Mate choice

Last updated
Mate choice is highly visible in lek mating. Here, black grouse males gather in a quagmire and the females then arrive and observe the male before choosing one. Lekadvert.JPG
Mate choice is highly visible in lek mating. Here, black grouse males gather in a quagmire and the females then arrive and observe the male before choosing one.

Mate choice is one of the primary mechanisms under which evolution can occur. It is characterized by a "selective response by animals to particular stimuli" which can be observed as behavior. [1] In other words, before an animal engages with a potential mate, they first evaluate various aspects of that mate which are indicative of quality—such as the resources or phenotypes they have—and evaluate whether or not those particular trait(s) are somehow beneficial to them. The evaluation will then incur a response of some sort. [1]

Contents

These mechanisms are a part of evolutionary change because they operate in a way that causes the qualities that are desired in a mate to be more frequently passed on to each generation over time. For example, if female peacocks desire mates who have a colourful plumage, then this trait will increase in frequency over time as male peacocks with a colourful plumage will have more reproductive success. [2] Further investigation of this concept, has found that it is in fact the specific trait of blue and green colour near the eyespot that seems to increase the females likelihood of mating with a specific peacock. [3]

Mate choice is a major component of sexual selection, another being intrasexual selection. Ideas on sexual selection were first introduced in 1871, by Charles Darwin, then expanded on by Ronald Fisher in 1915. At present, there are five sub mechanisms that explain how mate choice has evolved over time. These are direct phenotypic benefits, sensory bias, the Fisherian runaway hypothesis, indicator traits and genetic compatibility.

In the majority of systems where mate choice exists, one sex tends to be competitive with their same-sex members [4] and the other sex is choosy (meaning they are selective when it comes to picking individuals to mate with). There are direct and indirect benefits of being the selective individual. [5] [6] [7] In most species, females are the choosy sex which discriminates among competitive males, [4] but there are several examples of reversed roles (see below). It is preferable for an individual to choose a compatible mate of the same species, in order to maintain reproductive success. [8] Other factors that can influence mate choice include pathogen stress and the major histocompatibility complex (MHC).

Origins and history

Ronald Fisher in 1913 Youngronaldfisher2.JPG
Ronald Fisher in 1913
The peacock tail in flight, the classic example of a Fisherian runaway Peacock Flying.jpg
The peacock tail in flight, the classic example of a Fisherian runaway

Charles Darwin first expressed his ideas on sexual selection and mate choice in his book The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex in 1871. He was perplexed by the elaborate ornamentation that males of some species have, because such features appeared to be detrimental to survival and to have negative consequences for reproductive success. Darwin proposed two explanations for the existence of such traits: these traits are useful in male-male combat or they are preferred by females. [9] This article focuses on the latter. Darwin treated natural selection and sexual selection as two different topics, although in the 1930s biologists defined sexual selection as being a part of natural selection. [10]

In 1915, Ronald Fisher wrote a paper on the evolution of female preference and secondary sexual characteristics. [11] Fifteen years later, he expanded this theory in a book called The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection . There he described a scenario, Fisherian runaway, where feedback between mate preference and a trait results in elaborate characters such as the long tail of the male peacock.

In 1948, using Drosophila as a model, Angus John Bateman presented experimental evidence that male reproductive success is limited by the number of mates obtained, while female reproductive success is limited by the number of pregnancies that she can have in her lifetime. [12] Thus a female must be selective when choosing a mate because the quality of her offspring depends on it. Males must fight, in the form of intra-sexual competition, for the opportunity to mate because not all males will be chosen by females. This became known as Bateman's principle, and although this was a major finding that added to the work of Darwin and Fisher, it was overlooked until George C. Williams emphasised its importance in the 1960s and 1970s. [13] [14]

In 1972, soon after Williams' revival of the subject, Robert L. Trivers presented his parental investment theory. Trivers defined parental investment as any investment made by the parent that benefits his or her current offspring at the cost of investment in future offspring. These investments include the costs of producing gametes as well as any other care or efforts that parents provide after birth or hatching. Reformulating Bateman's ideas, Trivers argued that the sex which exhibits less parental investment (not necessarily the male) will have to compete for mating opportunities with the sex that invests more. The differences in levels of parental investment create the condition that favours mating biases. [15]

Direct and indirect benefits

The act of being choosy was likely selected for as a way to assess whether or not a potential partner's contribution(s) would be capable of producing and/or maintaining the viability of an offspring. Utilizing these behaviors usually results in two types of benefits to the individual who is being choosy:

Inbreeding avoidance

Usually, animal biologists assume that mate choice is biased against relatives because of the negative consequences of inbreeding. [16] However certain natural constraints act to limit the evolution of inbreeding avoidance, particularly when there is a risk of mating with a partner of a different species (heterospecific mating) and losing fitness through hybridization. [16] Inclusive fitness appears to be maximized in matings of intermediately related individuals. [17]

Mechanisms

As of 2018, five proposed mechanisms address the evolution of mate choice:

Direct and/or indirect benefits drive the mating biases described in each mechanism. It is possible that these mechanisms co-occur, although the relative roles of each have not been evaluated adequately. [4]

Direct phenotypic benefits

A choosy mate tends to have preferences for certain types of traits—also known as phenotypes—which would benefit them to have in a potential partner. These traits must be reliable, and commutative of something that directly benefits the choosy partner in some way. [18] Having a mating preference is advantageous in this situation because it directly affects reproductive fitness. Direct benefits are widespread and empirical studies provide evidence for this mechanism of evolution. [19] [20]

One example of a sexually selected trait with direct benefits is the bright plumage of the northern cardinal, a common backyard bird in the eastern United States. Male northern cardinals have conspicuous red feathers, while the females have a more cryptic coloration. In this example, the females are the choosy sex and will use male plumage brightness as a signal when picking a mate — research suggests that males with brighter plumage feed their young more frequently than males with duller plumage. [21] This increased help in caring for the young lifts some of the burden from the mother so that she can raise more offspring than she could without help.

Though this particular mechanism operates on the premise that all phenotypes must communicate something that benefits the choosy mate directly, such selected phenotypes can also have additional indirect benefits for the mother by benefiting the offspring. For example, with the increased help in feeding their young seen in Northern Cardinals with more plumage-brightness, comes an increase in the overall amount of food that is likely to be given to the offspring - even if the mother has more children. [22] Though females may choose this trait with the presumed directly advantageous aim of allowing them more time and energy to allocate to producing more offspring, it also benefits the offspring in that two parents provide food instead of one, thereby increasing the likelihood of the overall amount of food available to the offspring despite a possible increase in the amount of offspring siblings. [22]

Sensory bias

The sensory-bias hypothesis states that the preference for a trait evolves in a non-mating context and is then exploited by the less choosy sex in order to obtain more mating opportunities. The competitive sex evolves traits that exploit a pre-existing bias that the choosy sex already possesses. Following this hypothesis, increased selectivity for one of these specific traits can explain remarkable trait differences in closely related species because it produces a divergence in signaling systems which leads to reproductive isolation. [23]

Sensory bias has been demonstrated in guppies, freshwater fish from Trinidad and Tobago. In this mating system, female guppies prefer to mate with males with more orange body-coloration. However, outside of a mating context, both sexes prefer animate orange objects, which suggests that preference originally evolved in another context, like foraging. [24] Orange fruits are a rare treat that fall into streams where the guppies live. The ability to find these fruits quickly is an adaptive quality that has evolved outside of a mating context. Sometime after the affinity for orange objects arose, male guppies exploited this preference by incorporating large orange spots to attract females.

Another example of sensory exploitation is the case of the water mite Neumania papillator , an ambush predator which hunts copepods (small crustaceans) passing by in the water column. [25] When hunting, N. papillator adopts a characteristic stance termed the "net stance": its holds its first four legs out into the water column, with its four hind legs resting on aquatic vegetation; this allows it to detect vibrational stimuli produced by swimming prey and to use this to orient towards and clutch at prey. [26] During courtship, males actively search for females; [27] if a male finds a female, he slowly circles around the female whilst trembling his first and second leg near her. [25] [26] Male leg-trembling causes females (who were in the "net stance") to orient towards and often to clutch the male. [25] This does not damage the male or deter further courtship; the male then deposits spermatophores and begins to vigorously fan and jerk his fourth pair of legs over the spermatophore, generating a current of water that passes over the spermatophores and towards the female. [25] Sperm-packet uptake by the female would sometimes follow. [25] Heather Proctor hypothesised that the vibrations made by trembling male legs mimic the vibrations that females detect from swimming prey. This would trigger the female prey-detection responses, causing females to orient and then clutch at males, mediating courtship. [25] [28] If this was true and males were exploiting female predation responses, then hungry females should be more receptive to male trembling. Proctor found that unfed captive females did orient and clutch at males significantly more than fed captive females did, consistent with the sensory exploitation hypothesis. [25]

Other examples of the sensory-bias mechanism include traits in auklets, [29] wolf spiders, [30] and manakins. [31] Further experimental work is required to reach a fuller understanding of the prevalence and mechanisms of sensory bias. [32]

Fisherian runaway and sexy-son hypothesis

This creates a positive feedback loop in which a particular trait is desired by a female and present in a male, and that desire for and presence of that particular trait are then reflected in their offspring. [22] If this mechanism is strong enough, it can lead to a type of self-reinforcing coevolution. [22] If runaway selection is strong enough, it may incur significant costs, such as increased visibility to predators and energetic costs to maintain the trait's full expression. Hence peacocks' extravagant feathers, or any number of lek mating displays. This model does not predict a genetic benefit; rather, the reward is more mates.

In a study done on great reed warblers, models based on the polygyny threshold and sexy-son hypotheses predict that females should gain evolutionary advantage in either short-term or long-term in this mating system. Although the importance of female choice was demonstrated, the study did not support the hypotheses. [33] Other studies, such as those conducted on long-tailed widowbirds, have demonstrated the existence of female choice. [34] Here, females chose males with long tails, and even preferred those males with experimentally lengthened tails over shortened tails and those of naturally occurring length. Such a process shows how female choice could give rise to exaggerated sexual traits through Fisherian runaway selection.

Indicator traits

Indicator traits signal good overall quality of the individual. Traits perceived as attractive must reliably indicate broad genetic quality in order for selection to favor them and for preference to evolve. This is an example of indirect genetic benefits received by the choosy sex, because mating with such individuals will result in high-quality offspring. The indicator traits hypothesis is split into three highly related subtopics: the handicap theory of sexual selection, the good genes hypothesis, and the Hamilton–Zuk hypothesis.

People rate the importance of certain traits differently when referring to their own or to others' ideal long-term partners. Research suggests that women consider traits indicating genetic fitness as more important for their own partner, while prioritising traits that provide benefits to others for their sister's ideal partner. [35]

Indicator traits are condition-dependent and have associated costs. Therefore, individuals which can handle these costs well (cf. "I can do X [here, survive] with one hand tied behind my back") should be desired by the choosy sex for their superior genetic quality. This is known as the handicap theory of sexual selection. [36]

The good genes hypothesis states that the choosy sex will mate with individuals who possess traits that signify overall genetic quality. In doing so, they gain an evolutionary advantage for their offspring through indirect benefit.

The Hamilton–Zuk hypothesis posits that sexual ornaments are indicators of parasite- and disease-resistance. [37] To test this hypothesis, red jungle-fowl males were infected with a parasitic roundworm and monitored for growth and developmental changes. Female preference was also evaluated. The researchers found that parasites affected the development and final appearance of ornamental traits and that females preferred males who were not infected. This supports the idea that parasites are an important factor in sexual selection and mate choice. [38]

One of many examples of indicator traits is the condition-dependent patch of red feathers around the face and shoulders of the male house finch. This patch varies in brightness among individuals because the pigments that produce the red color (carotenoids) are limited in the environment. Thus, males who have a high-quality diet will have brighter red plumage. In a much-cited manipulation experiment, female house finches were shown to prefer males with brighter red patches. Also, males with naturally brighter patches proved better fathers and exhibited higher offspring-feeding rates than duller males. [20]

Genetic compatibility

Genetic compatibility refers to how well the genes of two parents function together in their offspring. Choosing genetically compatible mates could result in optimally fit offspring and notably affect reproductive fitness. However, the genetic compatibility model is limited to specific traits due to complex genetic interactions (e.g. major histocompatibility complex in humans and mice). The choosy sex must know their own genotype as well as the genotypes of potential mates in order to select the appropriate partner. [39] This makes testing components of genetic compatibility difficult and controversial.

Role of major histocompatibility complex

A controversial but well-known experiment suggests that human females use body odor as an indicator of genetic compatibility. In this study, males were given a plain T-shirt to sleep in for two nights in order to provide a scent sample. College women were then asked to rate odors from several men, some with similar MHC (major histocompatibility complex) genes to their own and others with dissimilar genes. MHC genes code for receptors that identify foreign pathogens in the body so that the immune system may respond and destroy them. Since each different gene in the MHC codes for a different type of receptor, it is expected that females will benefit from mating with males who have more dissimilar MHC genes. This will ensure better resistance to parasites and disease in offspring. Researchers found that women tended to rate the odors higher if the male's genes were more dissimilar to their own. They concluded that the odors are influenced by the MHC and that they have consequences for mate choice in human populations today. [40]

Similar to the humans of the odor-rating experiment, animals also choose mates based upon genetic compatibility as determined by evaluating the body odor of their potential mate(s). Some animals, such as mice, assess a mate's genetic compatibility based on their urine odor. [41]

In an experiment studying three-spined sticklebacks, researchers found that females prefer to mate with males that share a greater diversity of major histocompatibility complex (MHC) and in addition possess a MHC haplotype specific to fighting the common parasite Gyrodactylus salaris . [42] Mates that have MHC genes different from one another will be superior when reproducing with regard to parasite resistance, body condition and reproductive success and survival. [43]

The genetic diversity of animals and life reproductive success (LRS) at the MHC level is optimal at intermediate levels rather than at its maximum, [44] [45] despite MHC being one of the most polymorphic genes. [46] In a study, researchers discovered that mice heterozygous at all MHC loci were less resistant than mice homozygous at all loci to salmonella, so it appears disadvantageous to display many different MHC alleles due to the increased loss of T-cells, [47] which aid an organism's immune system and trigger its appropriate response. [48]

MHC diversity may also correlate with MHC gene expression. As long as a heritable component exists in expression patterns, natural selection is able to act upon the trait. Therefore, gene expression for MHC genes might contribute to the natural selection processes of certain species and be in fact evolutionarily relevant. For example, in another study of three-spined sticklebacks, exposure to parasite species increased MHC class IIB expression by over 25%, proving that parasitic infection increases gene expression. [49]

MHC diversity in vertebrates may also be generated by the recombination of alleles on the MHC gene. [50]

Sex role reversal in animals

In species where mating biases exist, females are typically the choosy sex because they provide a greater parental investment than males.[ citation needed ] However, there are some examples of sex role reversals where females must compete with each other for mating opportunities with males. Species that exhibit parental care after the birth of their offspring have the potential to overcome the sex differences in parental investment (the amount of energy that each parent contributes per offspring) and lead to a reversal in sex roles. [4] The following are examples of male mate choice (sex role reversal) across several taxa.

Speciation

For many years it has been suggested that sexual isolation caused by differences in mating behaviours is a precursor for reproductive isolation (lack of gene flow), and consequently speciation, in nature. [57] Mate choice behaviours are thought to be important forces that can result in speciation events because the strength of selection for attractive traits is often very strong. Speciation by this method occurs when a preference for some sexual trait shifts and produces a pre-zygotic barrier (preventing fertilisation). These processes have been difficult to test until recently with advances in genetic modelling. [58] Speciation by sexual selection is gaining popularity in the literature with increasing theoretical and empirical studies.

There is evidence of early speciation through mate preference in guppies. Guppies are located across several isolated streams in Trinidad and male colour patterns differ geographically. Female guppies have no coloration but their preference for these colour patterns also vary across locations. In a mate choice study, female guppies were shown to prefer males with colour patterns that are typical of their home stream. [59] This preference could result in reproductive isolation if two populations came into contact again. There is a similar trend shown in two species of the wood white butterfly, L. reali and L. sinapis. Female L. sinapis controls mate choice by engaging only in conspecific mating, while males attempt to mate with either species. This female mate choice has encouraged speciation of the two wood whites. [60]

The black-throated blue warbler, a North American bird, is another example. Asymmetric recognition of local and non-local songs has been found between two populations of black-throated blue warblers in the United States, one in the northern United States (New Hampshire) and the other in the southern United States (North Carolina). [61] Males in the northern population respond strongly to the local male songs but relatively weakly to the non-local songs of southern males. In contrast, southern males respond equally to both local and non-local songs. The fact that northern males exhibit differential recognition indicates that northern females tend not to mate with "heterospecific" males from the south; thus it is not necessary for the northern males to respond strongly to the song from a southern challenger. A barrier to gene flow exists from South to North as a result of the female choice, which can eventually lead to speciation.

Mate choice in humans

In humans, males and females differ in their strategies to acquire mates. Females exhibit more mate choice selectivity than males. According to Bateman's principle, human females display less variance in their Lifespan Reproductive Success, due to their high obligatory parental investment. [62] Human female sexual selection is indicated by sexually dimorphism, especially in traits that serve little other evolutionary purpose, such as the presence in men of beards, overall lower voice pitch, and average greater height. Women have reported a preference for men with beards and lower voices. [63] [64] The traits most salient to female human mate choice are parental investment, resource provision and the provision of good genes to offspring. [65] Women as well as men may seek short-term mating partners. [66] This could gain them resources; provide genetic benefit, as through the sexy son hypothesis; facilitate a desired break-up; and allow them to assess a mate's suitability as a long-term partner. [65] Women prefer long-term partners over short-term mates, as they have a larger investment in a child through pregnancy and lactation. [65] Factors in female mate choice include the woman's own perceived attractiveness, the woman's personal resources, mate copying and parasite stress. [65] Romantic love is the mechanism through which long-term mate choice occurs in human females. [67]

In humans, females have to endure a nine-month pregnancy and childbirth. [65] Females thus provide a greater biologically obligatory parental investment to offspring than males. [65] [68] This provides males with a greater window of opportunity to mate and reproduce than females, hence females are usually more choosy, but males still make mate choices. [68] When finding a short-term mate, males highly value women with sexual experience and physical attractiveness. Men seeking short-term sexual relationships are likely to avoid women who are interested in commitment or require investment. [69] For a long-term relationship, males may look for commitment, facial symmetry, femininity, physical beauty, waist–hip ratio, large breasts, [70] and youth. [71] [67] [72] [73] [74] Due to the higher obligatory biological investment, women are choosier in short-term mating, as the perceived paternal investment is low to non existent, whereas men and women are equally choosy when deciding for long-term mates, as men and women then have an equal parental investment, as men then invest heavily in the offspring by resource provisioning. [75]

The parasite-stress theory suggests that parasites or diseases stress an organism, making them look less sexually attractive. [76] Choosing a mate for attractiveness could thus help to find a healthy mate resistant to parasites. [77] [78]

Scarification could be viewed by prospective mates as evidence that a person has overcome parasites and is thus more attractive. [79] [80] Masculinity, especially in the face, could equally indicate robust parasite-free health. [81] [82] [83] [84] Polygamy is predicted by pathogen stress in the tropics. [85] [86]

Human leukocyte antigen (HLA) proteins are essential for immune system functioning and are highly variable, assumed to be a result of frequency-dependent parasite-driven selection and mate choice. There is some evidence that women detect and select HLA type by odour, though this is disputed. [87] [88] [89] [90] [91] Human facial preferences correlate with both MHC-similarity and MHC-heterozygosity. [92]

Mate choice for cognitive traits

In the late 19th century, Charles Darwin proposed that cognition, or "intelligence," was the product of two combined evolutionary forces: natural selection and sexual selection. [93] Research on human mate choice showed that intelligence is sexually selected for, and is highly esteemed by both sexes. [94] [95] Some evolutionary psychologists have suggested that humans evolved large brains because the cognitive abilities associated with this size increase were successful in attracting mates, consequently increasing reproductive success: brains are metabolically costly to produce and are an honest signal of mate quality. [96] Cognition may be functioning to attract mates in other taxa. [97] If the possession of higher cognitive skills enhances a male's ability to gather resources, then females may benefit directly from choosing more intelligent males, through courtship feeding or allofeeding. [98] Assuming cognitive skills are heritable to some degree, females may also benefit indirectly through their offspring. [97] Additionally, cognitive ability has been shown to vary significantly, both within and between species, and could be under sexual selection as a result. [99] Recently, researchers have started to ask to what extent individuals assess the cognitive abilities of the opposite sex when choosing a mate. [97] In fruit flies, the absence of sexual selection was accompanied by a decline in male cognitive performance. [100]

In non-human vertebrates

Female preference for males with enhanced cognitive ability "may be reflected in successful males' courtship displays, foraging performance, courtship feeding or diet-dependent morphological traits." [97] However, few are the studies that assess whether females can discriminate between males through direct observation of cognitively demanding tasks. Instead, researchers generally investigate female choice by reason of morphological traits correlated with cognitive ability. [97]

A male satin bowerbird guards its bower from rival males in the hopes of attracting females with its decorations. Satin Bowerbird nest.jpg
A male satin bowerbird guards its bower from rival males in the hopes of attracting females with its decorations.

Birds

  • Budgerigars: in this parrot species, direct observation of problem-solving skills was shown to increase male attractiveness. [101] In a two-step experiment testing for female preference, initially less-preferred male budgerigars became preferred after females observed them solve extractive foraging problems while initially preferred males failed to problem-solve. This shift in preference suggests that females discriminate between males through direct observation of cognitively-demanding tasks. [101]
  • Zebra finches: researchers conducted a problem-solving experiment similar to the one described above. [102] However, male problem-solving performance was not found to influence female mating preferences. Instead, females had a significant preference for males with better foraging efficiency. There is no evidence that females indirectly assess male cognitive ability through morphological traits. By selecting for males with a higher foraging efficiency, females obtained direct food benefits. [102]
  • Satin Bowerbirds: bowerbirds are famous because of their complex male courtship, in which they utilize decorations of certain colours. [103] This suggests that male bowerbirds with enhanced cognitive abilities should have a selective advantage during male courtship. [104] In a study testing problem-solving skills, male satin bowerbirds' general cognitive performance was found to correlate to mating success, rendering them more sexually attractive. [104] However, a more recent study evaluated bowerbirds on six cognitive tasks, and found very little intercorrelation between them. [105] In a more recent study, bowerbird performance in six cognitive tasks was positively correlated with mating success, but no general cognitive ability was found as intercorrelations between performances on each task were weak. Instead, mating success could be independently predicted via the male's performance on most tasks; this demonstrates the complexity of the link between cognitive ability and sexual selection. [105]
  • Red Crossbills: an experimental study showed that female red crossbills prefer males with higher foraging efficiency. [106] After observing two males simultaneously extracting seeds from cones, females spent more time in proximity of the faster foragers. Although females could benefit both directly and indirectly through their preference for faster foragers, the potential link between foraging efficiency and cognitive abilities was not investigated. [106] [107]
  • Mountain chickadees: in this monogamous bird species, spatial cognition is important as mountain chickadees rely on the retrieval of previously stored food caches to nourish themselves during the winter. [108] Spatial memory has been shown to correlate with reproductive success in mountain chickadees. [109] When females mate with males possessing enhanced spatial cognition, they laid larger clutches and fledged larger broods when compared to females mating with males possessing worse spatial cognition. Therefore, when mating with high quality males with enhanced spatial skills, females may attempt to gain indirect, genetic benefits by increasing their reproductive investment. [109]
The Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata), male (above), and female (below) Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata) male and female.png
The Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata), male (above), and female (below)

Other

  • Guppies: when assessing male cognitive ability via maze experiments, females preferred males that learnt two mazes at a faster rate. [110] However, females did not directly observe males during their learning tasks. Although females could be discriminating between males by assessing orange saturation - a morphological trait weakly correlated to learning rate - females did not find males with brighter orange spots more attractive. Instead, females may use an unknown visual trait to discriminate between males and their cognitive abilities. [110]
  • Meadow Voles: in this species of rodents, female preference for male spatial ability was investigated. [111] Male spatial ability was measured using a series of water maze tests, before females were given a choice between two males. [111] [112] Although females did not directly observe males complete spatial-ability tasks, they preferred males with enhanced spatial skills. This preference could explain why meadow vole brain regions associated with spatial ability are sexually dimorphic: spatial ability could serve as an honest signal of male quality due to the metabolic costs associated with brain development. [96] [111] [113]

Criticisms

Although there is some evidence that females assess male cognitive ability when choosing a mate, the effect that cognitive ability has on survival and mating preference remain unclear. [97] Many questions need to be answered to be able to better appreciate the implications that cognitive traits may have in mate choice. Some discrepancies also need to be resolved. For example, in 1996, Catchpole suggested that in songbirds, females preferred males with larger song repertoires. Learned song repertoire was correlated with the size of the High Vocal Center (HVC) in the brain; females may then use song repertoire as an indicator of general cognitive ability. [114] However, a more recent study found learned song repertoire to be an unreliable signal of cognitive ability. Rather than a general cognitive ability, male songbirds were found to have specific cognitive abilities that did not positively associate. [115] [116]

As of 2011, more research was needed on what extent cognitive abilities determine foraging success or courtship displays, what extent behavioural courtship displays rely on learning through practice and experience, what extent cognitive abilities affect survival and mating success, and what indicator traits could be used as a signal of cognitive ability. [97] Researchers have started to explore links between cognition and personality; some personality traits such as boldness or neophobia may be used as indicators of cognitive ability, although more evidence is required to characterize personality-cognition relationships. [117] As of 2011, empirical evidence for the benefits, both direct and indirect, of choosing mates with enhanced cognition is weak. One possible research direction would be on the indirect benefits of mating with males with enhanced spatial cognition in mountain chickadees. [97] [109] Additional focus in research is needed on developmental and environmental effects on cognitive ability, as such factors have been shown to influence song learning and could therefore influence other cognitive traits. [97]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sexual selection</span> Mode of natural selection involving the choosing of and competition for mates

Sexual selection is a mode of natural selection in which members of one biological sex choose mates of the other sex to mate with, and compete with members of the same sex for access to members of the opposite sex. These two forms of selection mean that some individuals have greater reproductive success than others within a population, for example because they are more attractive or prefer more attractive partners to produce offspring. Successful males benefit from frequent mating and monopolizing access to one or more fertile females. Females can maximise the return on the energy they invest in reproduction by selecting and mating with the best males.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Behavioral ecology</span> Study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures

Behavioral ecology, also spelled behavioural ecology, is the study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures. Behavioral ecology emerged from ethology after Niko Tinbergen outlined four questions to address when studying animal behaviors: What are the proximate causes, ontogeny, survival value, and phylogeny of a behavior?

Disassortative mating is a mating pattern in which individuals with dissimilar phenotypes mate with one another more frequently than would be expected under random mating. Disassortative mating reduces the mean genetic similarities within the population and produces a greater number of heterozygotes. The pattern is character specific, but does not affect allele frequencies. This nonrandom mating pattern will result in deviation from the Hardy-Weinberg principle.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reproductive success</span> Passing of genes on to the next generation in a way that they too can pass on those genes

Reproductive success is an individual's production of offspring per breeding event or lifetime. This is not limited by the number of offspring produced by one individual, but also the reproductive success of these offspring themselves.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parental investment</span> Parental expenditure (e.g. time, energy, resources) that benefits offspring

Parental investment, in evolutionary biology and evolutionary psychology, is any parental expenditure that benefits offspring. Parental investment may be performed by both males and females, females alone or males alone. Care can be provided at any stage of the offspring's life, from pre-natal to post-natal.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Psychological adaptation</span>

A psychological adaptation is a functional, cognitive or behavioral trait that benefits an organism in its environment. Psychological adaptations fall under the scope of evolved psychological mechanisms (EPMs), however, EPMs refer to a less restricted set. Psychological adaptations include only the functional traits that increase the fitness of an organism, while EPMs refer to any psychological mechanism that developed through the processes of evolution. These additional EPMs are the by-product traits of a species’ evolutionary development, as well as the vestigial traits that no longer benefit the species’ fitness. It can be difficult to tell whether a trait is vestigial or not, so some literature is more lenient and refers to vestigial traits as adaptations, even though they may no longer have adaptive functionality. For example, xenophobic attitudes and behaviors, some have claimed, appear to have certain EPM influences relating to disease aversion, however, in many environments these behaviors will have a detrimental effect on a person's fitness. The principles of psychological adaptation rely on Darwin's theory of evolution and are important to the fields of evolutionary psychology, biology, and cognitive science.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sexual conflict</span> Term in evolutionary biology

Sexual conflict or sexual antagonism occurs when the two sexes have conflicting optimal fitness strategies concerning reproduction, particularly over the mode and frequency of mating, potentially leading to an evolutionary arms race between males and females. In one example, males may benefit from multiple matings, while multiple matings may harm or endanger females, due to the anatomical differences of that species. Sexual conflict underlies the evolutionary distinction between male and female.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sexy son hypothesis</span> Postulate in biology

The sexy son hypothesis in evolutionary biology and sexual selection, proposed by Patrick J. Weatherhead and Raleigh J. Robertson of Queen's University in Kingston, Ontario in 1979, states that a female's ideal mate choice among potential mates is one whose genes will produce males with the best chance of reproductive success. This implies that other benefits the father can offer the mother or offspring are less relevant than they may appear, including his capacity as a parental caregiver, territory and any nuptial gifts. Fisher's principle means that the sex ratio is always near 1:1 between males and females, yet what matters most are her "sexy sons'" future breeding successes, more likely if they have a promiscuous father, in creating large numbers of offspring carrying copies of her genes. This sexual selection hypothesis has been researched in species such as the European pied flycatcher.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lek paradox</span>

The lek paradox is the conundrum of how additive or beneficial genetic variation is maintained in lek mating species in the face of consistent sexual selection based on female preferences. While many studies have attempted to explain how the lek paradox fits into Darwinian theory, the paradox remains. Persistent female choice for particular male trait values should erode genetic diversity in male traits and thereby remove the benefits of choice, yet choice persists. This paradox can be somewhat alleviated by the occurrence of mutations introducing potential differences, as well as the possibility that traits of interest have more or less favorable recessive alleles.

Bateman's principle, in evolutionary biology, is that in most species, variability in reproductive success is greater in males than in females. It was first proposed by Angus John Bateman (1919–1996), an English geneticist. Bateman suggested that, since males are capable of producing millions of sperm cells with little effort, while females invest much higher levels of energy in order to nurture a relatively small number of eggs, the female plays a significantly larger role in their offspring's reproductive success. Bateman's paradigm thus views females as the limiting factor of parental investment, over which males will compete in order to copulate successfully.

Odour is sensory stimulation of the olfactory membrane of the nose by a group of molecules. Certain body odours are connected to human sexual attraction. Humans can make use of body odour subconsciously to identify whether a potential mate will pass on favourable traits to their offspring. Body odour may provide significant cues about the genetic quality, health and reproductive success of a potential mate.

A biological ornament is a characteristic of an animal that appears to serve a decorative function rather than a utilitarian function. Many are secondary sexual characteristics, and others appear on young birds during the period when they are dependent on being fed by their parents. Ornaments are used in displays to attract mates, which may lead to the evolutionary process known as sexual selection. An animal may shake, lengthen, or spread out its ornament in order to get the attention of the opposite sex, which will in turn choose the most attractive one with which to mate. Ornaments are most often observed in males, and choosing an extravagantly ornamented male benefits females as the genes that produce the ornament will be passed on to her offspring, increasing their own reproductive fitness. As Ronald Fisher noted, the male offspring will inherit the ornament while the female offspring will inherit the preference for said ornament, which can lead to a positive feedback loop known as a Fisherian runaway. These structures serve as cues to animal sexual behaviour, that is, they are sensory signals that affect mating responses. Therefore, ornamental traits are often selected by mate choice.

Interlocus sexual conflict is a type of sexual conflict that occurs through the interaction of a set of antagonistic alleles at two or more different loci, or the location of a gene on a chromosome, in males and females, resulting in the deviation of either or both sexes from the fitness optima for the traits. A co-evolutionary arms race is established between the sexes in which either sex evolves a set of antagonistic adaptations that is detrimental to the fitness of the other sex. The potential for reproductive success in one organism is strengthened while the fitness of the opposite sex is weakened. Interlocus sexual conflict can arise due to aspects of male–female interactions such as mating frequency, fertilization, relative parental effort, female remating behavior, and female reproductive rate.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Major histocompatibility complex and sexual selection</span> Adaptive immune gene selection

The major histocompatibility complex in sexual selection concerns how major histocompatibility complex (MHC) molecules allow for immune system surveillance of the population of protein molecules in a host's cells. In 1976, Yamazaki et al. demonstrated a sexual selection mate choice by male mice for females of a different MHC.

The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology are the general and specific scientific theories that explain the ultimate origins of psychological traits in terms of evolution. These theories originated with Charles Darwin's work, including his speculations about the evolutionary origins of social instincts in humans. Modern evolutionary psychology, however, is possible only because of advances in evolutionary theory in the 20th century.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human mating strategies</span> Courtship behavior of humans

In evolutionary psychology and behavioral ecology, human mating strategies are a set of behaviors used by individuals to select, attract, and retain mates. Mating strategies overlap with reproductive strategies, which encompass a broader set of behaviors involving the timing of reproduction and the trade-off between quantity and quality of offspring.

Mate value is derived from Charles Darwin's theory of evolution and sexual selection, as well as the social exchange theory of relationships. Mate value is defined as the sum of traits that are perceived as desirable, representing genetic quality and/or fitness, an indication of a potential mate's reproductive success. Based on mate desirability and mate preference, mate value underpins mate selection and the formation of romantic relationships.

The ovulatory shift hypothesis holds that women experience evolutionarily adaptive changes in subconscious thoughts and behaviors related to mating during different parts of the ovulatory cycle. It suggests that what women want, in terms of men, changes throughout the menstrual cycle. Two meta-analyses published in 2014 reached opposing conclusions on whether the existing evidence was robust enough to support the prediction that women's mate preferences change across the cycle. A newer 2018 review does not show women changing the type of men they desire at different times in their fertility cycle.

In humans, males and females differ in their strategies to acquire mates and focus on certain qualities. There are two main categories of strategies that both sexes utilize: short-term and long-term. Human mate choice, an aspect of sexual selection in humans, depends on a variety of factors, such as ecology, demography, access to resources, rank/social standing, genes, and parasite stress.

Genetic incompatibility describes the process by which mating yields offspring that are nonviable, prone to disease, or genetically defective in some way. In nature, animals can ill afford to devote costly resources for little or no reward, ergo, mating strategies have evolved to allow females to choose or otherwise determine mates which are more likely to result in viable offspring.

References

  1. 1 2 Bateson, Paul Patrick Gordon. "Mate Choice." Mate Choice, Cambridge University Press, 1985
  2. Petrie, Marion; Halliday, Tim; Sanders, Carolyn (1991). "Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate trains". Animal Behaviour. 41 (2): 323–331. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80484-1. S2CID   53201236.
  3. Dakin, Roslyn; Montgomerie, Robert (2013). "Eye for an eyespot: How iridescent plumage ocelli influence peacock mating success". Behavioral Ecology. 24 (5): 1048–1057. doi: 10.1093/beheco/art045 .
  4. 1 2 3 4 Andersson, Malte (1994). Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press.[ page needed ]
  5. 1 2 Moller, A.; Jennions, M. (2001). "How important are direct benefits of sexual selection?". Naturwissenschaften. 88 (10): 401–415. Bibcode:2001NW.....88..401M. doi:10.1007/s001140100255. PMID   11729807. S2CID   23429175.
  6. 1 2 Kokko, H.; Brooks, R.; Jennions, M.; Morley, J. (2003). "The evolution of mate choice and mating biases". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 270 (1515): 653–664. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2235. PMC   1691281 . PMID   12769467.
  7. 1 2 Dawkins, Marian; Guilford, Tim (Nov 1996). "Sensory Bias and the Adaptiveness of Female Choice". The American Naturalist. 148 (5): 937–942. doi:10.1086/285964. JSTOR   2463414. S2CID   83701069.
  8. Halliday, T. R (1983). "The study of mate choice". In Bateson, Patrick (ed.). Mate Choice. pp. 3–32. ISBN   978-0-521-27207-0.
  9. Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. John Murray, London.
  10. Miller, Geoffrey (2000). The mating mind: how sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature, London, Heinemann, ISBN   0-434-00741-2 (also Doubleday, ISBN   0-385-49516-1) p. 8
  11. Fisher, R.A. (1915). "The evolution of sexual preference". Eugenics Review. 7 (3): 184–192. PMC   2987134 . PMID   21259607.
  12. Bateman, A. (1948). "Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila". Heredity. 2 (Pt. 3): 349–368. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1948.21 . PMID   18103134.
  13. Williams, G.C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ[ page needed ]
  14. Williams, G.C. (1975). Sex and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ[ page needed ]
  15. Trivers, Robert L. (1972). "Parental Investment and Sexual Selection". In Campbell, Bernard Grant (ed.). Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971. pp.  136–179. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.100.4543 . ISBN   978-0-202-02005-1.
  16. 1 2 Dorsey OC, Rosenthal GG. A taste for the familiar: explaining the inbreeding paradox. Trends Ecol Evol. 2023 Feb;38(2):132-142. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2022.09.007. Epub 2022 Oct 11. PMID 36241551
  17. Puurtinen M. Mate choice for optimal (k)inbreeding. Evolution. 2011 May;65(5):1501-5. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01217.x. Epub 2011 Jan 5. PMID 21521199
  18. Price, T.; Schluter, D.; Heckman, N. (1993). "Sexual selection when the female directly benefits". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 48 (3): 187–211. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1993.tb00887.x.
  19. Moller, A.P. (1994). Sexual selection and the barn swallow. Oxford University Press, Oxford.[ page needed ]
  20. 1 2 Hill, Geoffrey E. (1991). "Plumage coloration is a sexually selected indicator of male quality". Nature. 350 (6316): 337–339. Bibcode:1991Natur.350..337H. doi:10.1038/350337a0. hdl: 2027.42/62564 . S2CID   4256909.
  21. Linville, Susan U.; Breitwisch, Randall; Schilling, Amy J. (1998). "Plumage brightness as an indicator of parental care in northern cardinals". Animal Behaviour. 55 (1): 119–127. doi:10.1006/anbe.1997.0595. PMID   9480678. S2CID   20776750.
  22. 1 2 3 4 Andersson, Malte; Simmons, Leigh W. (2006). "Sexual selection and mate choice". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 21 (6): 296–302. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.595.4050 . doi:10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.015. PMID   16769428.
  23. Boughman, Janette Wenrick (2002). "How sensory drive can promote speciation". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 17 (12): 571–577. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02595-8.
  24. Rodd, F. H.; Hughes, K. A.; Grether, G. F.; Baril, C. T. (2002). "A possible non-sexual origin of mate preference: Are male guppies mimicking fruit?". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 269 (1490): 475–481. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1891. PMC   1690917 . PMID   11886639.
  25. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Proctor, Heather C. (1991). "Courtship in the water mite Neumania papillator: Males capitalize on female adaptations for predation". Animal Behaviour. 42 (4): 589–598. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8. S2CID   53166756.
  26. 1 2 Proctor, Heather C. (1992). "Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour: A cladistic test using water mites (Acari: Parasitengona)". Animal Behaviour. 44 (4): 745–752. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8. S2CID   54426553.
  27. Proctor, H. C. (1992). "Effect of Food Deprivation on Mate Searching and Spermatophore Production in Male Water Mites (Acari: Unionicolidae)". Functional Ecology. 6 (6): 661–665. Bibcode:1992FuEco...6..661P. doi:10.2307/2389961. JSTOR   2389961.
  28. Alcock, John (2013-07-01). Animal Behaviour: A Evolutionary Approach (10th ed.). Sinauer. pp. 70–72. ISBN   978-0-87893-966-4.
  29. Jones, Ian L.; Hunter, Fiona M. (1998). "Heterospecific mating preferences for a feather ornament in least auklets". Behavioral Ecology. 9 (2): 187–192. doi:10.1093/beheco/9.2.187.
  30. McClintock, William J.; Uetz, George W. (1996). "Female choice and pre-existing bias: Visual cues during courtship in two Schizocosawolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae)". Animal Behaviour. 52: 167–181. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0162. S2CID   24629559.
  31. Prum, Richard O. (1997). "Phylogenetic Tests of Alternative Intersexual Selection Mechanisms: Trait Macroevolution in a Polygynous Clade (Aves: Pipridae)". The American Naturalist. 149 (4): 668–692. doi:10.1086/286014. S2CID   85039370.
  32. Fuller, Rebecca C.; Houle, David; Travis, Joseph (2005). "Sensory Bias as an Explanation for the Evolution of Mate Preferences". The American Naturalist. 166 (4): 437–446. doi:10.1086/444443. PMID   16224700. S2CID   4849390.
  33. Huk, T.; Winkel, W. (2007-12-18). "Testing the sexy son hypothesis--a research framework for empirical approaches". Behavioral Ecology. 19 (2): 456–461. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm150 . ISSN   1045-2249.
  34. Andersson, Malte (October 1982). "Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird". Nature. 299 (5886): 818–820. Bibcode:1982Natur.299..818A. doi:10.1038/299818a0. ISSN   0028-0836. S2CID   4334275.
  35. Biegler, Robert; Kennair, Leif Edward Ottesen (2016). "Sisterly love: Within-generation differences in ideal partner for sister and self". Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. 10: 29–42. doi:10.1037/ebs0000060.
  36. Zahavi, Amotz (1975). "Mate selection – A selection for a handicap". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 53 (1): 205–214. Bibcode:1975JThBi..53..205Z. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.586.3819 . doi:10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3. PMID   1195756.
  37. Hamilton, W.; Zuk, M. (1982). "Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites?". Science. 218 (4570): 384–387. Bibcode:1982Sci...218..384H. doi:10.1126/science.7123238. PMID   7123238.
  38. Zuk, Marlene; Thornhill, Randy; Ligon, J. David; Johnson, Kristine (1990). "Parasites and mate choice in red jungle fowl". American Zoologist. 30 (2): 235–244. doi: 10.1093/icb/30.2.235 .
  39. Puurtinen, M.; Ketola, T.; Kotiaho, J. (2005). "Genetic compatibility and sexual selection". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 20 (4): 157–158. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.02.005. PMID   16701361.
  40. Wedekind, Claus; Seebeck, Thomas; Bettens, Florence; Paepke, Alexander J. (1995). "MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 260 (1359): 245–249. Bibcode:1995RSPSB.260..245W. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0087. PMID   7630893. S2CID   34971350.
  41. Brennan, Peter A.; Zufall, Frank (2006). "Pheromonal communication in vertebrates". Nature. 444 (7117): 308–315. Bibcode:2006Natur.444..308B. doi:10.1038/nature05404. PMID   17108955. S2CID   4431624.
  42. Eizaguirre, C.; Yeates, S. E.; Lenz, T. L.; Kalbe, M.; Milinski, M. (2009). "MHC-based mate choice combines good genes and maintenance of MHC polymorphism". Molecular Ecology. 18 (15): 3316–3329. Bibcode:2009MolEc..18.3316E. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04243.x. PMID   19523111. S2CID   39051591.
  43. Reusch, Thorsten B. H.; Häberli, Michael A.; Aeschlimann, Peter B.; Milinski, Manfred (2001). "Female sticklebacks count alleles in a strategy of sexual selection explaining MHC polymorphism". Nature. 414 (6861): 300–302. Bibcode:2001Natur.414..300R. doi:10.1038/35104547. PMID   11713527. S2CID   4390637.
  44. Kalbe, M.; Eizaguirre, C.; Dankert, I.; Reusch, T. B.H; Sommerfeld, R. D.; Wegner, K. M.; Milinski, M. (2009). "Lifetime reproductive success is maximized with optimal major histocompatibility complex diversity". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1658): 925–934. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1466. PMC   2664370 . PMID   19033141.
  45. Nowak, M. A.; Tarczy-Hornoch, K.; Austyn, J. M. (1992). "The optimal number of major histocompatibility complex molecules in an individual". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 89 (22): 10896–10899. Bibcode:1992PNAS...8910896N. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.22.10896 . JSTOR   2362018. PMC   50449 . PMID   1438295.
  46. Woelfing, B.; Traulsen, A.; Milinski, M.; Boehm, T. (2009). "Does intra-individual major histocompatibility complex diversity keep a golden mean?". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1513): 117–128. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0174. PMC   2666699 . PMID   18926972.
  47. Ilmonen, P.; Penn, D. J.; Damjanovich, K.; Morrison, L.; Ghotbi, L.; Potts, W. K. (2007). "Major Histocompatibility Complex Heterozygosity Reduces Fitness in Experimentally Infected Mice". Genetics. 176 (4): 2501–2508. doi:10.1534/genetics.107.074815. PMC   1950649 . PMID   17603099.
  48. Schad, J.; Dechmann, D K N.; Voigt, C. C.; Sommer, S. (2011). "MHC class II DRB diversity, selection pattern and population structure in a neotropical bat species, Noctilio albiventris". Heredity. 107 (2): 115–126. doi:10.1038/hdy.2010.173. PMC   3178406 . PMID   21245894.
  49. Wegner, K. M.; Kalbe, M.; Rauch, G.; Kurtz, J.; Schaschl, H.; Reusch, T. B. H. (2006). "Genetic variation in MHC class II expression and interactions with MHC sequence polymorphism in three-spined sticklebacks". Molecular Ecology. 15 (4): 1153–1164. Bibcode:2006MolEc..15.1153W. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294x.2006.02855.x. PMID   16599974. S2CID   24676935.
  50. Schaschl, H.; Wandeler, P.; Suchentrunk, F.; Obexer-Ruff, G.; Goodman, S. J. (2006). "Selection and recombination drive the evolution of MHC class II DRB diversity in ungulates". Heredity. 97 (6): 427–437. doi: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800892 . PMID   16941019.
  51. Vincent, A.; Anhesjo, I.; Berglund, A.; Rosenqvist, G. (1992). "Pipefish and seahorses: Are they all sex role reversed?". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 7 (7): 237–241. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(92)90052-D. PMID   21236017.
  52. Wells, K. (1978). "Courtship and parental behavior in a Panamanian poison-arrow frog (Dendrobates auratus)". Herpetologica. 34 (2): 148–155. JSTOR   3891667.
  53. Emlen, S. T.; Wrege, P. H. (2005). "Sex dimorphism, intrasexual competition and sexual selection in wattled jacana, a sex role reversed shore bird in Panama". The Auk. 121 (2): 391–403. doi:10.1642/0004-8038(2004)121[0391:sdicas]2.0.co;2. JSTOR   4090403. S2CID   36550715.
  54. Eens, M.; Pinxten, R. (2000). "Sex-role reversal in vertebrates: behavioral and endocrinological accounts". Behavioural Processes. 51 (1–3): 135–147. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00124-8. PMID   11074317. S2CID   20732874.
  55. Glickman, S. E.; Frank, L. G.; Davidson, J. M.; Smith, E. R.; Siiteri, P. K. (1987). "Androstenedione may organize or activate sex-reversed traits in female spotted hyenas". PNAS. 84 (10): 3444–3447. Bibcode:1987PNAS...84.3444G. doi: 10.1073/pnas.84.10.3444 . PMC   304887 . PMID   3472215.
  56. Frank, L. G. (1986). "Social organization of the spotted hyena II: Dominance and reproduction". Animal Behaviour. 35 (5): 1510–1527. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80221-4. S2CID   53193864.
  57. Mayr, E. (1942). Systematics and Origin of Species, Belknap Press
  58. Ritchie, Michael G. (2007). "Sexual Selection and Speciation". Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 38: 79–102. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095733.
  59. Endler, J. A.; Houde, A. E. (1995). "Geographic variation in female preferences for male traits in Poecelia reticulata". Evolution. 49 (3): 456–468. doi:10.2307/2410270. hdl: 10536/DRO/DU:30094497 . JSTOR   2410270. PMID   28565093.
  60. Friberg, Magne; Wiklund, Christer (2008). "Host plant preference and performance of the sibling species of butterflies Leptidea sinapis and Leptidea reali: a test of the trade-off hypothesis for food specialisation". Oecologia. 159 (1): 127–137. doi:10.1007/s00442-008-1206-8. PMID   19002503. S2CID   158128.
  61. Colbeck, G.J.; Sillett, T.S.; Webster, M.S. (2010). "Asymmetric discrimination of geographical variation in song in a migratory passerine". Animal Behaviour. 80 (2): 311–318. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.013. S2CID   29972445.
  62. Barrett, Louise; Dunbar, Robin; Lycett, John (2002). Human Evolutionary Psychology. Hampshire: Palgrave. ISBN   978-0-333-72558-0.[ page needed ]
  63. Collins, Sarah A. (2000). "Men's voices and women's choices". Animal Behaviour. 60 (6): 773–780. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1523. PMID   11124875. S2CID   15165482.
  64. Barber, Nigel (1995). "The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology". Ethology and Sociobiology. 16 (5): 395–424. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(95)00068-2.
  65. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Buss, David (2016). Evolutionary Psychology, The New Science of Mind. New York: Routledge. pp. 103–104. ISBN   978-0-205-99212-6.
  66. B. A. Scelza, "Choosy But Not Chaste: Multiple Mating in Human Females".2013
  67. 1 2 Bode, Adam; Kushnick, Geoff (2021). "Proximate and Ultimate Perspectives on Romantic Love". Frontiers in Psychology. 12: 573123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.573123 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   8074860 . PMID   33912094.
  68. 1 2 Edward, Dominic A.; Chapman, Tracey (2011). "The evolution and significance of male mate choice". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 26 (12): 647–654. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.012. PMID   21890230.
  69. Buss, David (2016). Evolutionary Psychology, The New Science of Mind. New York: Routledge. pp. 163–176. ISBN   978-0-205-99212-6.
  70. Dixson, Barnaby J.; Duncan, Melanie; Dixson, Alan F. (2015). "The Role of Breast Size and Areolar Pigmentation in Perceptions of Women's Sexual Attractiveness, Reproductive Health, Sexual Maturity, Maternal Nurturing Abilities, and Age". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 44 (6): 1685–1695. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0516-2. PMID   25828990. S2CID   38591710.
  71. Buss, David (2016). Evolutionary Psychology, The New Science of Mind. New York: Routledge. pp. 133–162. ISBN   978-0-205-99212-6.
  72. Antfolk, Jan (2017). "Age Limits: Men's and Women's Youngest and Oldest Considered and Actual Sex Partners". Evolutionary Psychology. 15 (1): 147470491769040. doi: 10.1177/1474704917690401 . PMC   10367477 . PMID   28127998.
  73. Antfolk, Jan; Salo, Benny; Alanko, Katarina; Bergen, Emilia; Corander, Jukka; Sandnabba, N. Kenneth; Santtila, Pekka (2015). "Women's and men's sexual preferences and activities with respect to the partner's age: Evidence for female choice". Evolution and Human Behavior. 36: 73–79. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.003.
  74. Rowland, Hannah; Burriss, Robert (2017). "Human color in mate choice and competition". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 372 (1724): 20160350. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0350. PMC   5444069 . PMID   28533465.
  75. Campbell, Anne (2013): “A Mind Of Her Own: The evolutionary psychology of women”. OUP Oxford; 2nd edition, p. 213
  76. Hamilton, William D.; Zuk, Marlene (1982). "Heritable True Fitness and Bright Birds: A Role for Parasites?". Science. 218 (4570): 384–387. Bibcode:1982Sci...218..384H. doi:10.1126/science.7123238. JSTOR   1688879. PMID   7123238.
  77. Fincher, Corey; Thornhill, Randy; Murray, Damian; Schaller, Mark (7 June 2018). "Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 275 (1640): 1279–1285. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0094. PMC   2602680 . PMID   18302996.
  78. Cartwright, John (2000). Evolution and human behavior: Darwinian perspectives on human nature. Basingstoke: Macmillan. pp. 146–147. ISBN   978-0-333-71457-7.
  79. Ludvico, L.R.; Kurland, J.A. (1995). "Symbolic or not-so symbolic wounds: The behavioral ecology of human scarification". Ethology and Sociobiology. 16 (2): 155–172. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)00075-i.
  80. Singh, Devendra; Mathew, Bronstad (1997). "Sex differences in the anatomical locations of human body scarification and tattooing as a function of pathogen prevalence". Evolution and Human Behavior. 18 (6): 403–416. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00089-5 .
  81. DeBruine, Lisa M.; Jones, Benedict C.; Crawford, John R.; Welling, Lisa L. M.; Little, Anthony C. (2010). "The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 277 (1692): 2405–2410. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2184. PMC   2894896 . PMID   20236978.
  82. Jones, Benedict C.; Feinberg, David R.; Watkins, Christopher D.; Fincher, Corey L.; Little, Anthony C.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (2012). "Pathogen disgust predicts women's preferences for masculinity in men's voices, faces, and bodies". Behavioral Ecology. 24 (2): 373–379. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ars173 .
  83. Thornhill, R.; Gangestad, S. W.; Scheib, J. E. (1999). "Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 266 (1431): 1913–1917. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0866. PMC   1690211 . PMID   10535106.
  84. DeBruine, Lisa M.; Little, Anthony C.; Jones, Benedict C. (2012). "Extending parasite-stress theory to variation in human mate preferences". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 35 (2): 86–87. doi:10.1017/s0140525x11000987. hdl: 1893/17923 . PMID   22289354. S2CID   7420555.
  85. White, D. R.; Burton, M. L. (1988). "Causes of polygyny: Ecology, economy, kinship, and warfare". American Anthropologist. 90 (4): 871–887. doi:10.1525/aa.1988.90.4.02a00060.
  86. Low, Bobbi S. (1990). "Marriage Systems and Pathogen Stress in Human Societies". American Zoologist. 30 (2): 325–339. doi: 10.1093/icb/30.2.325 .
  87. Yamazaki, K.; Yamaguchi, M.; Baranoski, L.; Bard, J.; Boyse, E. A.; Thomas, L. (1979). "Recognition among mice. Evidence from the use of a Y-maze differentially scented by congenic mice of different major histocompatibility types". Journal of Experimental Medicine. 150 (4): 755–760. doi:10.1084/jem.150.4.755. PMC   2185685 . PMID   512584.
  88. Wedekind, C.; Fu¨ri, S. (1997). "Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 264 (1387): 1471–1479. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204. PMC   1688704 . PMID   9364787.
  89. Pause, B. M.; Krauel, K.; Schraders, C.; Sojka, B.; Westphal, E.; Muller-Ruchholtz, W.; Ferstl, R. (2005). "The human brain is a detector of chemosensorily transmitted HLA-class I-similarity in same- and opposite-sex relations". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 273 (1585): 471–478. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3342. PMC   1560206 . PMID   16615215.
  90. Probst, F., Fischbacher, U., Lobmaier, J. S., Wirthmüller, U., & Knoch, D. (2017). Men's preferences for women's body odours are not associated with human leucocyte antigen. Proceedings. Biological sciences, 284(1864), 20171830.
  91. Santos, Pablo; Schinemann, Juliano; Gabardo, Juarez; Bicalho, Maria (2005). "New evidence that the MHC influences odor perception in humans: a study with 58 Southern Brazilian students". Hormones and Behavior. 47 (4): 384–388. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.005. PMID   15777804. S2CID   8568275.
  92. Havlicek, Jan; Roberts, S. Craig (2009). "MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 34 (4): 497–512. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007. PMID   19054623. S2CID   40332494.
  93. Darwin, Charles (1872). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. By Charles Darwin. New York: D. Appleton and company. doi:10.5962/bhl.title.2112.
  94. Buss, David M. (March 1989). "Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 12 (1): 1–14. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00023992 . ISSN   0140-525X.
  95. Prokosch, Mark D.; Coss, Richard G.; Scheib, Joanna E.; Blozis, Shelley A. (January 2009). "Intelligence and mate choice: intelligent men are always appealing". Evolution and Human Behavior. 30 (1): 11–20. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.07.004. ISSN   1090-5138.
  96. 1 2 Miller, Geoffrey (2008-10-07), "Sexual Selection for Indicators of Intelligence", The Nature of Intelligence, Novartis Foundation Symposia, vol. 233, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 260–275, doi:10.1002/0470870850.ch16, ISBN   978-0-471-49434-8
  97. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Boogert, N. J.; Fawcett, T. W.; Lefebvre, L. (2011-05-01). "Mate choice for cognitive traits: a review of the evidence in nonhuman vertebrates". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (3): 447–459. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq173 . ISSN   1045-2249.
  98. Møller, A.; Jennions, M. (October 2001). "How important are direct fitness benefits of sexual selection?". Naturwissenschaften. 88 (10): 401–415. Bibcode:2001NW.....88..401M. doi:10.1007/s001140100255. ISSN   0028-1042. PMID   11729807. S2CID   23429175.
  99. Dukas, Reuven; Ratcliffe, John M. (2009). Cognitive Ecology II. University of Chicago Press. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.001.0001. ISBN   978-0-226-16936-1.
  100. Hollis, Brian; Kawecki, Tadeusz J. (2014-04-22). "Male cognitive performance declines in the absence of sexual selection". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 281 (1781): 20132873. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.2873. PMC   3953837 . PMID   24573848.
  101. 1 2 Chen, Jiani; Zou, Yuqi; Sun, Yue-Hua; Cate, Carel ten (2019-01-11). "Problem-solving males become more attractive to female budgerigars". Science. 363 (6423): 166–167. Bibcode:2019Sci...363..166C. doi: 10.1126/science.aau8181 . ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   30630929. S2CID   57825892.
  102. 1 2 Chantal, Véronique; Gibelli, Julie; Dubois, Frédérique (2016). "Male foraging efficiency, but not male problem-solving performance, influences female mating preferences in zebra finches". PeerJ. 4: e2409. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2409 . ISSN   2167-8359. PMC   5012330 . PMID   27635358.
  103. BORGIA, GERALD (May 1995). "Complex male display and female choice in the spotted bowerbird: specialized functions for different bower decorations". Animal Behaviour. 49 (5): 1291–1301. doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0161. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53185805.
  104. 1 2 Keagy, Jason; Savard, Jean-François; Borgia, Gerald (2009-10-01). "Male satin bowerbird problem-solving ability predicts mating success". Animal Behaviour. 78 (4): 809–817. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.011. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   17075449.
  105. 1 2 Keagy, Jason; Savard, Jean-François; Borgia, Gerald (2011-05-01). "Complex relationship between multiple measures of cognitive ability and male mating success in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus". Animal Behaviour. 81 (5): 1063–1070. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.018. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   18525893.
  106. 1 2 Snowberg, L. K.; Benkman, C. W. (2009). "Mate choice based on a key ecological performance trait". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 22 (4): 762–769. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01699.x . ISSN   1420-9101. PMID   19320795. S2CID   17884865.
  107. Benkman, Craig W. (2003). "Divergent Selection Drives the Adaptive Radiation of Crossbills". Evolution. 57 (5): 1176–81. doi:10.1554/0014-3820(2003)057[1176:dsdtar]2.0.co;2. ISSN   0014-3820. PMID   12836833. S2CID   422285.
  108. Pravosudov, Vladimir V.; Clayton, Nicola S. (2001-02-22). "Effects of demanding foraging conditions on cache retrieval accuracy in food-caching mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 268 (1465): 363–368. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1401. ISSN   0962-8452. PMC   1088615 . PMID   11270432.
  109. 1 2 3 Branch, Carrie L.; Pitera, Angela M.; Kozlovsky, Dovid Y.; Bridge, Eli S.; Pravosudov, Vladimir V. (2019). "Smart is the new sexy: female mountain chickadees increase reproductive investment when mated to males with better spatial cognition". Ecology Letters. 22 (6): 897–903. Bibcode:2019EcolL..22..897B. doi: 10.1111/ele.13249 . ISSN   1461-0248. PMID   30848044.
  110. 1 2 Shohet, A. J.; Watt, P. J. (October 2009). "Female guppiesPoecilia reticulataprefer males that can learn fast". Journal of Fish Biology. 75 (6): 1323–1330. Bibcode:2009JFBio..75.1323S. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02366.x. ISSN   0022-1112. PMID   20738617.
  111. 1 2 3 Spritzer, Mark D.; Meikle, Douglas B.; Solomon, Nancy G. (2005-05-01). "Female choice based on male spatial ability and aggressiveness among meadow voles". Animal Behaviour. 69 (5): 1121–1130. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.033. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53190570.
  112. Morris, Richard G. M. (2014-12-11), "The Watermaze", The Maze Book, Neuromethods, vol. 94, Springer New York, pp. 73–92, doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-2159-1_3, ISBN   978-1-4939-2158-4
  113. Jacobs, L. F.; Gaulin, S. J.; Sherry, D. F.; Hoffman, G. E. (1990-08-01). "Evolution of spatial cognition: sex-specific patterns of spatial behavior predict hippocampal size". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 87 (16): 6349–6352. Bibcode:1990PNAS...87.6349J. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.16.6349 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   54531 . PMID   2201026.
  114. Catchpole, Clive K. (1996-09-01). "Song and female choice: good genes and big brains?". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 11 (9): 358–360. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)30042-6. ISSN   0169-5347. PMID   21237878.
  115. Anderson, Rindy C.; Searcy, William A.; Peters, Susan; Hughes, Melissa; DuBois, Adrienne L.; Nowicki, Stephen (2017-03-01). "Song learning and cognitive ability are not consistently related in a songbird". Animal Cognition. 20 (2): 309–320. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1053-7. ISSN   1435-9456. PMID   27844219. S2CID   7632795.
  116. "Do smart songbirds always get the girl? Study tests the links between cognition, sexiness and male songbirds' ability to serenade". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2020-01-30.
  117. Griffin, Andrea S.; Guillette, Lauren M.; Healy, Susan D. (2015-04-01). "Cognition and personality: an analysis of an emerging field". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 30 (4): 207–214. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2015.01.012. ISSN   0169-5347. PMID   25736691.