Consideration set

Last updated

Consideration set is a model used in consumer behaviour to represent all of the brands and products a consumer evaluates before making a final purchase decision. The term consideration set was first used in 1977 by Peter Wright and Fredrick Barbour. [1] The consideration set is a subset of the awareness set, which is all of the brands and products a consumer initially thinks of when faced with a purchasing decision. [2] The awareness set is filtered into the consideration set through the consumer's individual thoughts, preferences, and feelings – such as price, mood, previous experiences, and heuristics. Conversely, products that do not meet the criteria for the consideration set are either placed into the inert set or the inept set. These sets are fluid and the products in each set can change rapidly when the consumer is presented with new information. [3]

Contents

Set hierarchy and theory

Consider the universal set, , to be the set of all products and brands which will satisfy a given need. [3] The awareness or knowledge set, , is defined as all of the products and brands within the universal set that the consumer is aware of. [3] This awareness can be derived from a product search, brand familiarity, advertisements, word-of-mouth, or any other method which informs the consumer of a viable option. While the awareness set is largely composed of products that reside in the consumer's long-term memory, the awareness set can also be expanded by products found during the search process – such as recommended products on an e-commerce site or shelves in a supermarket. Thus .

The awareness set can be further divided into the following sets:

Thus,

Psychological basis

The consideration set models how humans behave when faced with many choices. While the consideration set is not directly observable, researchers believe that its existence is evident by a logical conjunction of prominent economic and psychological theories. [3]

In this model, consumers screen many product options before fully evaluating just a few options (usually 2–5), [5] which reduces the cognitive load and fatigue of an exhaustive search. [6] This screening process is mostly based on heuristics about the product, and is generally considered to be a lower-effort process than the evaluation of the consideration set, once it is formed. Studies on heuristic screening methodologies have shown that consumers are more satisfied with their purchase decision and less stressed by the decision-making process when the consideration set is smaller. [7] This is particularly true when information overload is high; that is, the quantity of available information exceeds the processing power a consumer is willing to dedicate to it.

Researchers have several theories as to why the consideration set is formed. The formation of the consideration set is considered the first step in common decision-making frameworks, with the second step being choosing an alternative from the set. [8]

Formation

Consumer behavior researchers have identified many frameworks, methodologies, and heuristics consumers often use to form a consideration set. It is important to note that this is not an exhaustive list, and that the formation of the consideration set varies significantly depending on the consumer and context of the decision. [5] Nonetheless, some commonly observed methodologies are:

Decision-making

Once the consideration set is formed, the next step is to select an alternative from the evaluated options. [8] There are several models that describe how consumers make these selections, however many researchers believe that this process is concurrent with the formation process; that is, product selection is often being contemplated while the consideration set is forming. [11] Nonetheless, the following methodologies are widely considered to be the second step of consumer decision making:

Limitations and critiques

The decision-making process is still not well enough understood to clarify the distinction between the models used to represent the process and the process of decision-making itself. [3] Many researchers reject the idea of a two-step decision-making process using a consideration set, and instead insist on viewing the consideration set as simply an indicator of preferences. [8] Many researchers claim that knowing a particular consumer's consideration set is not enough to predict their final product choice, and this knowledge is trivial when compared to something like the utility function, which is much more robust. Since neither the utility function or consideration set are directly observable, [9] researchers are still unsure whether either is an accurate or useful model for describing choice.

Another criticism of the consideration set is how it is applied. Marketers often assume that all consumers have the same consideration set; that is, they assume all consumers are selecting between the same set of options. The consideration set is actually theorized to be highly individualistic – and the products within it reflect a variety of factors such as the consumer's socioeconomic status, attitudes, and perceptions. [3] This implies that marketers should treat consideration set formation as probabilistic, rather than objective.

References

  1. "Phased Decision Strategies: Sequels to an Initial Screening". Stanford Graduate School of Business.
  2. "Awareness Set". Monash Business School.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Shocker, Allan D.; Ben-Akiva, Moshe; Boccara, Bruno; Nedungadi, Prakash (1991). "Consideration Set Influences on Consumer Decision-Making and Choice: Issues, Models, and Suggestions". Marketing Letters. 2 (3): 181–197. doi:10.1007/BF02404071. JSTOR   40216215.
  4. 1 2 "Evoked, Inert and Inept sets". Noble Marketeers. June 18, 2016.
  5. 1 2 Aurier, Philippe; Jean, Sylvie; Zaichkowsky, Judith L. (April 22, 2000). "Consideration Set Size and Familiarity With Usage Context". ACR North American Advances. NA-27.
  6. Hauser, John R. (August 2014). "Consideration-set heuristics". Journal of Business Research. 67 (8): 1688–1699. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.02.015. hdl: 1721.1/111102 .
  7. Vogrincic-Haselbacher, Claudia; Krueger, Joachim I.; Lurger, Brigitta; Dinslaken, Isabelle; Anslinger, Julian; Caks, Florian; Florack, Arnd; Brohmer, Hilmar; Athenstaedt, Ursula (28 April 2021). "Not Too Much and Not Too Little: Information Processing for a Good Purchase Decision". Frontiers in Psychology. 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.642641 . PMC   8115021 . PMID   33995196.
  8. 1 2 3 Horowitz, Joel L; Louviere, Jordan J (May 1995). "What is the role of consideration sets in choice modeling?". International Journal of Research in Marketing. 12 (1): 39–54. doi:10.1016/0167-8116(95)00004-L.
  9. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Lleras, Juan Sebastián; Masatlioglu, Yusufcan; Nakajima, Daisuke; Ozbay, Erkut Y. (July 2017). "When more is less: Limited consideration". Journal of Economic Theory. 170: 70–85. doi:10.1016/j.jet.2017.04.004.
  10. Manzini, Paola; Mariotti, Marco (December 2007). "Sequentially Rationalizable Choice" (PDF). American Economic Review. 97 (5): 1824–1839. doi:10.1257/aer.97.5.1824. JSTOR   30034586.
  11. Parkinson, Thomas L. (April 22, 1979). "An Information Processing Approach to Evoked Set Formation". ACR North American Advances. NA-06.
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 Laroche, Michel; Kim, Chankon; Matsui, Takayoshi (June 2003). "Which decision heuristics are used in consideration set formation?". Journal of Consumer Marketing. 20 (3): 192–209. doi:10.1108/07363760310472236.
  13. Hauser, John R.; Toubia, Olivier; Evgeniou, Theodoros; Befurt, Rene; Dzyabura, Daria (June 2010). "Disjunctions of Conjunctions, Cognitive Simplicity, and Consideration Sets". Journal of Marketing Research. 47 (3): 485–496. doi:10.1509/jmkr.47.3.485.