Earl K. Miller

Last updated

Earl Miller
Earl K. Miller - Nov 2022.jpg
Earl K. Miller 2022
Born
Earl Keith Miller

(1962-11-30) November 30, 1962 (age 61)
Alma mater Kent State University (BS, Doctor of Science, honoris causa)
Princeton University (MS, PhD)
AwardsGoldman-Rakic Prize for Outstanding Achievement in Cognitive Neuroscience (2016)

Election to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences (2017)

Contents

The George A. Miller Prize in Cognitive Neuroscience (2019)

Troland Research Award (2000)
Scientific career
Fields Neuroscience
Cognitive science [1]
Institutions Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Thesis Neurophysiological investigations of inferior temporal cortex of the macaque  (1990)
Doctoral advisor Charles G. Gross [2]
Notable students
Website ekmillerlab.mit.edu/earl-miller/

Earl Keith Miller (born November 30, 1962) is a cognitive neuroscientist whose research focuses on neural mechanisms of cognitive, or executive, control. [4] [1] Earl K. Miller is the Picower Professor of Neuroscience with the Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at Massachusetts Institute of Technology. [5] [6] [7] He is the Chief Scientist and co-founder of SplitSage. [8] He is a co-founder of Neuroblox.

Education

Earl Miller received a Bachelor of Arts degree (summa cum laude, with honors) in psychology from Kent State University in 1985, Master of Arts degree in psychology and neuroscience from Princeton University in 1987, and a PhD in psychology and neuroscience from Princeton University in 1990. In 2020, Earl Miller was awarded an honorary doctorate (Doctor of Science, honoris causa) from Kent State University.

Career

Earl Miller's dissertation on neurophysiological investigations of the inferior temporal cortex in the macaque was supervised by Charles G. Gross at Princeton University. [2] From 1990–1995 he was a postdoctoral research fellow in the laboratory of neuropsychology at the National Institute of Mental Health under supervision of Robert Desimone.[ citation needed ]

In 1995, Earl Miller joined the faculty of the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT as Assistant Professor of Neuroscience and quickly advanced the academic ranks. He received tenure in 1999 (two years ahead of schedule) and became a full Professor in 2002. He was appointed to the Picower chair at MIT in 2003. He was Associate Director of the Picower Institute for Learning and Memory at MIT from 2001 to 2009, and was Director of Graduate Studies in Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT. He has delivered numerous lectures worldwide, serves as editor, and on the editorial boards of, major journals in neuroscience, and on international advisory boards. He has served on the scientific advisory boards of NeuroFocus, Thync, Motimatic, and Neurable. [9] [10] Professor Miller is the co-founder and Chief Scientist of SplitSage [8] and a co-founder of Neuroblox.

Research

Professor Miller studies the neural basis of executive brain functions. Executive functions are the ability to carry out goal-directed behavior using complex mental processes and cognitive abilities. This includes working memory, attention, decision-making and learning. His lab has had made discoveries about the neural circuits, networks, and mechanisms by which the brain’s prefrontal cortex wields executive control. They have shown how categories and concepts are learned, how multifunctional, mixed-selectivity neurons endow the cortex with computational versatility and flexibility, and how neural oscillations regulate neural communication and consciousness. This work has established a foundation upon which to construct more detailed, mechanistic accounts of cognition and its dysfunction in diseases such as autism, schizophrenia and attention deficit disorder.

Examples of discoveries from Earl Miller's laboratory include the neural basis of abstract rules like "same vs. different", [3] categories, [11] quantity, [12] and the allocation of attentional resources. [13] They have also shown how the brain can learn and flexibly remap associations. [14] The Miller Lab has shown that cortical neurons can be multifunctional (i.e., show "mixed selectivity"). [15] This is a major advance beyond earlier theories that each neuron has a specific function. This property gives the brain greater computational horsepower and endows flexibility, a hallmark of higher-level cognition. [16]

Miller has innovated techniques for recording from many neurons simultaneously in multiple brain areas, a departure from the classic single-neuron recording approach. It has revealed network dynamics and emergent properties that are not possible by studying individual neurons. [17] Miller's lab has used this approach to understand how network interactions produce thought and action. [18] This includes discoveries that oscillating "brain waves" control the timing of shifts of attention [19] and that different items simultaneously held in working memory line up on different phases of each brain wave. [20] The latter may explain why we can only think about a few things at the same time. [21] They have shown that lower-frequency (alpha/beta) brain waves act as a top-down control signal that regulates sensory processing in cortex. [22] [23] They found that brain waves transfer information between the left and right cerebral cortex. [24] They have shown how the general anesthetic, propofol, induces unconsciousness by shifting cortical brain waves to low frequencies [25]

Miller's paper with Jonathan Cohen, An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Function, [26] has been designated a Current Classic as among the most cited papers in Neuroscience and Behavior. [27] It is the 5th most-cited paper in the history of Neuroscience. [28] His paper with Tim Buschman, Top-down versus Bottom-up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices [29] was The Scientist's Hot Paper for October 2009. [30] Earl K. Miler was named in the top 2% of scientists worldwide [31] He has been cited in over 50,000 publications [1]

Selected Awards and Honors

Doctor of Science (honoris causa), Kent State University (2020).

The George A. Miller Prize in Cognitive Neuroscience (2019).

Elected to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences (2017) [32]

Paul and Lilah Newton Brain Science Award (2017)

The Goldman-Rakic Prize for Outstanding Achievement in Cognitive Neuroscience (2016) [33]

Kent State University Professional Achievement Award (2016). [34]

Elected to the Memory Disorders Research Society, 2016

Amar G. Bose Research Fellowship (2014), [35]

MERIT Award, National Institute of Mental Health (2010)[ citation needed ]

The Mathilde Solowey Award in Neurosciences (2007)[ citation needed ]

Elected Fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science (2005),[ citation needed ]

Picower Professorship at MIT (endowed chair) (2003)

The Society for Neuroscience Young Investigator Award (2000) [36]

National Academy of Sciences Troland Research Award (2000) [37]

Tenured at MIT two years ahead of schedule (1999)

John Merck Scholar Award (1998),[ citation needed ]

McKnight Scholar Award (1996),[ citation needed ]

Pew Scholar Award (1996) [38]

Alfred P. Sloan Research Fellow (1996)

Phi Beta Kappa (1985)

Full list of awards and honors can be found here.

Business

The discovery that different individuals have different perceptual capacities in different parts of their field of view led Earl Miller to found SplitSage. [39] SplitSage uses a patented process to assess these individual differences. It can be used to customize displays, develop individualized training, and organize teams to maximize information throughput and improve situational awareness and performance.

Media Appearances

Earl Miller has made frequent appearances in the popular press. He was profiled in Discover Magazine [40] and The New Yorker. [41] He wrote a guest column in Fortune. [42] Professor Miller has appeared on NBC's Today Show [43] , on CBS Sunday Morning with Jane Pauley [44] , and has been a frequent guest on National Public Radio and other radio talk shows as well as podcasts. He has been quoted and/or his work profiled in the New York Times, The Washington Post, CNN, MSNBC, Time, [45] ABC News, Slate, The Boston Globe, The Times of London, Forbes [46] etc. A list of media appearances is online. [47]

Philanthropy

Earl Miller has established a charitable trust to create scholarships for disadvantaged students at his alma mater Kent State University [48]

Related Research Articles

Working memory is a cognitive system with a limited capacity that can hold information temporarily. It is important for reasoning and the guidance of decision-making and behavior. Working memory is often used synonymously with short-term memory, but some theorists consider the two forms of memory distinct, assuming that working memory allows for the manipulation of stored information, whereas short-term memory only refers to the short-term storage of information. Working memory is a theoretical concept central to cognitive psychology, neuropsychology, and neuroscience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive neuroscience</span> Scientific field

Cognitive neuroscience is the scientific field that is concerned with the study of the biological processes and aspects that underlie cognition, with a specific focus on the neural connections in the brain which are involved in mental processes. It addresses the questions of how cognitive activities are affected or controlled by neural circuits in the brain. Cognitive neuroscience is a branch of both neuroscience and psychology, overlapping with disciplines such as behavioral neuroscience, cognitive psychology, physiological psychology and affective neuroscience. Cognitive neuroscience relies upon theories in cognitive science coupled with evidence from neurobiology, and computational modeling.

In the philosophy of mind, neuroscience, and cognitive science, a mental image is an experience that, on most occasions, significantly resembles the experience of "perceiving" some object, event, or scene but occurs when the relevant object, event, or scene is not actually present to the senses. There are sometimes episodes, particularly on falling asleep and waking up, when the mental imagery may be dynamic, phantasmagoric, and involuntary in character, repeatedly presenting identifiable objects or actions, spilling over from waking events, or defying perception, presenting a kaleidoscopic field, in which no distinct object can be discerned. Mental imagery can sometimes produce the same effects as would be produced by the behavior or experience imagined.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dopaminergic pathways</span> Projection neurons in the brain that synthesize and release dopamine

Dopaminergic pathways in the human brain are involved in both physiological and behavioral processes including movement, cognition, executive functions, reward, motivation, and neuroendocrine control. Each pathway is a set of projection neurons, consisting of individual dopaminergic neurons.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prefrontal cortex</span> Part of the brain responsible for personality, decision-making, and social behavior

In mammalian brain anatomy, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) covers the front part of the frontal lobe of the cerebral cortex. It is the association cortex in the frontal lobe. The PFC contains the Brodmann areas BA8, BA9, BA10, BA11, BA12, BA13, BA14, BA24, BA25, BA32, BA44, BA45, BA46, and BA47.

David J. Heeger is an American neuroscientist, psychologist, computer scientist, data scientist, and entrepreneur. He is a professor at New York University, Chief Scientific Officer of Statespace Labs, and Chief Scientific Officer and co-founder of Epistemic AI.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Executive functions</span> Cognitive processes necessary for control of behavior

In cognitive science and neuropsychology, executive functions are a set of cognitive processes that are necessary for the cognitive control of behavior: selecting and successfully monitoring behaviors that facilitate the attainment of chosen goals. Executive functions include basic cognitive processes such as attentional control, cognitive inhibition, inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. Higher-order executive functions require the simultaneous use of multiple basic executive functions and include planning and fluid intelligence.

Mriganka Sur is the Newton Professor of Neuroscience and Director of the Simons Center for the Social Brain at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. He is also a Visiting Faculty Member in the Department of Computer Science and Engineering at the Indian Institute of Technology Madras and N.R. Narayana Murthy Distinguished Chair in Computational Brain Research at the Centre for Computational Brain Research, IIT Madras. He was on the Life Sciences jury for the Infosys Prize in 2010 and has been serving as Jury Chair from 2018.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Medial dorsal nucleus</span>

The medial dorsal nucleus is a large nucleus in the thalamus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex</span> Area of the prefrontal cortex of primates

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is an area in the prefrontal cortex of the primate brain. It is one of the most recently derived parts of the human brain. It undergoes a prolonged period of maturation which lasts into adulthood. The DLPFC is not an anatomical structure, but rather a functional one. It lies in the middle frontal gyrus of humans. In macaque monkeys, it is around the principal sulcus. Other sources consider that DLPFC is attributed anatomically to BA 9 and 46 and BA 8, 9 and 10.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Patricia Goldman-Rakic</span> American neuroscientist

Patricia Goldman-Rakic was an American professor of neuroscience, neurology, psychiatry and psychology at Yale University School of Medicine. She pioneered multidisciplinary research of the prefrontal cortex and working memory.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neural correlates of consciousness</span> Neuronal events sufficient for a specific conscious percept

The neural correlates of consciousness (NCC) are the minimal set of neuronal events and mechanisms sufficient for the occurrence of the mental states to which they are related. Neuroscientists use empirical approaches to discover neural correlates of subjective phenomena; that is, neural changes which necessarily and regularly correlate with a specific experience. The set should be minimal because, under the materialist assumption that the brain is sufficient to give rise to any given conscious experience, the question is which of its components are necessary to produce it.

Cultural neuroscience is a field of research that focuses on the interrelation between a human's cultural environment and neurobiological systems. The field particularly incorporates ideas and perspectives from related domains like anthropology, psychology, and cognitive neuroscience to study sociocultural influences on human behaviors. Such impacts on behavior are often measured using various neuroimaging methods, through which cross-cultural variability in neural activity can be examined.

Robert Desimone is an American neuroscientist who currently serves as the director of the McGovern Institute for Brain Research and the Doris and Don Berkey Professor of Neuroscience at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Consumer neuroscience is the combination of consumer research with modern neuroscience. The goal of the field is to find neural explanations for consumer behaviors in individuals both with or without disease.

Richard Alan Andersen is an American neuroscientist. He is the James G. Boswell Professor of Neuroscience at the California Institute of Technology in Pasadena, California. His research focuses on visual physiology with an emphasis on translational research to humans in the field of neuroprosthetics, brain-computer interfaces, and cortical repair.

The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) considers intelligence to relate to how well different brain regions integrate to form intelligent behaviors. The theory proposes that large scale brain networks connect brain regions, including regions within frontal, parietal, temporal, and cingulate cortices, underlie the biological basis of human intelligence. These regions, which overlap significantly with the task-positive network, allow the brain to communicate and exchange information efficiently with one another. Support for this theory is primarily based on neuroimaging evidence, with support from lesion studies. The P-FIT is influential in that it explains the majority of current neuroimaging findings, as well as increasing empirical support for cognition being the result of large-scale brain networks, rather than numerous domain-specific processes or modules. A 2010 review of the neuroscience of intelligence described P-FIT as "the best available answer to the question of where in the brain intelligence resides".

In psychology, associative memory is defined as the ability to learn and remember the relationship between unrelated items. This would include, for example, remembering the name of someone or the aroma of a particular perfume. This type of memory deals specifically with the relationship between these different objects or concepts. A normal associative memory task involves testing participants on their recall of pairs of unrelated items, such as face-name pairs. Associative memory is a declarative memory structure and episodically based.

Joni Wallis is a cognitive neurophysiologist and Professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of California, Berkeley.

Social cognitive neuroscience is the scientific study of the biological processes underpinning social cognition. Specifically, it uses the tools of neuroscience to study "the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the social world". Social cognitive neuroscience uses the epistemological foundations of cognitive neuroscience, and is closely related to social neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience employs human neuroimaging, typically using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Human brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation are also used. In nonhuman animals, direct electrophysiological recordings and electrical stimulation of single cells and neuronal populations are utilized for investigating lower-level social cognitive processes.

References

  1. 1 2 3 Earl K. Miller publications indexed by Google Scholar OOjs UI icon edit-ltr-progressive.svg
  2. 1 2 Miller, Earl Keith (1990). Neurophysiological investigations of inferior temporal cortex of the macaque. princeton.edu (PhD thesis). Princeton University. OCLC   84015941.
  3. 1 2 Wallis, Jonathan D.; Anderson, Kathleen C.; Miller, Earl K. (2001). "Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules". Nature. 411 (6840): 953–956. Bibcode:2001Natur.411..953W. doi:10.1038/35082081. ISSN   0028-0836. PMID   11418860. S2CID   4366539.
  4. "Miller Lab". Miller Lab.
  5. "Brain and Cognitive Sciences". bcs.mit.edu. Archived from the original on July 4, 2018. Retrieved June 3, 2016.
  6. http://web.mit.edu/picower/faculty/miller.html Miller's page at the Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
  7. Earl Miller Playlist Appearance on WMBR's Dinnertime Sampler Archived May 4, 2011, at the Wayback Machine radio show April 6, 2005
  8. 1 2 "SplitSage". SplitSage.
  9. "Nexus of Neuroscience, Engineering, Marketing". NeuroFocus. Archived from the original on October 5, 2011. Retrieved October 20, 2011.
  10. "The Executive Brain and Decision-Making". YouTube. Retrieved October 20, 2011.
  11. Freedman, D.J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Miller, E.K. (2001) Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 291:312–316.
  12. Nieder, A., Freedman, D.J., and Miller, E.K. (2002) Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297:1708–1711.
  13. Buschman, T.J. and Miller, E.K. (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315: 1860–1862.
  14. Pasupathy, A. and Miller, E.K. (2005) Different time courses for learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature, 433:873–876.
  15. Rigotti, Mattia; Barak, Omri; Warden, Melissa R.; Wang, Xiao-Jing; Daw, Nathaniel D.; Miller, Earl K.; Fusi, Stefano (May 2013). "The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks". Nature. 497 (7451): 585–590. Bibcode:2013Natur.497..585R. doi:10.1038/nature12160. ISSN   0028-0836. PMC   4412347 . PMID   23685452.
  16. Fusi, Stefano; Miller, Earl K.; Rigotti, Mattia (April 1, 2016). "Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognition". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 37: 66–74. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.010. ISSN   0959-4388. PMID   26851755. S2CID   13897721.
  17. Miller, E.K., and Wilson, M.A. (2008) All my circuits: Using multiple-electrodes to understand functioning neural networks. Neuron 60:483–488
  18. Buschman, T.J. and Miller, E.K. (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 315: 1860–1862, Pasupathy, A. and Miller, E.K. (2005) Different time courses for learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature, 433:873–876., Freedman, D.J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Miller, E.K (2003) A comparison of primate prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices during visual categorization. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12):5235–5246.
  19. Buschman, T.J. and Miller, E.K. (2009) Serial, covert, shifts of attention during visual search are reflected by the frontal eye fields and correlated with population oscillations. Neuron, 63: 386–396.Buschman, T.J. and Miller, E.K. (2009) Serial, covert, shifts of attention during visual search are reflected by the frontal eye fields and correlated with population oscillations. Neuron, 63: 386–396.
  20. Siegel, M., Warden, M.R., and Miller, E.K. (2009) Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106: 21341-21346
  21. Vogel, E.K., Fukada, K. In mind and out of phase, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106:21017-21018
  22. Miller, Earl K.; Lundqvist, Mikael; Bastos, André M. (October 24, 2018). "Working Memory 2.0". Neuron. 100 (2): 463–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023 . ISSN   0896-6273. PMC   8112390 . PMID   30359609.
  23. Bastos, A.M., Lundqvist, M., Waite, A.S., Kopell, N. and Miller, E.K. (2020) Layer and rhythm specificity for predictive routing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Published November 23, 2020; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2014868117
  24. Brincat, S.L, Donoghue, J.A., Mahnke, M.K., Kornblith, S., Lundqvist, M. and Miller, E.K. (2021) Interhemispheric transfer of working memories. Neuron. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2021.01.016
  25. Bastos, A.M., Donoghue, J.A., Brincat, S.L., Mahnke, M., Yanar, J., Correa, J., Waite, A.S., Lundqvist, M., Roy, J., Brown, E.N. and Miller, E.K. (2021). Neural effects of propofol-induced unconsciousness and its reversal using thalamic stimulation. eLife, DOI: 10.7554/eLife.60824.
  26. Miller, E.K. and Cohen, J.D. (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24:167–202.
  27. "April 2008 – Current Classics". ScienceWatch.com. Retrieved October 20, 2011.
  28. Yeung, Andy W. K.; Goto, Tazuko K.; Leung, W. Keung (September 11, 2018). "At the Leading Front of Neuroscience: A Bibliometric Study of the 100 Most-Cited Articles". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 11: 363. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00363 . PMC   5520389 . PMID   28785211.
  29. Buschman, T.J. and Miller, E.K. (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 315: 1860–1862.
  30. Akst, Jef. "Cortical crosstalk – The Scientist – Magazine of the Life Sciences". The Scientist. Archived from the original on July 6, 2010. Retrieved October 20, 2011.
  31. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Boyack, Kevin W.; Baas, Jeroen (2020). "Updated science-wide author databases of standardized citation indicators". PLOS Biology. 18 (10): e3000918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000918 . PMC   7567353 . PMID   33064726.
  32. "Eleven from MIT elected to American Academy of Arts and Sciences for 2017".
  33. "Earl Miller receives Goldman-Rakic Prize for Outstanding Achievement in Cognitive Neuroscience".
  34. Kent State University Alumni Association (October 5, 2015). "2015 Professional Achievement Award - Earl Miller, '85" via YouTube.
  35. "Bose grants reward risk".
  36. "404". www.sfn.org.{{cite web}}: Cite uses generic title (help)
  37. "Troland Research Awards". www.nasonline.org.
  38. "Directory of Scholars". www.pewtrusts.org.
  39. "Home". splitsage.com.
  40. Piore, Adam (October 2016). "Attention, Please: Earl Miller Wants to Make Us All Smarter". Discovery Magazine.
  41. "The Eureka Hunt". The New Yorker . July 21, 2008.
  42. "Here's Why You Shouldn't Multitask, According to an MIT Neuroscientist". Fortune. December 2016.
  43. "This is your brain on Facebook, Twitter, Instagram other digital platforms".
  44. "Take it easy–The importance of being lazy".
  45. Heid, Markham (June 2017). "You Asked: How Can I Use More of My Brain?". Time.
  46. Stahl, Ashley (October 2017). "4 Ways To Be More Productive At Work". Forbes.
  47. "In the News". March 27, 2013.
  48. "ALUMNUS DR. EARL K. MILLER AWARDS $2 MILLION GIFT TO NEUROSCIENCE AND PSYCHOLOGY PROGRAMS AT KENT STATE UNIVERSITY". Kent State University. Retrieved May 1, 2021.