Nominal (linguistics)

Last updated

In linguistics, the term nominal refers to a category used to group together nouns and adjectives based on shared properties. The motivation for nominal grouping is that in many languages nouns and adjectives share a number of morphological and syntactic properties. The systems used in such languages to show agreement can be classified broadly as gender systems, noun class systems or case marking, classifier systems, and mixed systems. [1] Typically an affix related to the noun appears attached to the other parts of speech within a sentence to create agreement. Such morphological agreement usually occurs in parts within the noun phrase, such as determiners and adjectives. Languages with overt nominal agreement vary in how and to what extent agreement is required.

Contents

History

The history of research on nominals dates back to European studies on Latin and Bantu in which agreement between nouns and adjectives according to the class of the noun can be seen overtly.

Latinate grammar tradition

Within the study of European languages, recognition of the nominal grouping is reflected in traditional grammar studies based on Latin, which has a highly productive marking system. Nominals can be seen in the shared morphemes that attach to the ends of nouns and adjectives and agree in case and gender. In the example below, 'son' and 'good' agree in nominative case because they are the subject of the sentence and at the same time they agree in gender because the ending is masculine. Likewise, 'the dog' and 'wild' share the same morphemes that show they agree in accusative case and masculine gender. In Latin agreement goes beyond nouns and adjectives.

Explanation- The good son loves the wild dog.
Latin:fīliusbonusamat(1)canem(2) (acc)ferocem(3) (acc).
English:[The] songood[he] loves[the] dogwild.

Bantuist grammar tradition

The earliest study of noun classes was conducted in 1659 on Bantu languages, [2] and this study has to this day undergone only very minor modifications. These alterations began with Wilhelm Bleek's Ancient Bantu which led to Proto-Bantu. [2] The following example is from the Bantu language Ganda. For nominal classes in Bantu, see below.

(1)mú-límí mú-néné
1-farmer 1-fat
[2]

Theory of word-classes

Although much of the research on nominals focuses on their morphological and semantic properties, syntactically nominals can be considered a "super category" which subsumes noun heads and adjective heads. This explains why languages that take overt agreement features have agreement in adjectives and nouns.

Chomsky's analysis

In Chomsky's 1970 [±V, ±N] analysis, words with the feature "plus noun" that are not verbs "minus verb", are predicted to be nouns, while words with the feature "plus verb" and "minus noun" would be verbs. Following from this, when a word has both characteristics of nouns and verbs we get adjectives. When a word lacks either feature, one logically gets prepositions. [3]

A cross table of features at the lexical level
+V–V
+NAdjectivesNouns
–NVerbsPrepositions

The following tree demonstrates that the category [+N] groups together nouns and adjectives.

The super category nominals Nominals.png
The super category nominals

This suggests English illustrates characteristics of nominals at a syntactic level because nouns and adjectives take the same complements at the head level. Likewise, verbs and prepositions take the same kinds of complements at the head level. This parallel distribution is predicted by the feature distribution of lexical items.

The complements taken by the phrases seen in parallel distribution X-bar theory heads and compliments.png
The complements taken by the phrases seen in parallel distribution

Cross-linguistic evidence

Slavic languages

In Russian, the nominal category contains nouns, pronouns, adjectives and numerals. These categories share features of case, gender, and number each of which are inflected with different suffixes. Nominals are seen as secondary inflection of agreement. Understanding the different noun classes and how they relate to gender and number is important because the agreement of adjectives will change depending on the type of noun. [4]

Example of nominal predicate:

'The girl is very beautiful' Девушка очень красив-а

Semantic noun class 1–5

Although there is not complete agreement about the categorization of noun classes in Russian, a common view breaks the noun classes up into five categories or classes, each of which gets different affixes depending on gender, case and number.

Noun class 1 refers to mass nouns, collective nouns, and abstract nouns.

examples: вода 'water', любовь 'love'

Noun class 2 refers to items with which the eye can focus on and must be non-active

examples: дом 'house', школа 'school'

Noun class 3 refers to non-humans that are active.

examples: рыба 'fish', чайка 'seagull'

Noun Class 4 refers to human beings that are not female.

examples: отец 'father, 'один' man

Noun Class 5 refers to human beings that are female.

examples: женщина "woman", мать 'mother'

Declensional noun class

Declensional class refers to the form rather than semantics.

Declinational class 1Declinational class 2Indeclinational
Noun ending in -CNoun ending in -aOther
MasculineFemaleNeuter

[4]

Morphological evidence

Nouns and adjectives inflect for case and gender.

In Russian, nominals occur when:

  • Adjectives and non-personal pronouns take the same agreement as their referent
  • Personal pronouns agree with the natural gender of the referent

Cases

Gender and class

Russian has three grammatical genders: masculine, feminine and neuter. Gender and class are closely related in that the noun class will reflect the gender marking a nominal will get. Reflecting gender in Russian is usually restricted to the singular with a few exceptions in the plural. Gender is reflected on both the noun and the adjective or pronoun. Gendered nominals are clearly reflected in anaphors and relative pronouns because even if there is no explicit inflection upon the nouns they inherit animacy, gender and number from their antecedent. [4]

Affixes identifying one gender

Number/CaseAffixGender
SG/NOM ACCMasc
SG/NOM ACC-oNeuter
SG INST-juFem
SG GEN-uMasc
SG LOCúMasc
SG LOCíFem
PL NOM'eMasc

Affixes linked with two genders

Number/CaseAffixGender
SG/NOM ACCMasc, Fem
SG NOM-aMasc, Fem
SG ACC-uMasc, Fem
SG INT-ojMasc, Fem
SG GEN-aMasc, Neut
SG DAT-uMasc, Neut
SG INST-omMasc, Neut
PL NOM/ACC-aMasc, Neut
Number

Russian has two numbers: singular and plural. Number is inherent to the noun so it is reflected by inflection on the noun and the agreeing nominals such as attributive adjectives, predicates and relative pronouns. There is only alteration of singular and plural between semantic classes 2–5 because class 1 does not distinguish between one or more than one. [4]

Adjectives

Adjectives agree with gender, case and number markings and consequently agree with the noun class.

Short form basic inflectional pattern

MascNeutFemPL
-o-a-i

[4]

Australian languages

Nominals are a common feature of Indigenous Australian languages, many of which do not categorically differentiate nouns from adjectives.

Some features of nominals in some Australian languages include:

Morphological evidence: Australian

  • Mayali has four major noun class prefixes which attach to items within the nominal phrase: masculine, feminine, vegetable, and neuter.

An example paradigm is given below, adapted from . [6] One can see that each of the nominal morphemes in each class attaches to both the nouns and the adjectives.

Type NounAdjectiveEnglish
masculinena-rangemna-kimuk'big boy'
femininengal-kohbanjngal-kimuk'big old-woman'
vegetableman-mimman-kimuk'big seed'
neuterkun-wardekun-kimuk'big rock'

Bantu languages

Nominal structures are also found in Bantu languages. These languages constitute a sub-set of the Niger-Congo languages in Africa. There are approximately 250 different varieties of Bantu. Within these languages, nouns have historically been classified into certain groups based on shared characteristics. For example, noun class 1 and 2 represent humans and other animate objects, while noun class 11 represents long thing objects, and abstract nouns. [7]

Bantu languages use different combinations of the approximately 24 different Proto-Bantu noun classes. [2] The language with the highest number of documented noun classes is Ganda, which utilizes 21 of the 24 noun classes. [2] This ranges all the way to zero, which is the case in Komo D23, whose noun class system has faded out over time. [2] Languages that have approximately six classes paired for singular and plural and about six other classes that are not paired (e.g. infinitive and locative classes) are classified as canonical noun class systems, systems that have many noun classes. [2] These systems are far more typical of Bantu languages than the alternative, reduced noun class systems, such as Komo D23 and other languages that have limited noun classes. [2]

Morphological evidence: Bantu

A common feature of Bantu languages is nominal gender class agreement. This agreement can also be described as an extensive system of concord. [2] For every noun class, there is a corresponding gender class prefix. The nominal gender prefixes are shown below, with the "Proto-Bantu" (i.e. historical) prefixes on the left-hand side, and modern day Sesotho prefixes on the right-hand side. Note that modern day Sesotho has lost many noun classes. [7] This is typical of many other Bantu languages, as well.

Proto-BantuSesothoProto-BantuSesotho
1: mo-mo-12: ka-
1a: øø13: to-
2: βa-ba-14: βo-bo-
2a: β̀o-bo-15: ko-ho-
3: mo-mo-16: pa-
4: me-me-17: ko-ho-
5: le-le-18: mo-
6: ma-ma-19: pi-
7: ke-se-20: ɣo
8: βi-/didi-21: ɣi
9: n-(N)-22: ɣa
10: di-n-di(N)-23: ɣe
11: lo-

[7]

As can be seen in the table above, in the Sesotho variety of Bantu (spoken mainly in South Africa) there are approximately 15 nominal gender class prefixes. In this language, nouns and adjectives share the same gender class prefix. Adjectives take a 'pre'-prefix in addition to the main prefix. The main prefix (the one closest to the adjective, which is in bold in the example below) agrees with the prefix attached to the noun, whereas the 'pre'-prefix does not always agree with the noun. [7]

(2)se-kolo se-se-holo 'big school' [7]

The following examples from Swahili demonstrate class agreement with the noun, adjective and verb. [2] The different classes of the nouns in Swahili dictate which prefix will also agree with the adjective and verb. [2] It is not always the case in Bantu languages that the verb has noun agreement in the form of nominals, or in any form, but in Swahili it is a good representation of how these prefixes travel across the associated words. [2] In the first Swahili example, the noun has the prefix m- because it is part of class 1 for human beings. The prefix m- then agrees with the adjective m-dogo. The verb agreement is different simply because the verb agreement for class 1 is a- rather than m-. The second example has the prefix ki- because the noun basket is part of class 7. Class 7 has the same prefix form for nouns, adjectives and verbs. [2]

(3)

M-toto

CL1.child

m-dogo

CL1.little

a-mefika

CL1.arrived

M-toto m-dogo a-mefika

CL1.child CL1.little CL1.arrived

The little child arrived [2]

(4)

Ki-kapu

CL7.basket

ki-dogo

CL7.little

ki-mefika

CL7.arrived

Ki-kapu ki-dogo ki-mefika

CL7.basket CL7.little CL7.arrived

The little basket arrived [2]

See also

Related Research Articles

In linguistics, declension is the changing of the form of a word, generally to express its syntactic function in the sentence, by way of some inflection. Declensions may apply to nouns, pronouns, adjectives, adverbs, and articles to indicate number, case, gender, and a number of other grammatical categories. Meanwhile, the inflectional change of verbs is called conjugation.

Esperanto is the most widely used constructed language intended for international communication; it was designed with highly regular grammatical rules, and as such is considered an easy language to learn.

In linguistics, a noun class is a particular category of nouns. A noun may belong to a given class because of the characteristic features of its referent, such as gender, animacy, shape, but such designations are often clearly conventional. Some authors use the term "grammatical gender" as a synonym of "noun class", but others consider these different concepts. Noun classes should not be confused with noun classifiers.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prefix</span> Affix which is placed before the stem of a word

A prefix is an affix which is placed before the stem of a word. Particularly in the study of languages, a prefix is also called a preformative, because it alters the form of the word to which it is affixed.

In grammar, a noun is a word that represents a concrete or abstract thing, such as living creatures, places, actions, qualities, states of existence, and ideas. A noun may serve as an object or subject within a phrase, clause, or sentence.

In grammar, a part of speech or part-of-speech is a category of words that have similar grammatical properties. Words that are assigned to the same part of speech generally display similar syntactic behavior, sometimes similar morphological behavior in that they undergo inflection for similar properties and even similar semantic behavior. Commonly listed English parts of speech are noun, verb, adjective, adverb, pronoun, preposition, conjunction, interjection, numeral, article, and determiner.

Wiyot or Soulatluk (lit. 'your jaw') is an Algic language spoken by the Wiyot people of Humboldt Bay, California. The language's last native speaker, Della Prince, died in 1962.

In linguistics, especially within generative grammar, phi features are the morphological expression of a semantic process in which a word or morpheme varies with the form of another word or phrase in the same sentence. This variation can include person, number, gender, and case, as encoded in pronominal agreement with nouns and pronouns. Several other features are included in the set of phi-features, such as the categorical features ±N (nominal) and ±V (verbal), which can be used to describe lexical categories and case features.

In linguistics, agreement or concord occurs when a word changes form depending on the other words to which it relates. It is an instance of inflection, and usually involves making the value of some grammatical category "agree" between varied words or parts of the sentence.

Tooro or Rutooro is a Bantu language spoken mainly by the Tooro people (Abatooro) from the Tooro Kingdom in western Uganda. There are three main areas where Tooro as a language is mainly used: Kabarole District, Kyenjojo District and Kyegegwa District. Tooro is unique among Bantu languages as it lacks lexical tone. It is most closely related to Runyoro.

Sesotho nouns signify concrete or abstract concepts in the language, but are distinct from the Sesotho pronouns.

The Sesotho parts of speech convey the most basic meanings and functions of the words in the language, which may be modified in largely predictable ways by affixes and other regular morphological devices. Each complete word in the Sesotho language must comprise some "part of speech."

This article presents a brief overview of the grammar of the Sesotho and provides links to more detailed articles.

Odia grammar is the study of the morphological and syntactic structures, word order, case inflections, verb conjugation and other grammatical structures of Odia, an Indo-Aryan language spoken in South Asia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Inflection</span> Process of word formation

In linguistic morphology, inflection is a process of word formation in which a word is modified to express different grammatical categories such as tense, case, voice, aspect, person, number, gender, mood, animacy, and definiteness. The inflection of verbs is called conjugation, and one can refer to the inflection of nouns, adjectives, adverbs, pronouns, determiners, participles, prepositions and postpositions, numerals, articles, etc, as declension.

The American linguist Joseph Greenberg (1915–2001) proposed a set of linguistic universals based primarily on a set of 30 languages. The following list is verbatim from the list printed in the appendix of Greenberg's Universals of Language and "Universals Restated", sorted by context.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">English nouns</span> Part of speech

English nouns form the largest category of words in English, both in terms of the number of different words and in terms of how often they are used in typical texts. The three main categories of English nouns are common nouns, proper nouns, and pronouns. A defining feature of English nouns is their ability to inflect for number, as through the plural –s morpheme. English nouns primarily function as the heads of noun phrases, which prototypically function at the clause level as subjects, objects, and predicative complements. These phrases are the only English phrases whose structure includes determinatives and predeterminatives, which add abstract specifying meaning such as definiteness and proximity. Like nouns in general, English nouns typically denote physical objects, but they also denote actions, characteristics, relations in space, and just about anything at all. Taken all together, these features separate English nouns from the language's other lexical categories, such as adjectives and verbs.

Mungbam is a Southern Bantoid language of the Lower Fungom region of Cameroon. It is traditionally classified as a Western Beboid language, but the language family is disputed. Good et al. uses a more accurate name, the 'Yemne-Kimbi group,' but proposes the term 'Beboid.'

Settla, or Settler Swahili, is a Swahili pidgin mainly spoken in large European settlements in Kenya and Zambia. It was used mainly by native English speaking European colonists for communication with the native Swahili speakers.

Swahili is a Bantu language which is native to or mainly spoken in the East African region. It has a grammatical structure that is typical for Bantu languages, bearing all the hallmarks of this language family. These include agglutinativity, a rich array of noun classes, extensive inflection for person, tense, aspect and mood, and generally a subject–verb–object word order.

References

Bibliography