Anti-exhaustion hypothesis

Last updated

The anti-exhaustion hypothesis is a possible explanation for the existence of large repertoires and the song switching behaviour exhibited in birds. [1] This hypothesis states that muscle exhaustion occurring due to repeating song bouts can be avoided by switching to a different song in the bird's repertoire. [2] The anti-exhaustion hypothesis therefore predicts that birds with larger repertoires are less susceptible to exhaustion because they can readily change the song that they are producing. [3]

Contents

The anti-exhaustion hypothesis was first proposed by Marcel Lambrechts and André Dhondt in 1988 after they carried out a study using recordings from great tits, Parus major, during the dawn chorus. [4] There have been several studies carried out in which results have contradicted the anti-exhaustion hypothesis. Recent studies have shown that there is no evidence that the anti-exhaustion hypothesis is the cause of large repertoires in birds. [1] [5] Since the proposal of the anti-exhaustion hypothesis, several hypotheses have been proposed to explain the existence of repertoires and song switching behaviour in birds, including the motivation hypothesis and the warm-up hypothesis. [3] [2]

Anti-exhaustion hypothesis in great tits

Great tit (Parus major) Great tit - Parus major.jpg
Great tit (Parus major)
Audio clip of a great tit, changing its song at the 38s mark

The great tit, Parus major , is a passerine bird belonging to the family Paridae. Passerines are commonly referred to as songbirds, with most passerines singing multiple species-specific songs making a repertoire. [4] Birds can be ranked depending on how they perform the songs in their repertoire. On one side, there are birds that sing with eventual variety and have small repertoires, meaning that each song type in their repertoire is repeated before they switch to a different song type. [4]

On the other side, there are birds that sing with immediate variety and have larger repertoires, meaning that they switch song types continuously. [4] The great tit, in particular, sings with eventual variety and has a small repertoire, usually consisting of two to seven different song types. [4] Songs can be broken down into several simpler components. Songs are made up of bouts which last from about 30 seconds-600 seconds. [4]

A bout is a stereotyped repetition of one to five notes which are called a phrase. [4] Between two and 20 phrases are sung are short bursts which are called strophes. [4] In-between strophes are periods of silence, and this is referred to as the inter-strophe pause. [4] Therefore, a great tit sings several strophes of one song type before switching to a bout of another song type from their repertoire. [4]

Biologically, having a large repertoire is advantageous in territorial defence and larger repertoires are also correlated with higher reproductive success. [4] Marcel Lambrechts and André Dhondt proved that average strophe length and repertoire size can be used as proxies for male quality. [4] Male quality refers to the fitness of the bird, measuring how well it survives and its reproductive success. [4] Lambrechts and Dhondt set out to find the answers to four questions also pertaining to percentage performance time and male quality in the great tit. [4]

The first of four questions they sought out to determine was if high quality males, those with a higher fitness, have a higher percentage performance time in their bouts compared to lower quality males, with percentage performance time meaning the percentage of time during which song was being produced. [4] The second was if high quality males sing longer bouts than low quality males. [4] The third answer they sought was whether the percentage performance time changed or stayed the same during a bout. [4] The final question they sought the answer to was whether or not the percentage performance time changed after a bird switches song types. [4]

In order to determine the answer to the four proposed questions, Marcel and André recorded male great tits from 1983–1986 in two plots (L and B) in the Peerdsbos and another plot (U) on campus at the University of Antwerp in Wilrijk. [4] They came up with the anti-exhaustion hypothesis as an explanation for the results obtained from their study. [4] Their results showed that high quality males, as predicted, had a higher percentage performance time than low quality males. [4] Lambrechts and Dhondt also found that all great tits can show a systematic decrease in the percentage performance time during a bout, which is also known as drift. [4]

The finding that drove their hypothesis was that the males were able to recover a high percentage performance by switching song types. If the male was producing shorter strophes and having longer inter-strophe pauses (low percentage performance), then by switching to a different song type the bird would once again be able to produce longer strophes and have shorter inter-strophe pauses. [4] Lambrechts and Dhondt proposed the anti-exhaustion hypothesis, which provided both a functional and casual explanation for the song switching behaviour in birds along with having song repertoires. [4]

The anti-exhaustion hypothesis stated that when it is necessary for a bird to sing for a prolonged period of time at a high rate, it must continuously switch song types. [4] The hypothesis was focused around the idea that extended bouts of singing would lead to neuromuscular exhaustion because of the repetitive and stereotyped fashion of song bouts. [1] Marcel and André proposed that the male great tits would have longer inter-strophe pauses towards the end of a song due to this exhaustion. [4] By switching song types, the birds would be using alternative sound-producing muscles and nerves, therefore they would be able to recover a high percentage performance once again. [4]

Anti-exhaustion hypothesis in blue tits

Eurasian blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) Eurasian blue tit Lancashire.jpg
Eurasian blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus)
Audio of a blue tit, changing its song at the 28s mark

A study completed by Angelika Poesel and Bart Kempenaers (2002) was aimed at explaining drift during blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) song and among other Parus species and to also explain their findings in relation to the anti-exhaustion hypothesis and the motivation hypothesis. [3] They studied a group of 20 male blue tits at Kolbeterberg in Vienna, Austria that were living in mixed deciduous woods. [3] The results from their study showed that male blue tits did show a decrease in performance output (percentage performance time) the longer that they performed one song typed, which was illustrated by an increase in inter-strophe pauses. [3]

In order to confirm the anti-exhaustion hypothesis, the two factors proposed by Lambrechts and Dhondt that influenced a low percentage performance time needed to be confirmed. [3] These two factors are the initial level of song output (the greater the initial output, the greater the drift would be), and the number of switches between song types (after a switch in song types, performance output was increased). [3] The results from this study could conclude that a repeated stereotyped song is difficult to maintain over a long period of time, which supported the anti-exhaustive hypothesis. [3]

Motivation hypothesis

The motivation hypothesis was a competing hypothesis of the anti-exhaustion hypothesis. The motivation hypothesis, proposed by Weary in 1988, explained that drift may be due to a lack of motivation to keep singing the same song, not because of neuromuscular exhaustion. [3] This study also worked with great tits, playing song to them during the day. [3] Weary suggested that if drift was due to a lack of motivation, then if a bird was presented with the song of a rival, for example, then the bird should be able to increase its song output because of the motivational stimulus. [3]

Weary also argued that if drift was caused by neuromuscular exhaustion, then birds would not be able to increase song output if they did not switch song types, which was not the case in all birds. [3] Lambrechts argued back that the test completed by Weary was not appropriate, as it was completed during the day, not during the dawn chorus when song output is at its maximum. [3] In order to solve this conflict, Weary and Lambrechts got together and performed a series of tests during the day and during the dawn chorus testing the responsiveness to playback. [3]

Their results showed that both hypotheses were supported, with drift being more common during the dawn chorus (supporting the anti-exhaustive hypothesis), but also that the increase in song output was similar during periods of high and low output (supporting the motivation hypothesis). [3] These results concluded that the anti-exhaustive hypothesis and the motivation hypothesis are both possible and can both occur at the same time. [3]

Warm-up hypothesis

Adelaide's Warbler (Setophaga adelaidae) Adelaide's Warbler.jpg
Adelaide's Warbler (Setophaga adelaidae)

The warm-up hypothesis proposed by Schraft et al. (2016) seems to contradict the anti-exhaustion hypothesis . [2] This hypothesis foresees that birds have greater singing performance depending on the number of songs the bird has sung that day, known as recent practice, regardless of song type. [2] This study was carried out between March and June 2012 at the Cabo Rojo National Wildlife Refuge in Puerto Rico. [2] Male Adelaide's warblers, Setophaga adelaidae, recorded during their breeding season in their resident woods were found to have an average repertoire of 29 songs/male. [2]

Schraft et al. tested several hypotheses, each with a different prediction and a different independent variable. [2] The song type specific hypothesis predicted performance would decrease with consecutive repetitions of a song type, the independent variable being the run number. [2] This hypothesis is consistent with the anti-exhaustion hypothesis. The song type general hypothesis predicted performance would increase with a higher latency period between songs, the independent variable being latency. [2]

The warm-up hypothesis predicted performance would increase with number of songs sung, the independent variable being order. [2] The Type I singing showcases higher performance predicted Type I songs would have higher performance than Type II songs, the independent variable. [2] The vocal interaction hypothesis predicted performance would increase when countersinging. [2] It was also hypothesized that time-dependent factors influenced performance, the independent variable being time. [2]

The results from the study showed that singing performance improves with time throughout the morning. [2] This was explained only by the warm-up hypothesis, the cumulative number of songs that the bird had sung that morning. [2] The other hypotheses were found to have no effect on singing performance, including the song type specific hypothesis which is consistent with the anti-exhaustion hypothesis. [2] Schraft et al. proposed that the anti-exhaustion hypothesis and the warm-up hypothesis are not mutually exclusive as the birds that warm up still may need to switch song types because of fatigue. [2]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reciprocal altruism</span> Form of behaviour between organisms

In evolutionary biology, reciprocal altruism is a behaviour whereby an organism acts in a manner that temporarily reduces its fitness while increasing another organism's fitness, with the expectation that the other organism will act in a similar manner at a later time.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Songbird</span> Suborder of birds

A songbird is a bird belonging to the suborder Passeri of the perching birds (Passeriformes). Another name that is sometimes seen as the scientific or vernacular name is Oscines, from Latin oscen, "songbird". The Passeriformes contains 5,000 or so species found all over the world, in which the vocal organ typically is developed in such a way as to produce a diverse and elaborate bird song.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Snow bunting</span> Species of bird

The snow bunting is a passerine bird in the family Calcariidae. It is an Arctic specialist, with a circumpolar Arctic breeding range throughout the northern hemisphere. There are small isolated populations on a few high mountain tops south of the Arctic region, including the Cairngorms in central Scotland and the Saint Elias Mountains on the southern Alaska-Yukon border, as well as the Cape Breton Highlands. The snow bunting is the most northerly recorded passerine in the world.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Eurasian blue tit</span> Species of bird

The Eurasian blue tit is a small passerine bird in the tit family, Paridae. It is easily recognisable by its blue and yellow plumage and small size.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Great tit</span> Passerine bird in the tit family Paridae

The great tit is a passerine bird in the tit family Paridae. It is a widespread and common species throughout Europe, the Middle East, Central Asia and east across the Palearctic to the Amur River, south to parts of North Africa where it is generally resident in any sort of woodland; most great tits do not migrate except in extremely harsh winters. Until 2005 this species was lumped with numerous other subspecies. DNA studies have shown these other subspecies to be distinct from the great tit and these have now been separated as two distinct species, the cinereous tit of southern Asia, and the Japanese tit of East Asia. The great tit remains the most widespread species in the genus Parus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parrotbill</span> Group of birds

The parrotbills are a family, Paradoxornithidae, of passerine birds that are primarily native to East and Southeast Asia, though feral populations exist elsewhere. They are generally small, long-tailed birds that inhabit reedbeds and similar habitats. They feed mainly on seeds, e.g. of grasses, to which their bill, as the name implies, is well-adapted. Living in tropical to southern temperate climates, they are usually non-migratory.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Great reed warbler</span> Eurasian bird

The great reed warbler is a Eurasian in the passerine genus Acrocephalus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bird vocalization</span> Sounds birds use to communicate

Bird vocalization includes both bird calls and bird songs. In non-technical use, bird songs are the bird sounds that are melodious to the human ear. In ornithology and birding, songs are distinguished by function from calls.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Northern mockingbird</span> Species of bird

The northern mockingbird is a mockingbird commonly found in North America. This bird is mainly a permanent resident, but northern birds may move south during harsh weather. This species has rarely been observed in Europe. This species was first described by Carl Linnaeus in his 1758 10th edition of Systema Naturae as Turdus polyglottos. The northern mockingbird is known for its mimicking ability, as reflected by the meaning of its scientific name, "many-tongued mimic". The northern mockingbird has gray to brown upper feathers and a paler belly. Its tail and wings have white patches which are visible in flight.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Black-capped chickadee</span> Species of small, non-migratory, North American songbird

The black-capped chickadee is a small, nonmigratory, North American songbird that lives in deciduous and mixed forests. It is a passerine bird in the tit family, the Paridae. It is the state bird of Massachusetts and Maine in the United States, and the provincial bird of New Brunswick in Canada. It is well known for its ability to lower its body temperature during cold winter nights, its good spatial memory to relocate the caches where it stores food, and its boldness near humans.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Babbling</span> Stage in child development and language acquisition

Babbling is a stage in child development and a state in language acquisition during which an infant appears to be experimenting with uttering articulate sounds, but does not yet produce any recognizable words. Babbling begins shortly after birth and progresses through several stages as the infant's repertoire of sounds expands and vocalizations become more speech-like. Infants typically begin to produce recognizable words when they are around 12 months of age, though babbling may continue for some time afterward.

The marginal value theorem (MVT) is an optimality model that usually describes the behavior of an optimally foraging individual in a system where resources are located in discrete patches separated by areas with no resources. Due to the resource-free space, animals must spend time traveling between patches. The MVT can also be applied to other situations in which organisms face diminishing returns.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal song</span>

Animal song is not a well-defined term in scientific literature, and the use of the more broadly defined term vocalizations is in more common use. Song generally consists of several successive vocal sounds incorporating multiple syllables. Some sources distinguish between simpler vocalizations, termed “calls”, reserving the term “song” for more complex productions. Song-like productions have been identified in several groups of animals, including cetaceans, avians (birds), anurans (frogs), and humans. Social transmission of song has been found in groups including birds and cetaceans.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mate choice</span> One of the primary mechanisms under which evolution can occur

Mate choice is one of the primary mechanisms under which evolution can occur. It is characterized by a "selective response by animals to particular stimuli" which can be observed as behavior. In other words, before an animal engages with a potential mate, they first evaluate various aspects of that mate which are indicative of quality—such as the resources or phenotypes they have—and evaluate whether or not those particular trait(s) are somehow beneficial to them. The evaluation will then incur a response of some sort.

The Cameroon indigobird is a species of bird in the family Viduidae. It is considered by some authors to be a subspecies of the variable indigobird. They range from Sierra Leone to east Cameroon, north east Zaire and South Sudan.

A mating call is the auditory signal used by animals to attract mates. It can occur in males or females, but literature is abundantly favored toward researching mating calls in females. In addition, mating calls are often the subject of mate choice, in which the preferences of one gender for a certain type of mating call can drive sexual selection in a species. This can result in sympatric speciation of some animals, where two species diverge from each other while living in the same environment.

The challenge hypothesis outlines the dynamic relationship between testosterone and aggression in mating contexts. It proposes that testosterone promotes aggression when it would be beneficial for reproduction, such as mate guarding, or strategies designed to prevent the encroachment of intrasexual rivals. The positive correlation between reproductive aggression and testosterone levels is seen to be strongest during times of social instability. The challenge hypothesis predicts that seasonal patterns in testosterone levels are a function of mating system, paternal care, and male-male aggression in seasonal breeders.

Polygyny is a mating system in which one male lives and mates with multiple females but each female only mates with a single male. Systems where several females mate with several males are defined either as promiscuity or polygynandry. Lek mating is frequently regarded as a form of polygyny, because one male mates with many females, but lek-based mating systems differ in that the male has no attachment to the females with whom he mates, and that mating females lack attachment to one another.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sexual selection in birds</span>

Sexual selection in birds concerns how birds have evolved a variety of mating behaviors, with the peacock tail being perhaps the most famous example of sexual selection and the Fisherian runaway. Commonly occurring sexual dimorphisms such as size and color differences are energetically costly attributes that signal competitive breeding situations. Many types of avian sexual selection have been identified; intersexual selection, also known as female choice; and intrasexual competition, where individuals of the more abundant sex compete with each other for the privilege to mate. Sexually selected traits often evolve to become more pronounced in competitive breeding situations until the trait begins to limit the individual's fitness. Conflicts between an individual fitness and signaling adaptations ensure that sexually selected ornaments such as plumage coloration and courtship behavior are “honest” traits. Signals must be costly to ensure that only good-quality individuals can present these exaggerated sexual ornaments and behaviors.

The nest protection hypothesis (NPH) is one of multiple hypotheses that seek to explain the behaviour of birds repeatedly introducing green, often aromatic, plant material into the nest after its completion and throughout the incubation and nestling periods. The hypothesis suggests that this behaviour is an evolutionary strategy to ward off or kill ectoparasites that would otherwise cause higher nestling mortality through blood loss and the spread of pathogens. The aromatic species of greenery are often collected from trees and long-living shrubs containing strong aromatic compounds which are expected to either disrupt olfaction in host seeking parasites or kill harmful parasites and pathogens.

References

  1. 1 2 3 Nolan, Val Jr.; Ketterson, Ellen D.; Thompson, Charles F. (2013-11-11). Current Ornithology. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN   9781475799156.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Schraft, Hannes A.; Medina, Orlando J.; McClure, Jesse; Pereira, Daniel A.; Logue, David M. (2017). "Within-day improvement in a behavioural display: Wild birds 'warm up'". Animal Behaviour. 124: 167–174. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.12.026. S2CID   54299141.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Poesel, A. & Kempenaers, B. (30 May 2000). "When a bird is tired from singing: a study of drift during the dawn chorus" (PDF). Etología. 8: 1–7.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 Lambrechts, Marcel; Dhondt, André A. (1988-04-01). "The anti-exhaustion hypothesis: a new hypothesis to explain song performance and song switching in the great tit". Animal Behaviour. 36 (2): 327–334. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80002-2. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   54375430.
  5. Brumm, Henrik; Lachlan, Robert F.; Riebel, Katharina; Slater, Peter J.B. (2009-01-01). "On the function of song type repertoires: testing the 'antiexhaustion hypothesis' in chaffinches". Animal Behaviour. 77 (1): 37–42. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.009. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53194340.