Context effect

Last updated

"THE CAT" is a classic example of context effect. We have little trouble reading "H" and "A" in their appropriate contexts, even though they take on the same form in each word. TheCat.png
"THE CAT" is a classic example of context effect. We have little trouble reading "H" and "A" in their appropriate contexts, even though they take on the same form in each word.

A context effect is an aspect of cognitive psychology that describes the influence of environmental factors on one's perception of a stimulus. [1] The impact of context effects is considered to be part of top-down design. The concept is supported by the theoretical approach to perception known as constructive perception. Context effects can impact our daily lives in many ways such as word recognition, learning abilities, memory, and object recognition. It can have an extensive effect on marketing and consumer decisions. For example, research has shown that the comfort level of the floor that shoppers are standing on while reviewing products can affect their assessments of product's quality, leading to higher assessments if the floor is comfortable and lower ratings if it is uncomfortable. Because of effects such as this, context effects are currently studied predominantly in marketing. [2]

Contents

Cognitive principles of context effects

Context effects employ top-down design when analyzing information. Top down design fuels understanding of an image by using prior experiences and knowledge to interpret a stimulus. This process helps us analyze familiar scenes and objects when encountering them. [3] During perception of any kind, people generally use either sensory data (bottom-up design) or prior knowledge of the stimulus (top-down design) when analyzing the stimulus. Individuals generally use both types of processing to examine stimuli. [4] The use of both sensory data and prior knowledge to reach a conclusion is a feature of optimal probabilistic reasoning, known as Bayesian inference; cognitive scientists have shown mathematically how context effects can emerge from the Bayesian inference process. [5] [6] When context effects occur, individuals are using environmental cues perceived while examining the stimuli in order to help analyze it. In other words, individuals often make relative decisions that are influenced by the environment or previous exposure to objects.

These decisions may be greatly influenced by these external forces and alter the way individuals view an object. For example, research has shown that people rank television commercials as either good or bad in relation to their enjoyment levels of the show during which the commercials are presented. The more they like or dislike the show the more likely they are to rate the commercials shown during the show more positively or negatively (respectively). [7] Another example shows during sound recognition a context effect can use other sounds in the environment to change the way we categorize a sound. [8] [ page needed ]

Context effects can come in several forms, including configural superiority effect which demonstrates varying degrees of spatial recognition depending on if stimuli are present in an organized configuration or present in isolation. [9] [ failed verification ] For example, one may recognize a fully composed object faster than its individual parts (object-superiority effect).

Impact

Context effects can have a wide range of impacts in daily life. In reading difficult handwriting context effects are used to determine what letters make up a word. [10] This helps us analyze potentially ambiguous messages and decipher them correctly. It can also affect our perception of unknown sounds based on the noise in the environment. [11] For example, we may fill in a word we cannot make out in a sentence based on the other words we could understand. Context can prime our attitudes and beliefs about certain topics based on current environmental factors and our previous experiences with them. [12]

Context effects can be nullified if we are made consciously aware of the outside stimulus or past history that may influence our decision. For example, a study conducted by Norbert Schwarz and Gerald Clore showed that when asked to rate their overall life satisfaction on either sunny or rainy days, people expressed greater satisfaction on sunny days and less satisfaction on rainy days. However, when people were reminded of the weather their satisfaction rating returned to an almost even distribution. [13] This study demonstrates the effect the environment can have on perception, and that when pointed out, context effects can be nullified.

Context effects also affect memory. We are often better able to recall information in the location in which we learned it or studied it. For example, while studying for a test it is better to study in the environment that the test will be taken in (i.e. classroom) than in a location where the information was not learned and will not need to be recalled. [14] This phenomenon is called transfer-appropriate processing.

Marketing

"READ" is a good example of the "context effects" role in the word superiority effect by having us assume that the E and the A behind the ink blot are completed. Contexteffectsandwordsuperiority.gif
"READ" is a good example of the "context effects" role in the word superiority effect by having us assume that the E and the A behind the ink blot are completed.

Perhaps the greatest amount of research concerning context effects comes from marketing research. Context effects can influence consumers' choice behavior. [15] There are three main context effects that are researched in marketing. The first, the compromise effect, states that objects that are priced in the middle of choice sets are looked on more favorably. [16] The middle choice seems like a good compromise between choices that may be viewed as too extreme. For example, if there are bikes on sale at the local store ranging from $50 to $75; the average shopper, not looking for a luxury bicycle, would normally opt to choose the $60 bike because it is average and matches their level of expertise. However, when adding a $100 bike to the sale, most average shoppers would opt to select the higher $75 bike. This demonstrates the compromise effect of choosing a bike to match their expectations regarding middle prices. [17]

The attractiveness effect, the second contextual effect on consumer behavior, maintains that one item will increase the attractiveness of another item that is similar, but superior to it. By showing that an item is superior to a similar one the likability and possible purchasing power of the superior item increases. [18]

The similarity effect, the third contextual effect on consumers' behavior, states that an item will hurt a similar item more in sales than it will a dissimilar item. With two or more similar items competing for attention they will only detract from each other in the marketplace. [15]

Firms looking to increase product resale can use these context effects to construct more profitable marketing strategies. The idea of these context effects in the marketing industry is to allow for more profitable items a boost in optional sharing; a productivity boost based on contextual effects for and against certain items. [15]

Context Effect on Consumer Behavior

In a study conducted on 55 undergraduate marketing students at a university in Korea, researchers set up a mixed design to test if a visual framing promoting a greater use of alternative-based processing would reduce the perceived attractiveness of compromise options. They also hypothesized that the decision process would have minimal influence on the choice of asymmetrically dominating options.

Researchers split the participants into three conditions: attribute based processing treatment, alternative based processing treatment, and the control. In order to perpetuate attribute and alternative based processing in their participants, researchers used different visual tactics to present each product. In the attribute processing group, horizontal lines were drawn in between each attribute of a product option, highlighting the various attributes of the different products within the same choice set. Conversely, in the alternative treatment group, vertical lines were drawn in between individual product options to visually separate them from one another. The control group had no visual framing treatment. Further, researchers simultaneously assessed how the attractiveness and compromise effect impacts the probability of the consumer to choose a target brand by listing two attributes for each of the three products in the choice set. Depending on the extremity in differences between each product attribute, options were either placed in the compromise or asymmetrically dominant subgroup.  

The findings of this study proved their hypothesis, as the frequency of how often the compromise option was chosen depends heavily on the difference in visual framing of the attribute and alternative based processing treatments. The study found that when the alternative treatment was not promoted, the compromise effect took precedence over the participants’ decision making.  Furthermore, the study showed that there was no significant difference between the attribute and control treatments, as the probability of choosing an asymmetrically dominant option was equally high across all three framing conditions.

Perception of Artwork

Context also affects the perception of artwork. Artworks presented in a classical museum context were liked more and rated more interesting than when presented in a sterile laboratory context. While specific results depend heavily on the style of the presented artwork, overall, the effect of context proved to be more important for the perception of artwork then the effect of genuineness (whether the artwork was being presented as original or as a facsimile/copy). [19]

Related Research Articles

Sales promotion is one of the elements of the promotional mix. The primary elements in the promotional mix are advertising, personal selling, direct marketing and publicity/public relations. Sales promotion uses both media and non-media marketing communications for a pre-determined, limited time to increase consumer demand, stimulate market demand or improve product availability. Examples include contests, coupons, freebies, loss leaders, point of purchase displays, premiums, prizes, product samples, and rebates.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Conjoint analysis</span> Survey-based statistical technique

Conjoint analysis is a survey-based statistical technique used in market research that helps determine how people value different attributes that make up an individual product or service.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Consumer behaviour</span> Study of individuals, groups, or organisations and all the activities associated with consuming

Consumer behaviour is the study of individuals, groups, or organisations and all the activities associated with the purchase, use and disposal of goods and services. Consumer behaviour consists of how the consumer's emotions, attitudes, and preferences affect buying behaviour. Consumer behaviour emerged in the 1940–1950s as a distinct sub-discipline of marketing, but has become an interdisciplinary social science that blends elements from psychology, sociology, social anthropology, anthropology, ethnography, ethnology, marketing, and economics.

As part of consumer behavior, the buying decision process is the decision-making process used by consumers regarding the market transactions before, during, and after the purchase of a good or service. It can be seen as a particular form of a cost–benefit analysis in the presence of multiple alternatives.

Salience is the state or condition of being prominent. The Oxford English Dictionary defines salience as "most noticeable or important." The concept is discussed in communication, semiotics, linguistics, sociology, psychology, and political science. It has been studied with respect to interpersonal communication, persuasion, politics, and its influence on mass media.

The kappa effect or perceptual time dilation is a temporal perceptual illusion that can arise when observers judge the elapsed time between sensory stimuli applied sequentially at different locations. In perceiving a sequence of consecutive stimuli, subjects tend to overestimate the elapsed time between two successive stimuli when the distance between the stimuli is sufficiently large, and to underestimate the elapsed time when the distance is sufficiently small.

Customer satisfaction is a term frequently used in marketing to evaluate customer experience. It is a measure of how products and services supplied by a company meet or surpass customer expectation. Customer satisfaction is defined as "the number of customers, or percentage of total customers, whose reported experience with a firm, its products, or its services (ratings) exceeds specified satisfaction goals." Enhancing customer satisfaction and fostering customer loyalty are pivotal for businesses, given the significant importance of improving the balance between customer attitudes before and after the consumption process.

Choice modelling attempts to model the decision process of an individual or segment via revealed preferences or stated preferences made in a particular context or contexts. Typically, it attempts to use discrete choices in order to infer positions of the items on some relevant latent scale. Indeed many alternative models exist in econometrics, marketing, sociometrics and other fields, including utility maximization, optimization applied to consumer theory, and a plethora of other identification strategies which may be more or less accurate depending on the data, sample, hypothesis and the particular decision being modelled. In addition, choice modelling is regarded as the most suitable method for estimating consumers' willingness to pay for quality improvements in multiple dimensions.

Overchoice or choice overload is the paradoxical phenomenon that choosing between a large variety of options can be detrimental to decision making processes. The term was first introduced by Alvin Toffler in his 1970 book, Future Shock.

Choice architecture is the design of different ways in which choices can be presented to decision makers, and the impact of that presentation on decision-making. For example, each of the following:

The framing effect is a cognitive bias in which people decide between options based on whether the options are presented with positive or negative connotations. Individuals have a tendency to make risk-avoidant choices when options are positively framed, while selecting more loss-avoidant options when presented with a negative frame. In studies of the bias, options are presented in terms of the probability of either losses or gains. While differently expressed, the options described are in effect identical. Gain and loss are defined in the scenario as descriptions of outcomes, for example, lives lost or saved, patients treated or not treated, monetary gains or losses.

Itamar Simonson is a professor of marketing, holding the Sebastian S. Kresge Chair of Marketing in the Graduate School of Business, Stanford University. He is known for his work on the factors that determine the choices that buyers make. His academic career started at the University of California at Berkeley, where he taught for six years, before he moved to Stanford. Many of his former PhD students hold senior positions at some of the best universities in the world.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Color psychology</span> Study of influence of color on human behavior

Color psychology is the study of hues as a determinant of human behavior. Color influences perceptions that are not obvious, such as the taste of food. Colors have qualities that can cause certain emotions in people. How color influences individuals may differ depending on age, gender, and culture. Although color associations can vary contextually between cultures, color preference is thought to be relatively uniform across gender and race.

Processing fluency is the ease with which information is processed. Perceptual fluency is the ease of processing stimuli based on manipulations to perceptual quality. Retrieval fluency is the ease with which information can be retrieved from memory.

Subliminal stimuli are any sensory stimuli below an individual's threshold for conscious perception, in contrast to supraliminal stimuli.

The memory system plays a key role in the decision-making process because individuals constantly choose among alternative options. Due to the volume of decisions made, much of the decision-making process is unconscious and automatic. Information about how a decision is made is remembered and used for future decisions. Memory is susceptible to biases, but it is integral to the formation of preferences and to differentiation between choices.

Media context studies refers to the group of studies investigating “how and which media context variables influence the effects of the advertisements embedded in the context“. Media researchers found that media context affects ad recall, ad recognition, level and nature of ad processing, ad attitude and ad cognitions, brand attitude, and purchase intention.

Consumer value is used to describe a consumer's strong relative preference for certain subjectively evaluated product or service attributes.

The frequency illusion, also known as the Baader-Meinhof phenomenon, is a cognitive bias where people who notice something new, like a word or object, begin to encounter it frequently. This perception is influenced by cognitive processes such as selective attention, where the brain subconsciously emphasizes new information, and confirmation bias, which reinforces the belief of increased frequency. For example, after hearing a unique piece of music for the first time, you might start noticing it in various places, from radio stations to social media feeds, not because it's being played more, but because your brain is now primed to recognize it.

Ana Valenzuela is a Marketing and Consumer Psychology professor and academic. She is Professor of Marketing at the Allen G. Aaronson Department of Marketing and International Business at City University of New York’s Baruch College, and Coordinator of the Marketing Specialization of the PhD Program in Business. She is also a Visiting Professor at the Department of Marketing of ESADE Business School at University Ramon Llull.

References

  1. Nikolić, D. (2010). The brain is a context machine. Review of psychology, 17(1), 33-38.
  2. Meyers-Levy, Joan; Zhu, Rui (Juliet); Jiang, Lan (1 June 2010). "Context Effects from Bodily Sensations: Examining Bodily Sensations Induced by Flooring and the Moderating Role of Product Viewing Distance". Journal of Consumer Research. 37 (1): 1–14. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.173.940 . doi:10.1086/649028.
  3. Cavanagh, Patrick (1991). "What's up in top-down processing?" (PDF). In Gorea, Andrei (ed.). Representations of Vision: Trends and Tacit Assumptions in Vision Research. Cambridge University Press. pp. 295–304. ISBN   9780521412285.
  4. Borenstein, E.; Ullman, S. (1 December 2008). "Combined Top-Down/Bottom-Up Segmentation". IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence. 30 (12): 2109–2125. doi:10.1109/TPAMI.2007.70840. PMID   18988946. S2CID   2279060.
  5. Lloyd, K.; Leslie, D. S. (20 February 2013). "Context-dependent decision-making: a simple Bayesian model". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 10 (82): 20130069. doi:10.1098/rsif.2013.0069. PMC   3627089 . PMID   23427101.
  6. Goldreich, Daniel; Peterson, Mary A. (2012). "A Bayesian Observer Replicates Convexity Context Effects in Figure–Ground Perception". Seeing and Perceiving. 25 (3): 365–395. doi:10.1163/187847612X634445. PMID   22564398.
  7. Goldberg, Marvin E.; Gorn, Gerald J. (1 December 1987). "Happy and Sad TV Programs: How They Affect Reactions to Commercials". Journal of Consumer Research. 14 (3): 387. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.1025.5403 . doi:10.1086/209122.
  8. Cavanaugh, William J.; Tocci, Gregory C.; Wilkes, Joseph A. (2010). Architectural acoustics : principles and practice (2nd ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. ISBN   978-0470190524.
  9. Pomerantz, James R. (2003). "Perception: Overview". Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. doi:10.1002/0470018860.s00589. ISBN   0470016191. 3:527-537.
  10. McClelland, James L.; Rumelhart, David E. (1981). "An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings". Psychological Review. 88 (5): 375–407. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375.
  11. Moore, Brian C.J (1 July 2003). "Temporal integration and context effects in hearing". Journal of Phonetics. 31 (3–4): 563–574. doi:10.1016/S0095-4470(03)00011-1.
  12. Tourangeau, Roger; Rasinski, Kenneth A. (1988). "Cognitive processes underlying context effects in attitude measurement". Psychological Bulletin. 103 (3): 299–314. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.581.8942 . doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.299.
  13. Kruglanski, Arie W.; Higgins, Edward Tory (2007). Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed.). Guilford Press. p. 389. ISBN   9781572309180.
  14. Tulving, Endel; Thomson, Donald M. (1973). "Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory". Psychological Review. 80 (5): 352–373. doi:10.1037/h0020071. S2CID   14879511.
  15. 1 2 3 Rooderkerk, Robert P.; Van Heerde, Harald J.; Bijmolt, Tammo H.A. (1 August 2011). "Incorporating Context Effects Into a Choice Model". Journal of Marketing Research. 48 (4): 767–780. doi:10.1509/jmkr.48.4.767. S2CID   145311303.
  16. Simonson, Itamar (1 September 1989). "Choice Based on Reasons: The Case of Attraction and Compromise Effects" (PDF). Journal of Consumer Research. 16 (2): 158. doi:10.1086/209205. S2CID   144558711. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 February 2020.
  17. Wernerfelt, Birger (March 1995). "A Rational Reconstruction of the Compromise Effect: Using Market Data to Infer Utilities" (PDF). Journal of Consumer Research. 21 (4): 627–33. doi:10.1086/209423 . Retrieved 25 October 2012.
  18. Hedgcock, William; Rao, Akshay R. (1 February 2009). "Trade-Off Aversion as an Explanation for the Attraction Effect: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study". Journal of Marketing Research. 46 (1): 1–13. doi:10.1509/jmkr.46.1.1. S2CID   18010540.
  19. Susanne Grüner; Eva Specker & Helmut Leder (2019). "Effects of Context and Genuineness in the Experience of Art". Empirical Studies of the Arts. 37 (2): 138–152. doi:10.1177/0276237418822896. S2CID   150115587. Archived from the original on 24 January 2020. Retrieved 26 August 2021.