Music therapy for non-fluent aphasia

Last updated

Music therapy for non-fluent aphasia is a method for treating patients who have lost the ability to speak after a stroke or accident. Non-fluent aphasia, also called expressive aphasia, is a neurological disorder that deprives patients of the ability to express language. It is usually caused by stroke or lesions in Broca's area, which is a language-dominant area that is responsible for speech production located in the left hemisphere of the brain. However, when lesions form in Broca's area this only affects patients’ speech ability, while their ability to sing remains unaffected. [1] [2] Since several studies have shown that right hemispheric regions are more active during singing, [3] [4] [5] music therapy involving melodic elements is deemed to be a potential treatment for non-fluent aphasia, as singing might activate patients’ right hemisphere to compensate with their lesioned left hemisphere. [6] [7] Aside from singing, many other music therapy techniques have also been attempted such as rhythms and poetic emphasis, this may add to the effectiveness of some techniques is also shown. Although there are many possible explanations for the mechanism of music therapy, the underlying mechanism remains unclear, as some studies indicate contradictory results. [8] [9] [10] [11]

Contents

Techniques

Therapeutic singing

A common method of therapeutic singing is guiding patients to sing familiar songs. [12] Patients are led to sing these familiar songs along with their therapists. Each therapist needs to be cognoscente of what would be familiar to the patients. Having someone in their 70's be asked to sing a Taylor Swift song, would not be deemed appropriate. The therapist also might need to adjust the tempo, volume, and pitch of the songs according to each individual patient. By doing this, it helps someone who might be a struggling to formulate thoughts and words quickly have time to catch up. This can optimize the therapy process. The lyrics that patients can most easily produce are repeated several times. [13] The utilization of singing during the treatment of aphasia is not a novel therapy technique, as in the past therapists already noticed that patients who had difficulty speaking were still able to sing. [14]

Vocal intonation

To begin is it needed to define what vocal Intonation. In its simplest terms, it is the rise and fall of voicing at the end of questions, and or statements to convey a meaning, or a mood and anything else that will help the listeners understand what your intentions are. [15] Vocal intonation is introduced to make aphasic patients intentionally exaggerate the intonation of daily phrases. [12] The most well-known application of vocal intonation in music therapy is Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT). MIT was developed when researchers noticed a pattern which spanned 100 years of observation, it showed patients with aphasia were far better able to sing then to speak in sentences. [7]

When receiving MIT, patients intone phrases with 2–3 syllables on only two pitches, which are determined by the natural prosody of phrases. [16] Stressed syllables are intoned on a higher pitch, while unstressed syllables are intoned on a lower pitch. However, various intonation in daily conversational phrases may be used, so patients can practice communicating different meanings through different intonation. [13] In addition, body cues, such as head or hand movement, may be used to help patients perceive different pitch in vocal intonation. [12]

Dynamically cued singing

Dynamically cued singing was also used by therapists a long time ago. [14] Therapists will guide patients to finish an incomplete sentence by singing the name of a designated object, such as “hand” if a therapist points to a patient’s hand. Additionally, dynamically cued singing can be employed together with singing familiar songs. [12] Therapists may pause at the end of each phrase of a song to let patients finish the phrase by singing. This technique creates a strong interpersonal interaction that not only enhances patients’ motivation to participate, but also offers a circumstance similar to real conversation that benefits patients’ skills outside of music. [13]

Breathing into syllable sound

At beginning, therapists will ask patients to focus on breathing single-syllable sounds only. [12] The effect of this technique is enhanced if therapists can guide patients to produce the sounds with their natural breathing patterns that help them relax, such as yawning, signing, or cleaning voices. [13] Later on, patients will be encouraged to develop the breathing sounds into pitched sounds. Hand movement may be used to help patients synchronize syllable sounds with exhaling. [13] Usually, the single-syllable sounds begin from vowels to consonants. But it is also suggested that the procedure should be from bilabial sounds (i.e. /m/, /b/), tongue-tip sounds (i.e. /n/, /d/), to tongue-base sounds (i.e. /g/, /k/). [12]

Musically assisted speech

Patients will produce conversational phrases with melodies that the patients are familiar with to practice the real conversation that the patients may need in their daily life. [12] This technique leads to more successful outcomes if original lyrics of the melodies are presented before the melodies are associated with conversational phrases. [13] The method was also conducted by therapists more than 40 years ago and indicated to be effective. Keith and Aronson [14] had recorded that their stroke patient rehabilitated from aphasia by singing simple conversational phrases to communicate with people.

Rhythmic speech cueing

Rhythmic speech cueing is one of the essential components in MIT, which requires patients to use their left hand to tap each syllable of the intoned phrases. [7] In general, therapists will guide patients to clap or tap the speech rhythm to phrases obtained from song lyrics, conversations, or any resources related to the immediate context. [13] Instead of left-hand tapping, clapping or beating a drum are also possible alternatives. [12] Rhythmic cueing speed, which has been shown to be more crucial then the melody, will correspond to the patients’ speech speed, prosodic rhythms of the phrases, or musical rhythms of the lyrics, depending on the circumstances of each individual and the types of phrases. [17]

Oral motor exercises

This technique aims to improve the oral motor formation of patients. [12] Therapists will demonstrate their mouth and tongue movements in an exaggerated way when singing a song, and ask patients to imitate the exaggerated oral movements. Patients tend to have better performance if therapists coordinate their exaggerated expressions to match the rhythm of the lyrics that are being practiced. [13] Feedback will be given by therapists to allow patients to imitate the movements as identically as possible by repeated practices and correction.

Inner rehearsal

When employing this technique, therapists first hum a melody, and then sing words with the melody while tapping patients’ hand. Therapists will ask patients to hear the words with melody "inside". If patients have difficulty understanding how to do it, they will be asked to imagine hearing the melody of "Happy Birthday" or their parents’ voice saying "Do your homework". [16] After patients understand the concept, they will be asked to hear themselves singing the words inside their brain along with therapists when therapists are singing to them. Through this technique, patients can establish an auditory target to compare the overtly produced phrases to their covertly produced phrases. Mastering this skill, patients will be able to create their own auditory target phrases, and transfer these phrases into expressive speech without much assistance from therapists. [16]

Auditory-motor feedback training

The purpose of this technique is the same as that of inner rehearsal: to allow patients to gain independence from the assistance of therapists. [16] The technique aims to train patients to hear the difference between the target phrases sung by therapists and the phrases produced by themselves. The target phrases can be those they create by using inner rehearsal. After mastering this technique, patients will be able to correct their production of phrases on their own whenever they practice singing words.

Benefits

The effectiveness of traditional speech therapy tends to be limited by patients’ poor attention and poor motivation, while music therapy can have better therapeutical effects by boosting patients’ willingness to participate in therapy process. [13] Music therapy can also improve non-fluent aphasic patients’ breathing and vocal ability, articulation and prosody of speech, and verbal and non-verbal communicative behaviours. [13]

In addition, music therapy gives confidence to non-fluent aphasic patients and reduces their frustrated feelings. At first, non-fluent aphasic patients may find it difficult to accept the fact that they cannot speak as they previously did anymore and suffer from depression. [18] Nevertheless, once the patients discover that they are still able to sing some sentences after receiving music therapy, they will be more confident and less frustrated. [19] Moreover, as words in singing are pronounced more slowly than in speaking, patients can be more fluent when singing words because they have more time to distinguish the phonemes composed of those words. [20]

Mechanisms

Although people who suffer from non-fluent aphasia lose their speech ability, their ability to sing is often preserved. [21] Their preserved ability to sing might result from their preserved brain circuit for singing lyrics, [22] which suggests that singing and speaking involve different neural pathway in human brain. [23] As non-fluent aphasia is usually caused by lesions in patients’ left hemisphere, the undamaged right hemisphere is regarded by researchers as the reason why patients preserve their ability to sing (Jeffries et al., 2003). Patients who undergo brain imaging scan also show an increased right hemispheric activity when they are singing than speaking. [6] [7] Therefore, the substitution of the right hemisphere for the damaged left hemisphere is one of the possible mechanisms of music therapy for non-fluent aphasia. [7]

In terms of the training of the right hemisphere, music therapy techniques involving melodic components, such as singing and vocal intonation, are often a topic of discussion by researchers. Since pitch difference in language is perceived by the right hemisphere, singing or intoning phrases may allow patients to use the neural pathway in their right hemisphere. [24] As many patients have been noted to have an improvement in speech production after receiving therapeutic singing or vocal intonation practice, some researchers have suggested these techniques are able to activate their right hemispheric regions which have been potentially shown as being responsible for language processing. [7]

Although many studies suggest that singing in music therapy can improve non-fluent aphasic patients’ speech production, the study by Stahl et al. [25] shows that rhythm, instead of singing, is the key element in music therapy that benefits aphasic patients. Therefore, rhythmic components in music therapy might explain why music therapy can benefit non-fluent aphasic patients. One example is the left-hand tapping used in Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT). Since this technique also serves as a way to stimulate the right hemisphere, Schlaug et al. [7] speculate that left-hand tapping may favor verbal production indirectly. They further propose that hand movements and articulation might share a mutual neurological network in the right hemisphere because hand movements are often in a tight relationship with one's articulation in daily life. Therefore, left-hand tapping is likely to benefit aphasic patients' articulation by engaging the neurological network that coordinates both hand movement and articulatory movement in patients’ right hemisphere.[ citation needed ]

Challenges for future studies

Even though many studies have proposed the possible neural mechanism of music therapy for non-fluent aphasia, many crucial questions are still unanswered. For example, in contrast to the idea that non-fluent aphasic patients’ ability to sing is due to their undamaged right hemisphere, [3] other studies suggest that speaking and singing words are based on the same neural mechanisms. [1] [26] Therefore, patients’ better performance on singing words might result from their long-term memories for familiar lyrics. [23]

In addition, in spite of some research results indicating that MIT can activate regions potentially responsible for language processing in aphasic patients’ right hemisphere, some studies obtain completely opposite results. [8] [9] [10] Belin et al.’s study [8] showed that Broca's area and the adjacent regions in aphasic patients’ left hemisphere were activated after patients received MIT, while no activation was observed in the right hemisphere. Contrarily, they observed activation in patients’ right hemisphere when patients underwent word repetition tasks without MIT; they interpreted the right hemispheric activation as the persistence of aphasia, instead of cortical reorganization. Unfortunately, these currently conflicting research results demonstrate that the underlying mechanism of music therapy remains unclear. If the neural mechanism of music therapy cannot be elucidated, the effective application of these techniques will remain hard to achieve. [11]

Another challenge for the research on music therapy is how to design the studies. [11] Although many studies have illustrated positive effects of music therapy on non-fluent aphasia, these studies tend to be case studies with small sample size. Moreover, these studies usually lack control conditions and randomized design. Therefore, Van der Meulen et al. [11] strongly urge in their article that Randomized Controlled Trials (RCTs) are needed to examine the effectiveness of music therapy.[ citation needed ]

Sustaining the treatment effectiveness of music therapy over the long term is also a challenge for its application on non-fluent aphasia. To overcome this barrier, two techniques, inner rehearsal and auditory-motor feedback training, are introduced to help patients gain independence from the assistance of music therapists. [16] However, related studies of the two techniques are still limited. Future studies that are able to take in larger sample populations, to compare the many different results are required to fully understand the efficacy and potential applications of the two techniques.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Aphasia</span> Inability to comprehend or formulate language

In aphasia, a person may be unable to comprehend or unable to formulate language because of damage to specific brain regions. The major causes are stroke and head trauma; prevalence is hard to determine but aphasia due to stroke is estimated to be 0.1–0.4% in the Global North. Aphasia can also be the result of brain tumors, epilepsy, autoimmune neurological diseases, brain infections, or neurodegenerative diseases.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Expressive aphasia</span> Language disorder involving inability to produce language

Expressive aphasia, also known as Broca's aphasia, is a type of aphasia characterized by partial loss of the ability to produce language, although comprehension generally remains intact. A person with expressive aphasia will exhibit effortful speech. Speech generally includes important content words but leaves out function words that have more grammatical significance than physical meaning, such as prepositions and articles. This is known as "telegraphic speech". The person's intended message may still be understood, but their sentence will not be grammatically correct. In very severe forms of expressive aphasia, a person may only speak using single word utterances. Typically, comprehension is mildly to moderately impaired in expressive aphasia due to difficulty understanding complex grammar.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Receptive aphasia</span> Language disorder involving inability to understand language

Wernicke's aphasia, also known as receptive aphasia, sensory aphasia, fluent aphasia, or posterior aphasia, is a type of aphasia in which individuals have difficulty understanding written and spoken language. Patients with Wernicke's aphasia demonstrate fluent speech, which is characterized by typical speech rate, intact syntactic abilities and effortless speech output. Writing often reflects speech in that it tends to lack content or meaning. In most cases, motor deficits do not occur in individuals with Wernicke's aphasia. Therefore, they may produce a large amount of speech without much meaning. Individuals with Wernicke's aphasia are typically unaware of their errors in speech and do not realize their speech may lack meaning. They typically remain unaware of even their most profound language deficits.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Broca's area</span> Speech production region in the dominant hemisphere of the hominid brain

Broca's area, or the Broca area, is a region in the frontal lobe of the dominant hemisphere, usually the left, of the brain with functions linked to speech production.

Aphasiology is the study of language impairment usually resulting from brain damage, due to neurovascular accident—hemorrhage, stroke—or associated with a variety of neurodegenerative diseases, including different types of dementia. These specific language deficits, termed aphasias, may be defined as impairments of language production or comprehension that cannot be attributed to trivial causes such as deafness or oral paralysis. A number of aphasias have been described, but two are best known: expressive aphasia and receptive aphasia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Music therapy</span> Health profession

Music therapy, an allied health profession, "is the clinical and evidence-based use of music interventions to accomplish individualized goals within a therapeutic relationship by a credentialed professional who has completed an approved music therapy program." It is also a vocation, involving a deep commitment to music and the desire to use it as a medium to help others. Although music therapy has only been established as a profession relatively recently, the connection between music and therapy is not new. In addition there are several researches of sound healing on different patients with positive outcome.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anomic aphasia</span> Medical condition

Anomic aphasia is a mild, fluent type of aphasia where individuals have word retrieval failures and cannot express the words they want to say. By contrast, anomia is a deficit of expressive language, and a symptom of all forms of aphasia, but patients whose primary deficit is word retrieval are diagnosed with anomic aphasia. Individuals with aphasia who display anomia can often describe an object in detail and maybe even use hand gestures to demonstrate how the object is used, but cannot find the appropriate word to name the object. Patients with anomic aphasia have relatively preserved speech fluency, repetition, comprehension, and grammatical speech.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wernicke's area</span> Speech comprehension region in the dominant hemisphere of the hominid brain

Wernicke's area, also called Wernicke's speech area, is one of the two parts of the cerebral cortex that are linked to speech, the other being Broca's area. It is involved in the comprehension of written and spoken language, in contrast to Broca's area, which is primarily involved in the production of language. It is traditionally thought to reside in Brodmann area 22, which is located in the superior temporal gyrus in the dominant cerebral hemisphere, which is the left hemisphere in about 95% of right-handed individuals and 70% of left-handed individuals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Conduction aphasia</span> Medical condition

Conduction aphasia, also called associative aphasia, is an uncommon form of difficulty in speaking (aphasia). It is caused by damage to the parietal lobe of the brain. An acquired language disorder, it is characterised by intact auditory comprehension, coherent speech production, but poor speech repetition. Affected people are fully capable of understanding what they are hearing, but fail to encode phonological information for production. This deficit is load-sensitive as the person shows significant difficulty repeating phrases, particularly as the phrases increase in length and complexity and as they stumble over words they are attempting to pronounce. People have frequent errors during spontaneous speech, such as substituting or transposing sounds. They are also aware of their errors and will show significant difficulty correcting them.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Global aphasia</span> Medical condition

Global aphasia is a severe form of nonfluent aphasia, caused by damage to the left side of the brain, that affects receptive and expressive language skills as well as auditory and visual comprehension. Acquired impairments of communicative abilities are present across all language modalities, impacting language production, comprehension, and repetition. Patients with global aphasia may be able to verbalize a few short utterances and use non-word neologisms, but their overall production ability is limited. Their ability to repeat words, utterances, or phrases is also affected. Due to the preservation of the right hemisphere, an individual with global aphasia may still be able to express themselves through facial expressions, gestures, and intonation. This type of aphasia often results from a large lesion of the left perisylvian cortex. The lesion is caused by an occlusion of the left middle cerebral artery and is associated with damage to Broca's area, Wernicke's area, and insular regions which are associated with aspects of language.

Transcortical sensory aphasia (TSA) is a kind of aphasia that involves damage to specific areas of the temporal lobe of the brain, resulting in symptoms such as poor auditory comprehension, relatively intact repetition, and fluent speech with semantic paraphasias present. TSA is a fluent aphasia similar to Wernicke's aphasia, with the exception of a strong ability to repeat words and phrases. The person may repeat questions rather than answer them ("echolalia").

Amusia is a musical disorder that appears mainly as a defect in processing pitch but also encompasses musical memory and recognition. Two main classifications of amusia exist: acquired amusia, which occurs as a result of brain damage, and congenital amusia, which results from a music-processing anomaly present since birth.

Transcortical motor aphasia (TMoA), also known as commissural dysphasia or white matter dysphasia, results from damage in the anterior superior frontal lobe of the language-dominant hemisphere. This damage is typically due to cerebrovascular accident (CVA). TMoA is generally characterized by reduced speech output, which is a result of dysfunction of the affected region of the brain. The left hemisphere is usually responsible for performing language functions, although left-handed individuals have been shown to perform language functions using either their left or right hemisphere depending on the individual. The anterior frontal lobes of the language-dominant hemisphere are essential for initiating and maintaining speech. Because of this, individuals with TMoA often present with difficulty in speech maintenance and initiation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mixed transcortical aphasia</span>

Mixed transcortical aphasia is the least common of the three transcortical aphasias. This type of aphasia can also be referred to as "Isolation Aphasia". This type of aphasia is a result of damage that isolates the language areas from other brain regions. Broca's, Wernicke's, and the arcuate fasiculus are left intact; however, they are isolated from other brain regions.

Semantic dementia (SD), also known as semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA), is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder characterized by loss of semantic memory in both the verbal and non-verbal domains. However, the most common presenting symptoms are in the verbal domain. Semantic dementia is a disorder of semantic memory that causes patients to lose the ability to match words or images to their meanings. However, it is fairly rare for patients with semantic dementia to develop category specific impairments, though there have been documented cases of it occurring. Typically, a more generalized semantic impairment results from dimmed semantic representations in the brain.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Primary progressive aphasia</span> Medical condition

Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a type of neurological syndrome in which language capabilities slowly and progressively become impaired. As with other types of aphasia, the symptoms that accompany PPA depend on what parts of the left hemisphere are significantly damaged. However, unlike most other aphasias, PPA results from continuous deterioration in brain tissue, which leads to early symptoms being far less detrimental than later symptoms.

Dysprosody, which may manifest as pseudo-foreign accent syndrome, refers to a disorder in which one or more of the prosodic functions are either compromised or eliminated.

Paraphasia is a type of language output error commonly associated with aphasia, and characterized by the production of unintended syllables, words, or phrases during the effort to speak. Paraphasic errors are most common in patients with fluent forms of aphasia, and come in three forms: phonemic or literal, neologistic, and verbal. Paraphasias can affect metrical information, segmental information, number of syllables, or both. Some paraphasias preserve the meter without segmentation, and some do the opposite. However, most paraphasias affect both partially.

Aprosodia is a neurological condition characterized by the inability of a person to properly convey or interpret emotional prosody. Prosody in language refers to the ranges of rhythm, pitch, stress, intonation, etc. These neurological deficits can be the result of damage of some form to the non-dominant hemisphere areas of language production. The prevalence of aprosodias in individuals is currently unknown, as testing for aprosodia secondary to other brain injury is only a recent occurrence.

Apraxia of speech (AOS), also called verbal apraxia, is a speech sound disorder affecting an individual's ability to translate conscious speech plans into motor plans, which results in limited and difficult speech ability. By the definition of apraxia, AOS affects volitional movement pattern. However, AOS usually also affects automatic speech.

References

  1. 1 2 Hébert, S., Racette, A., Gagnon, L., & Peretz, I. (2003). Revisiting the dissociation between singing and speaking in expressive aphasia. Brain, 126(8), 1838–1850. doi:10.1093/brain/awg186
  2. Racette, A., Bard, C., & Peretz, I. (2006). Making non-fluent aphasics speak: sing along! Brain, 129(10), 2571–2584. doi:10.1093/brain/awl250
  3. 1 2 Jeffries, K. J., Fritz, J. B., & Braun, a R. (2003). Words in melody: an H(2)15O PET study of brain activation during singing and speaking. NeuroReport, 14(5), 749–754. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000066198.94941.a4
  4. Ozdemir, E., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2006). Shared and distinct neural correlates of singing and speaking. NeuroImage, 33(2), 628–635. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.013
  5. Callan, D. E., Tsytsarev, V., Hanakawa, T., Callan, A. M., Katsuhara, M., Fukuyama, H., & Turner, R. (2006). Song and speech: brain regions involved with perception and covert production. NeuroImage, 31(3), 1327–1342. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.036
  6. 1 2 Zipse, L., Norton, a, Marchina, S., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Singing versus Speaking in Nonfluent Aphasia. NeuroImage, 47, S119. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(09)71121-8
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Schlaug, Gottfried; Norton, Andrea; Marchina, Sarah; Zipse, Lauryn; Wan, Catherine Y (September 2010). "From singing to speaking: facilitating recovery from nonfluent aphasia". Future Neurology. 5 (5): 657–665. doi:10.2217/fnl.10.44. ISSN   1479-6708. PMC   2982746 . PMID   21088709.
  8. 1 2 3 Belin, P., Zilbovicius, M., Remy, P., Francois, C., Guillaume, S., Chain, F., Samson, Y. (1996). Recovery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation therapy: A PET study. Neurology, 47(6), 1504–1511. doi:10.1212/WNL.47.6.1504
  9. 1 2 Breier, J. I., Randle, S., Maher, L. M., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2010). Changes in maps of language activity activation following melodic intonation therapy using magnetoencephalography: two case studies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 309–314. doi:10.1080/13803390903029293
  10. 1 2 Van de Sandt-Koenderman, M., Smits, M., van der Meulen, I., Visch-Brink, E., van der Lugt, A., & Ribbers, G. (2010). A Case Study of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) in the Subacute Stage of Aphasia: Early Re-re activation of Left Hemisphere Structures. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 241–243. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.121
  11. 1 2 3 4 Van der Meulen, I., van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. E., & Ribbers, G. M. (2012). Melodic Intonation Therapy: present controversies and future opportunities. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 93(1), S46–S52. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.05.029
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Kim, M., & Tomaino, C. M. (2008). Protocol evaluation for effective music therapy for persons with nonfluent aphasia. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 15(6), 555–569. doi:10.1310/tsr1506-555
  13. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Tomaino, C. M. (2012). Effective music therapy techniques in the treatment of nonfluent aphasia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 312–317. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06451.x
  14. 1 2 3 Keith, R. L., & Aronson, A. E. (1975). Singing as therapy for apraxia of speech and aphasia: Report of a case. Brain and Language, 2(4), 483–488. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(75)80085-X
  15. Nordquist, Richard (August 27, 2020). [Nordquist, Richard. (2020, August 27). Intonation Definition and Examples in Speech. Retrieved from https://www.thoughtco.com/intonation-speech-term-1691184 "Intonation Definition and Examples in Speech"].{{cite web}}: Check |url= value (help)
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 Norton, A., Zipse, L., Marchina, S., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Melodic intonation therapy: shared insights on how it is done and why it might help. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 431–436. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04859.x
  17. Hausen, Maija; Torppa, Ritva; Salmela, Viljami R.; Vainio, Martti; Särkämö, Teppo (2013). "Music and speech prosody: a common rhythm". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 566. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00566 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   3759063 . PMID   24032022.
  18. Parr, S. (1994). Coping with aphasia: Conversations with 20 aphasic people. Aphasiology, 8(5), 457–466. doi:10.1080/02687039408248670
  19. Conklyn, D., Novak, E., Boissy, A., Bethoux, F., & Chemali, K. (2012). The effects of modified melodic intonation therapy on nonfluent aphasia: a pilot study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(5), 1463–1471. Retrieved from http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1782681
  20. Lim, K.-B., Kim, Y.-K., Lee, H.-J., Yoo, J., Hwang, J. Y., Kim, J.-A., & Kim, S.-K. (2013). The therapeutic effect of neurologic music therapy and speech language therapy in post-stroke aphasic patients. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine, 37(4), 556–562. doi:10.5535/arm.2013.37.4.556
  21. Warren, J. D., Warren, J. E., Fox, N. C., & Warrington, E. K. (2003). Nothing to say, something to sing: primary progressive dynamic aphasia. Neurocase, 9(2), 140–155. doi:10.1076/neur.9.2.140.15068
  22. Samson, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (1991). Recognition memory for text and melody of songs after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: Evidence for dual encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 793–804. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.4.793
  23. 1 2 Straube, T., Schulz, A., Geipel, K., Mentzel, H.-J., & Miltner, W. H. R. (2008). Dissociation between singing and speaking in expressive aphasia: the role of song familiarity. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1505–1512. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.008
  24. Loui, P., Li, H. C., & Schlaug, G. (2011). White matter integrity in right hemisphere predicts pitch-related grammar learning. NeuroImage, 55(2), 500–507. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.022
  25. Stahl, B., Kotz, S. a, Henseler, I., Turner, R., & Geyer, S. (2011). Rhythm in disguise: why singing may not hold the key to recovery from aphasia. Brain, 134(Pt 10), 3083–3093. doi:10.1093/brain/awr240
  26. Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., Hébert, S., & Macoir, J. (2004). Singing in the Brain: Insights from Cognitive Neuropsychology. Music Perception, 21(3), 373–391. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.373