sylvia; Weiner, Edmund (2014). Oxford Dictionary of English Grammar (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN   978-0-19-965823-7.
  22. Pullum, Geoffrey K. (20 June 2020). "English language". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.265. ISBN   978-0-19-938465-5 . Retrieved 18 November 2023.
  23. Abraham, Yvonne (27 September 2023). "Try this in a small town. And in every town" . The Boston Globe .
  24. Wittgenstein, Ludwig (1922). Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus . Translated by Ogden, C. K. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.
  25. Hogg, Richard M., ed. (1992). The Cambridge History of the English Language . Vol. I. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 35.
  26. 1 2 3 4 5 Wilbur, Terence H. (1963). "The Germanic interrogatives of the how type". Word . 19 (3): 328–334. doi: 10.1080/00437956.1963.11659802 .
  27. "Who" . Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford University Press . Retrieved 18 November 2023.
  28. "Whither" . Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford University Press . Retrieved 18 November 2023.
  29. "Which" . Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford University Press . Retrieved 18 November 2023.
  30. 1 2 3 Minkova, Donka (2004). "Philology, linguistics, and the history of [hw]~[w]". In Curzan, Anne; Emmons, Kimberly (eds.). Studies in the history of the English language II: Unfolding conversations. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN   3-11-018097-9.
  31. "Map 8: The maintenance of the /hw/~/w/ contrast". Telsur Project. Linguistics Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania. Retrieved 2023-11-18.
  32. Li, Zeyu; Gut, Ulrike (2023-05-01). "The distribution of /w/ and /ʍ/ in Scottish Standard English". Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. De Gruyter. 19 (2): 271–287. doi: 10.1515/cllt-2021-0052 . ISSN   1613-7035.
  33. 1 2 Cruttenden, Alan (2014). Gimson's Pronunciation of English (8th ed.). London: Routledge.