Private speech

Last updated

Private speech is speech spoken to oneself for communication, self-guidance, and self-regulation of behaviour. [1] [2] [3] Children can be observed engaging in private speech between the ages of two and seven. [1] [2] [3] Although it is audible, it is neither intended for nor directed at others. [4] [5] Private speech was first studied by Lev Vygotsky (1934/1986) and Jean Piaget (1959); in the past 30 years private speech has received renewed attention from researchers. [6] Researchers have noted a positive correlation between children's use of private speech and their task performance and achievement, [7] [8] [9] a fact also noted previously by Vygotsky. [2] It is when children begin school that their use of private speech decreases and "goes underground". [10]

Contents

History and theory

Private speech is typically observed in children from about two to seven years old. [1] [2] [3] Private speech or "self-talk" is observed speech spoken to oneself for communication, self-guidance, and self-regulation of behaviour. [2] Private speech is often thought to enhance the developing early literacy skills and help to increase a child's task performance, success, and achievement. [2] Numerous sources trace the first theories of private speech back to two early well-known developmental psychologists, Vygotsky and Piaget. [1] [3] Both of these psychologists mainly studied private speech in young children, yet they had different views and terms.

In 1923, Piaget published The Language and Thought of the Child. [4] In this book he recorded his observations of children talking to themselves in classrooms and termed the idea of self-talk as "egocentric speech", [2] which was the earliest concept of private speech. For Piaget egocentric speech was a sign of cognitive immaturity. [11] He thought egocentric speech would later develop into a fully mature and effective speech after a child gains a fair amount of cognitive and communicative skills. [2]

In Thought and Language, Vygotsky argued that egocentric speech was a part of normal development of communication, self-guidance, self-regulation of behaviour, planning, pacing, and monitoring skills. [12] Vygotsky explains that private speech stems from a child's social interactions as a toddler, then reaches a peak during preschool or kindergarten when children talk aloud to themselves. [13] Private speech serves as "the social/cultural tool or symbol system of language, first used for interpersonal communication is used by the child overtly not for communication with others, but for intrapersonal communication and self-guidance." [14] Private speech slows down in late elementary school years and children stop talking aloud, and instead start to use inner speech. [2]

Vygotsky's theory of private speech has been deemed noteworthy to more recent developmental psychologists, providing a stepping-stone[ colloquialism ] for over 75 years. [3] Berk, Winsler, Diaz, Montero, Neal, Amaya-Williams, and Wertsch are amongst some of the current well-known developmental psychologists and researchers who have been specialising in the field of private speech. [1] [3] [11] Although the concept dates back to the 1930s, private speech is still an emerging field in psychology with a vast amount of research opportunities. [12]

Benefits and uses for children

Evidence has supported Vygotsky's theory that private speech provides many developmental benefits for children. [11] Above all, private speech aids children in different types of self-guidance and self-regulation. [11] More specific uses and benefits of private speech are listed below.

Behavioural self-regulation and emotion regulation

Young children's behaviours are strongly influenced by the environment. [3] For instance, the presence of an interesting new toy in the preschool classroom is likely to draw a child's attention and influence his or her play. Private speech helps children to verbally guide their own behaviour and attention by helping them to detach themselves from stimuli in their environment. [3] As private speech is very important for children to engage in at early ages,[ according to whom? ] this speech should not be interrupted or limited by parental control. For example, a child may use private speech to direct themselves away from the distracting toy and toward the activity that the teacher told the child to do. Thus, private speech helps children to be less strongly influenced by their immediate environment and instead to self-control their behaviour. [2]

The relationship between private speech and behavioural self-regulation is further demonstrated by research showing that children use more private speech when asked to do more difficult tasks or when asked to do tasks without the help of a teacher or parent. [2] [11] In other words, in circumstances when more behavioural self-regulation is required of a young child, the child is more likely to use private speech. Private speech has also been linked to three-year-olds' ability to engage in task-related goals, when explicitly taught to use private speech as a strategy for this purpose. [2]

Young children also use private speech to help them regulate their emotions. [11] One way that children regulate their emotions and comfort themselves through private speech is by mimicking their parents' comforting speech. [11] For instance, a child may help themself calm down for sleep by repeating nighttime phrases that their parents have said to them previously to calm down. Young children who are better at controlling their emotions have also shown an increase in the amount of private speech they use. [11]

Memory, motivation, communication, and creativity

Private speech is used by children spontaneously and is a learned strategy to enhance memory. [12] Private speech is used as a repetitive strategy, to enhance working memory by maintaining information to be remembered. [2] For instance, a child might repeat a rule or story to themselves in order to remember it. Children also use private speech to aid their ability to suppress certain responses or information, and instead use other, less common responses or information, a process known as inhibitory control. [2]

By expressing goals, opinions, feelings, and self-thoughts through private speech, private speech increases children's motivation. [2] For instance, a child may talk themselves through a challenging task. This type of motivating private speech is associated with self-efficacy. [2] Moreover, children have been observed using motivational private speech especially during difficult tasks, and using motivational private speech is related to improved outcomes on the task. [2]

Some researchers have hypothesised that private speech helps young children to master speech communication, [12] by immersing themselves in speech more than they could with others. In doing so, children gain insight about their own communication abilities, practice communication, and build effective speech and communication skills. [12]

Children often use private speech during creative and imaginative play. [12] For instance, children often talk to themselves when playing imaginative and pretend games. Private speech is related to more creative play the more frequently children engage in private speech, the more creative, flexible, and original thought they display. [12]

Research

Current research[ timeframe? ] is becoming focused on use of private speech in the early childhood classroom setting and teachers' practices and attitudes regarding children's private speech. [11] Many studies have shown that preschool aged children engage in a considerable amount of overt private speech in their early childhood classrooms. [2] Specifically, researchers have found that children use more self-talk when they are busy with a goal-directed task activity (e.g., completing a puzzle). [3] It was also found that preschool aged children were least likely to use private speech in the presence of a teacher. [2]

Many methodological advancements and tools can now[ when? ] be used to better examine the role of speech in self-regulation. [12] With these advancements, there will be more research in the future on children's awareness of inner and private speech. [12] There is also a possibility that researchers will perform additional work on the early precursors of self-talk, early childhood interventions and better understanding the role language has on the formation of inner and private speech. [12]

In adults

In a self-reported questionnaire, young adults reported high levels of private speech, particularly when engaged in tasks with cognitive, mnemonic, and attentional components. [15] This suggests that private speech may be retained to some extent into adulthood, serving a similar purpose as it does in children. [15] Adults who stutter are much less likely to do so during private speech. [16]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Developmental psychology</span> Scientific study of psychological changes in humans over the course of their lives

Developmental psychology is the scientific study of how and why humans grow, change, and adapt across the course of their lives. Originally concerned with infants and children, the field has expanded to include adolescence, adult development, aging, and the entire lifespan. Developmental psychologists aim to explain how thinking, feeling, and behaviors change throughout life. This field examines change across three major dimensions, which are physical development, cognitive development, and social emotional development. Within these three dimensions are a broad range of topics including motor skills, executive functions, moral understanding, language acquisition, social change, personality, emotional development, self-concept, and identity formation.

Educational psychology is the branch of psychology concerned with the scientific study of human learning. The study of learning processes, from both cognitive and behavioral perspectives, allows researchers to understand individual differences in intelligence, cognitive development, affect, motivation, self-regulation, and self-concept, as well as their role in learning. The field of educational psychology relies heavily on quantitative methods, including testing and measurement, to enhance educational activities related to instructional design, classroom management, and assessment, which serve to facilitate learning processes in various educational settings across the lifespan.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lev Vygotsky</span> Soviet psychologist (1896–1934)

Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky was a Russian and Soviet psychologist, best known for his work on psychological development in children and creating the framework known as cultural-historical activity theory. After his early death, his books and research were banned in the Soviet Union until Joseph Stalin's death in 1953, with a first collection of major texts published in 1956.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jean Piaget</span> Swiss psychologist, biologist, logician, philosopher and academic

Jean William Fritz Piaget was a Swiss psychologist known for his work on child development. Piaget's theory of cognitive development and epistemological view are together called genetic epistemology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Intrapersonal communication</span> Communication with oneself

Intrapersonal communication is communication with oneself or self-to-self communication. Examples are thinking to oneself "I will do better next time" after having made a mistake or imagining a conversation with one's boss in preparation for leaving work early. It is often understood as an exchange of messages in which sender and receiver are the same person. Some theorists use a wider definition that goes beyond message-based accounts and focuses on the role of meaning and making sense of things. Intrapersonal communication can happen alone or in social situations. It may be prompted internally or occur as a response to changes in the environment.

Instructional scaffolding is the support given to a student by an instructor throughout the learning process. This support is specifically tailored to each student; this instructional approach allows students to experience student-centered learning, which tends to facilitate more efficient learning than teacher-centered learning. This learning process promotes a deeper level of learning than many other common teaching strategies.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Imaginary friend</span> Psychological and social phenomenon

Imaginary friends are a psychological and a social phenomenon where a friendship or other interpersonal relationship takes place in the imagination rather than physical reality.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Early childhood education</span> The teaching of children from birth to age eight

Early childhood education (ECE), also known as nursery education, is a branch of education theory that relates to the teaching of children from birth up to the age of eight. Traditionally, this is up to the equivalent of third grade. ECE is described as an important period in child development.

The psychology of learning refers to theories and research on how individuals learn. There are many theories of learning. Some take on a more behaviorist approach which focuses on inputs and reinforcements. Other approaches, such as neuroscience and social cognition, focus more on how the brain's organization and structure influence learning. Some psychological approaches, such as social constructivism, focus more on one's interaction with the environment and with others. Other theories, such as those related to motivation, like the growth mindset, focus more on individuals' perceptions of ability.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Piaget's theory of cognitive development</span> Theory that discusses human intelligence from an epistemological perspective

Piaget's theory of cognitive development, or his genetic epistemology, is a comprehensive theory about the nature and development of human intelligence. It was originated by the Swiss developmental psychologist Jean Piaget (1896–1980). The theory deals with the nature of knowledge itself and how humans gradually come to acquire, construct, and use it. Piaget's theory is mainly known as a developmental stage theory.

Cognitive development is a field of study in neuroscience and psychology focusing on a child's development in terms of information processing, conceptual resources, perceptual skill, language learning, and other aspects of the developed adult brain and cognitive psychology. Qualitative differences between how a child processes their waking experience and how an adult processes their waking experience are acknowledged. Cognitive development is defined as the emergence of the ability to consciously cognize, understand, and articulate their understanding in adult terms. Cognitive development is how a person perceives, thinks, and gains understanding of their world through the relations of genetic and learning factors. There are four stages to cognitive information development. They are, reasoning, intelligence, language, and memory. These stages start when the baby is about 18 months old, they play with toys, listen to their parents speak, they watch TV, anything that catches their attention helps build their cognitive development.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Egocentrism</span> Inability to differentiate between self and others

Egocentrism refers to difficulty differentiating between self and other. More specifically, it is difficulty in accurately perceiving and understanding perspectives other than one's own. Egocentrism is found across the life span: in infancy, early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Although egocentric behaviors are less prominent in adulthood, the existence of some forms of egocentrism in adulthood indicates that overcoming egocentrism may be a lifelong development that never achieves completion. Adults appear to be less egocentric than children because they are faster to correct from an initially egocentric perspective than children, not because they are less likely to initially adopt an egocentric perspective.

In psychology, centration is the tendency to focus on one salient aspect of a situation and neglect other, possibly relevant aspects. Introduced by the Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget through his cognitive-developmental stage theory, centration is a behaviour often demonstrated in the preoperational stage. Piaget claimed that egocentrism, a common element responsible for preoperational children's unsystematic thinking, was causal to centration. Research on centration has primarily been made by Piaget, shown through his conservation tasks, while contemporary researchers have expanded on his ideas.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Zone of proximal development</span> Difference between what a learner can do without help and what they can do with help

The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is a concept in educational psychology. It represents the space between what a learner is capable of doing unsupported and what the learner cannot do even with support. It is the range where the learner is able to perform, but only with support from a teacher or a peer with more knowledge or expertise. The concept was introduced, but not fully developed, by psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) during the last three years of his life. Vygotsky argued that a child gets involved in a dialogue with the "more knowledgeable other", such as a peer or an adult, and gradually, through social interaction and sense-making, develops the ability to solve problems independently and do certain tasks without help. Following Vygotsky, some educators believe that the role of education is to give children experiences that are within their zones of proximal development, thereby encouraging and advancing their individual learning skills and strategies.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Child development</span> Stages in the development of children

Child development involves the biological, psychological and emotional changes that occur in human beings between birth and the conclusion of adolescence. It is—particularly from birth to five years— a foundation for a prosperous and sustainable society.

Infant cognitive development is the first stage of human cognitive development, in the youngest children. The academic field of infant cognitive development studies of how psychological processes involved in thinking and knowing develop in young children. Information is acquired in a number of ways including through sight, sound, touch, taste, smell and language, all of which require processing by our cognitive system. However, cognition begins through social bonds between children and caregivers, which gradually increase through the essential motive force of Shared intentionality. The notion of Shared intentionality describes unaware processes during social learning at the onset of life when organisms in the simple reflexes substage of the sensorimotor stage of cognitive development do not maintain communication via the sensory system.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Childhood memory</span> Early life experiences often memorable for life

Childhood memory refers to memories formed during childhood. Among its other roles, memory functions to guide present behaviour and to predict future outcomes. Memory in childhood is qualitatively and quantitatively different from the memories formed and retrieved in late adolescence and the adult years. Childhood memory research is relatively recent in relation to the study of other types of cognitive processes underpinning behaviour. Understanding the mechanisms by which memories in childhood are encoded and later retrieved has important implications in many areas. Research into childhood memory includes topics such as childhood memory formation and retrieval mechanisms in relation to those in adults, controversies surrounding infantile amnesia and the fact that adults have relatively poor memories of early childhood, the ways in which school environment and family environment influence memory, and the ways in which memory can be improved in childhood to improve overall cognition, performance in school, and well-being, both in childhood and in adulthood.

In the framework of the Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (CHAT) the leading activity is the activity, or cooperative human action, which plays the most essential role in child development during a given developmental period. Although many activities may play a role in a child's development at any given time, the leading activity is theorized to be the type of social interaction that is most beneficial in terms of producing major developmental accomplishments, and preparing the child for the next period of development. Through engaging in leading activities, a child develops a wide range of capabilities, including emotional connection with others, motivation to engage in more complex social activities, the creation of new cognitive abilities, and the restructuring of old ones.

The Three Mountains Task was a task developed by Jean Piaget, a developmental psychologist from Switzerland. Piaget came up with a theory for developmental psychology based on cognitive development. Cognitive development, according to his theory, took place in four stages. These four stages were classified as the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational and formal operational stages. The Three Mountain Problem was devised by Piaget to test whether a child's thinking was egocentric, which was also a helpful indicator of whether the child was in the preoperational stage or the concrete operational stage of cognitive development.

Adam Winsler is a developmental psychologist known for his research on early child development, private speech, and benefits of arts education. Winsler is Professor of Applied Developmental Psychology at George Mason University.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Manning, B. H. (1991). Cognitive self-instruction for classroom processes . State University of New York Press. ISBN   0-471-92573-X.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). The MIT Press. ISBN   0-262-72010-8.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Winsler, A.; Diaz, R. M.; Montero, I. (1997). "The role of private speech in the transition from collaborative to independent task performance in young children". Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 12: 59–79. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(97)90043-0.
  4. 1 2 Piaget, J (1959). The language and thought of the child. Psychology Press. ISBN   0-415-26750-1.
  5. White, C. S.; Manning, B. H. (1994). "The effects of verbal scaffolding instruction on young children's private speech and problem- solving capabilities". Instructional Science. 22: 39–59. doi:10.1007/BF00889522. S2CID   145447942.
  6. Deniz, C. B. (2001a, April). History of research methods in private speech. Session presented at the annual meeting of the National Council on Measurement and Evaluation, Seattle, WA.
  7. Azmitia, M. (1992). Diaz, R.M.; Berk, L. E. (eds.). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 101–122. ISBN   0-8058-0886-8.
  8. Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children's private speech. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443-463.
  9. Lee, J. (1999). The effects of 5-year-old preschoolers' use of private speech on performance and attention for two kinds of problem solving tasks. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 06A. (UMI No. AAG99-32671) https://www.researchgate.net/publication/292089848_The_effects_of_five-year-old_preschoolers'_use_of_private_speech_on_performance_and_attention_for_two_kinds_of_problems-solving_tasks
  10. Kohlberg, Lawrence; Yaeger, Judy; Hjertholm, Else (1968). "Private Speech: Four Studies and a Review of Theories". Child Development . 39 (3): 691–736. doi:10.2307/1126979. ISSN   1467-8624. JSTOR   1126979. PMID   5687321.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Agres, J. (2012, September 29). Why do children talk to themselves? Retrieved from http://cla.calpoly.edu/~jbattenb/Papers/agres.html Archived 2015-05-15 at the Wayback Machine .
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Winsler, A.; Fernyhough, C.; Montero, I. (2009). Private Speech, Executive Functioning, and the Development of Verbal Self-regulation. Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-521-86607-1.
  13. Berk, L. E. (1999). "Children's Private Speech: An Overview of Theory and the Status of Research". In Lloyd, P (ed.). Critical Assessments: Thought and Language, Vol. II. Taylor & Frances/Routledge. pp. 33–70. ISBN   0-415-11153-6.
  14. Gholami, M.; Salehi, N.; Azizi, E.; Fazli, B. (2016). "Private speech and cognitive development: A review of the two theories" (PDF).
  15. 1 2 Duncan, Robert M.; Cheyne, J. Allan (1999). "Incidence and Functions of Self-Reported Private Speech in Young Adults: A Self-Verbalization Questionnaire". Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 31 (2): 133–136. doi:10.1037/h0087081.
  16. Jackson, Eric S.; Miller, Lindsay R.; Warner, Haley J.; Yaruss, J. Scott (2021). "Adults Who Stutter Do Not Stutter During Private Speech". Journal of Fluency Disorders. 70: 105878. doi:10.1016/j.jfludis.2021.105878. PMC   8629878 . PMID   34534899.