Time-Place learning

Last updated

Time-place learning (TPL) is the process by which animals link events (e.g. finding food, encountering a predator) with both the location and time of occurrence. [1] It enables them to decide which locations to visit or to avoid based on previous experience and knowledge of the current time of day. TPL presumably allows animals to maximize their chances of finding resources (food, mates) and avoiding predators, increasing survival chances. TPL requires spatial memory and a sense of time. The latter may be based on external time-cues (Zeitgebers), or internally generated circadian rhythms ("biological clock"). TPL may fundamentally underlie episodic memory.

Contents

Insects

The first evidence for time-place learning in animals came from studies in the 1930s on honeybees, which could be trained to visit two different feeders, one in the morning and the other in the afternoon. [2] Subsequent work in the 1980s showed that only a few individuals in the colony were able to learn that task, and did so with more precision for the morning than for the afternoon feeding. [3] Honeybees can also be trained to recognize one visual pattern to obtain food in the morning, and another pattern to get food in the afternoon; when presented with both patterns simultaneously, the same bees choose the "morning" pattern in the morning and the "afternoon" pattern in the afternoon. [4]

The Neotropical ant Ectatomma ruidum is also capable of time-place learning. They can learn to visit a feeding site in the morning, another one at midday, and a third one in the afternoon. They follow this spatio-temporal pattern even when food is withheld on test days. They then stay at the feeder for the approximate duration that food was normally available before moving on, at the right time, to the next feeder. This shows that the ants do not rely on direct cues from the food itself, but use instead a learned association between daily time and place. [5]

Fish

In fish, time-place learning has been demonstrated in the golden shiner [6] and the inanga. [7] Golden shiners could be taught to seek food in one half of their aquarium in the morning, in the other half at midday, and back to the first half in the afternoon. They maintained this spatio-temporal pattern even when food was withheld on test days. The spatio-temporal pattern also shifted gradually over several days when the day-night cycle was suddenly shifted early by 6 h, as is typical of circadian rhythms.

Inangas could be taught to seek food in one half of their aquarium in the morning and in the other half in the afternoon, even on test days when food was withheld. However, they could not be taught to avoid one half in the morning and the other half in the afternoon in response to simulated attacks by a heron.

A test of time-place learning with young convict cichlids yielded negative results, for two as well as four time-place associations per day. [8] This was attributed to a low cost of travel between the aquarium corners in which the food was delivered, such that fish could quickly sample all corners sequentially without having to learn which exact corner gave food.

Birds

In birds, time-place learning has been confirmed in garden warblers, [9] [10] [11] starlings, [12] weavers, [13] and pigeons. [14] Garden warblers could learn to visit four rooms inside a large aviary, one during each quarter of a day. It took them only 11 days to learn, with 70% accuracy, to visit the correct room at the correct daily time to get food. The spatio-temporal pattern of visits was then maintained even when food was made available in all rooms at all times. As with circadian rhythms of activity, the spatio-temporal pattern of room visits shifted gradually over several days following a 6-h advance of the day-night cycle, and it ran freely with a non-24 h periodicity for up to 6 days when the birds were placed under constant 24-h dim light and constant food availability. Starlings can show similar patterns, with free-runs up to 11 days in constant dim light.

The insectivorous weaver bird Ploceus bicolor can also learn to associate four feeding rooms with four feeding times, and it maintains the correct spatio-temporal pattern even when one of the rooms is blocked on test days (the bird then waits for the next feeding time and visits the appropriate room for that time); however, room blocking disrupts the spatio-temporal pattern in the granivorous weaver bird Euplectes hordaceus , which suggests that time-place learning may be stronger in species for which food in nature is more likely to vary spatio-temporally (as is the case for insects, as opposed to grain).

Finally, pigeons can learn to peck one key to get food in the morning, and another key in the afternoon, and they maintain this pattern for four days in constant light.

Mammals

Laboratory rats have been taught to enter one arm of a maze in the morning and another in the afternoon, though only 63% of the animals could attain the criterion of nine correct choices over ten consecutive trials. [15] In a protocol not based on food acquisition, rats swimming in a tank could learn the location of one resting platform in the morning, and another in the afternoon. [16] [17] However, other studies have failed to find evidence of time-place learning in rats. Outcomes of time-place tests with rats seem to depend on what behaviors are measured to assess learning, and on the (sometimes too low) costs of not performing well. [18]

Faced with a choice of entering one of three arms at three different times of day, laboratory mice can learn which arm to enter at the correct time, be it to obtain food (a positive reinforcement) or to avoid receiving a mild electric shock (a negative reinforcement). [19] [20]

Related Research Articles

Ethology Scientific objective study of animal behaviour

Ethology is the scientific and objective study of animal behaviour, usually with a focus on behaviour under natural conditions, and viewing behaviour as an evolutionarily adaptive trait. Behaviourism as a term also describes the scientific and objective study of animal behaviour, usually referring to measured responses to stimuli or to trained behavioural responses in a laboratory context, without a particular emphasis on evolutionary adaptivity. Throughout history, different naturalists have studied aspects of animal behaviour. Ethology has its scientific roots in the work of Charles Darwin (1809-1882) and of American and German ornithologists of the late 19th and early 20th century, including Charles O. Whitman, Oskar Heinroth (1871-1945), and Wallace Craig. The modern discipline of ethology is generally considered to have begun during the 1930s with the work of Dutch biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907-1988) and of Austrian biologists Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch (1886-1982), the three recipients of the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Ethology combines laboratory and field science, with a strong relation to some other disciplines such as neuroanatomy, ecology, and evolutionary biology. Ethologists typically show interest in a behavioural process rather than in a particular animal group, and often study one type of behaviour, such as aggression, in a number of unrelated species.

Bee learning and communication Cognitive and sensory processes in bees

Bee learning and communication includes cognitive and sensory processes in all kinds of bees, that is the insects in the seven families making up the clade Anthophila. Some species have been studied more extensively than others, in particular Apis mellifera, or European honey bee. Color learning has also been studied in bumblebees.

Animal cognition intelligence of non-human animals

Animal cognition encompasses the mental capacities of non-human animals. The study of animal conditioning and learning used in this field was developed from comparative psychology. It has also been strongly influenced by research in ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology; the alternative name cognitive ethology is sometimes used. Many behaviors associated with the term animal intelligence are also subsumed within animal cognition.

Foraging Searching for wild food resources

Foraging is searching for wild food resources. It affects an animal's fitness because it plays an important role in an animal's ability to survive and reproduce. Foraging theory is a branch of behavioral ecology that studies the foraging behavior of animals in response to the environment where the animal lives.

Spatial memory Memory about ones environment and spatial orientation

In cognitive psychology and neuroscience, spatial memory is a form of memory responsible for the recording of information about one's environment and spatial orientation. For example, a person's spatial memory is required in order to navigate around a familiar city, just as a rat's spatial memory is needed to learn the location of food at the end of a maze. It is often argued that in both humans and animals, spatial memories are summarized as a cognitive map.

Waggle dance Particular figure-eight dance of the honey bee

Waggle dance is a term used in beekeeping and ethology for a particular figure-eight dance of the honey bee. By performing this dance, successful foragers can share information about the direction and distance to patches of flowers yielding nectar and pollen, to water sources, or to new nest-site locations with other members of the colony.

Explicit memory is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts. Explicit memory can be divided into two categories: episodic memory, which stores specific personal experiences, and semantic memory, which stores factual information. Explicit memory requires gradual learning, with multiple presentations of a stimulus and response.

Ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus

The ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus is a nucleus of the hypothalamus. "The ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) is a distinct morphological nucleus involved in terminating hunger, fear, thermoregulation, and sexual activity." This nuclear region is involved with the recognition of the feeling of fullness.

Diurnality

Diurnality is a form of plant or animal behavior characterized by activity during daytime, with a period of sleeping or other inactivity at night. The common adjective used for daytime activity is "diurnal". The timing of activity by an animal depends on a variety of environmental factors such as the temperature, the ability to gather food by sight, the risk of predation, and the time of year. Diurnality is a cycle of activity within a 24-hour period; cyclic activities called circadian rhythms are endogenous cycles not dependent on external cues or environmental factors. Animals active during twilight are crepuscular, those active during the night are nocturnal, and animals active at sporadic times during both night and day are cathemeral.

Latent learning

Latent learning is the subconscious retention of information without reinforcement or motivation. In latent learning, one changes behavior only when there is sufficient motivation later than when they subconsciously retained the information.

The study of time perception is a field within psychology, cognitive linguistics and neuroscience that refers to the subjective experience, or sense, of time, which is measured by someone's own perception of the duration of the indefinite and unfolding of events. The perceived time interval between two successive events is referred to as perceived duration. Though directly experiencing or understanding another person's perception of time is not possible, such a perception can be objectively studied and inferred through a number of scientific experiments. Some temporal illusions help to expose the underlying neural mechanisms of time perception.

Dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus

The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is a nucleus of the hypothalamus. It is involved in feeding, drinking, body-weight regulation and circadian activity. More specifically, it is a necessary component for the expression of numerous behavioral and physiological circadian rhythms. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus receives information from neurons and humors involved in feeding regulation, body weight and energy consumption, and then passes this information on to brain regions involved in sleep and wakefulness regulation, body temperature and corticosteroid secretion.

Horsfields bronze cuckoo Species of bird

Horsfield's bronze-cuckoo is a small cuckoo in the family Cuculidae. Its size averages 22g and is distinguished by its green and bronze iridescent colouring on its back and incomplete brown barring from neck to tail. What distinguishes the Horsfield's bronze-cuckoo from other bronze-cuckoos is its white eyebrow and brown eye stripe. The Horsfield's bronze-cuckoo is common throughout Australia preferring the drier open woodlands away from forested areas.

A chronotype is the behavioral manifestation of underlying circadian rhythms of myriad physical processes. A person's chronotype is the propensity for the individual to sleep at a particular time during a 24-hour period. Eveningness and morningness are the two extremes with most individuals having some flexibility in the timing of their sleep period. However, across development there are changes in the propensity of the sleep period with pre-pubescent children preferring an advanced sleep period, adolescents preferring a delayed sleep period and many elderly preferring an advanced sleep period.

Task allocation and partitioning is the way that tasks are chosen, assigned, subdivided, and coordinated within a colony of social insects. Task allocation and partitioning gives rise to the division of labor often observed in social insect colonies, whereby individuals specialize on different tasks within the colony. Communication is closely related to the ability to allocate tasks among individuals within a group. This entry focuses exclusively on social insects. For information on human task allocation and partitioning, see division of labour, task analysis, and workflow.

Episodic-like memory is the memory system in animals that is comparable to human episodic memory. The term was first described by Clayton & Dickinson referring to an animal’s ability to encode and retrieve information about ‘what’ occurred during an episode, ‘where’ the episode took place, and ‘when’ the episode happened. This ability in animals is considered ‘episodic-like’ because there is currently no way of knowing whether or not this form of remembering is accompanied by conscious recollection—a key component of Endel Tulving’s original definition of episodic memory.

Animals have many different tactics for defending themselves, depending on the severity of the threat they are encountering. Stages of threat vary along a spectrum referred to as the "predatory imminence continuum", spanning from low-risk (pre-encounter) to high-risk (interaction) threats. The main assumption of the predatory imminence continuum is that as threat levels increase, defensive response strategies change. During the pre-encounter period, an animal may engage in activities like exploration or foraging. But if the animal senses that a predator is nearby, the animal may begin to express species specific defense reactions such as freezing in an attempt to avoid detection by the predator. However, in situations where a threat is imminent, once the animal is detected by its predator, freezing may no longer be the optimal behaviour for survival. At this point, the animal enters the circa-strike phase, where its behaviour will transition from passive freezing to active flight, or even attack if escape is not possible.

Social learning refers to learning that is facilitated by observation of, or interaction with, another animal or its products. Social learning has been observed in a variety of animal taxa, such as insects, fish, birds, reptiles, amphibians and mammals.

Cognitive bias in animals is a pattern of deviation in judgment, whereby inferences about other animals and situations may be affected by irrelevant information or emotional states. It is sometimes said that animals create their own "subjective social reality" from their perception of the input. In humans, for example, an optimistic or pessimistic bias might affect one's answer to the question "Is the glass half empty or half full?"

Insect cognition describes the mental capacities and study of those capacities in insects. The field developed from comparative psychology where early studies focused more on animal behavior. Researchers have examined insect cognition in bees, fruit flies, and wasps. 

References

  1. Mulder, C.K.; Gerkema, M.P.; Van der Zee, E.A. (2013). "Circadian clocks and memory: Time-place learning". Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 6: 1–10. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2013.00008. PMC   3622895 . PMID   23596390.
  2. Wahl, O (1932). "Neue Untersuchungen über das Zeitgedachtnis der Bienen". Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie. 16: 529–589.
  3. Moore, D.; Siegfried, D.; Wilson, R.; Rankin, M.A. (1989). "The influence of time of day on the foraging behavior of the honeybee, Apis mellifera". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 4 (3): 305–325. doi:10.1177/074873048900400301.
  4. Zhang, S.; Schwarz, S.; Pahl, M.; Zhu, H.; Tautz, J. (2006). "Honeybee memory: a honeybee knows what to do and when". Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (22): 4420–4428. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02522 .
  5. Schatz, B.; Lachaud, J.-P.; Beugnon, G. (1999). "Spatio-temporal learning by the ant Ectatomma ruidum" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 202: 1897–1907.
  6. Reebs, S.G. (1996). "Time-place learning in golden shiners (Pisces: Notemigonus crysoleucas)". Behavioural Processes. 36: 253–262. doi:10.1016/0376-6357(96)88023-5. PMID   24896874.
  7. Reebs, S.G. (1999). "Time-place learning based on food but not on predation risk in a fish, the inanga (Galaxias maculatus)". Ethology. 105 (4): 361–371. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00390.x.
  8. Reebs, S.G. (1993). "A test of time-place learning in a cichlid fish". Behavioural Processes. 30 (3): 273–282. doi:10.1016/0376-6357(93)90139-I.
  9. Biebach, H.; Gordijn, M.; Krebs, J.R. (1989). "Time-and-place learning by garden warblers, Sylvia borin". Animal Behaviour. 37: 353–360. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90083-3.
  10. Krebs, J.R.; Biebach, H. (1989). "Time-place learning by garden warblers (Sylvia borin): route or map?". Ethology. 83 (3): 248–256. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1989.tb00532.x.
  11. Biebach, H.; Falk, H.; Krebs, J.R. (1991). "The effect of constant light and phase-shifts on a learned time-place association in garden warblers (Sylvia borin): hourglass or circadian clock?". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 6 (4): 353–365. doi:10.1177/074873049100600406.
  12. Wenger, D.; Biebach, H.; Krebs, J.R. (1991). "Free-running circadian rhythm of a learned feeding pattern in starlings". Naturwissenschaften. 78 (2): 87–89. doi:10.1007/bf01206266.
  13. Falk, H.; Biebach, H.; Krebs, J.R. (1992). "Learning a time-place pattern of food availability: a comparison between an insectivorous and a granivorous weaver species (Ploceus bicolor and Euplectes hordeaceus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 31: 9–15. doi:10.1007/bf00167811.
  14. Saksida, L.M.; Wilkie, D.M. (1994). "Time-of-day discrimination by pigeons, Columbia livia". Animal Learning & Behavior. 22 (2): 143–154. doi: 10.3758/bf03199914 .
  15. Means, L.W.; Ginn, S.R.; Arolfo, M.P.; Pence, J.D. (2000). "Breakfast in the nook and dinner in the dining room: time-of-day discrimination in rats". Behavioural Processes. 49: 21–33. doi:10.1016/s0376-6357(00)00068-1.
  16. Lukoyanov, N.V.; Pereira, P.A.; Mesquita, R.M.; Andrade, J.P. (2002). "Restricted feeding facilitates time-place learning in adult rats". Behavioural Brain Research. 134 (1–2): 283–290. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00036-0.
  17. Widman, D.R.; Sermania, C.M.; Genismore, K.E. (2004). "Evidence for time-place learning in the Morris water maze without food restriction but with increased response cost". Behavioural Processes. 67 (2): 183–193. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2004.04.001.
  18. Deibel, S.H.; Thorpe, C.M. (2013). "The effects of response cost and species-typical behaviors on a daily time-place task". Learning & Behavior. 41: 42–53. doi: 10.3758/s13420-012-0076-4 .
  19. Van der Zee, E.A.; Havekes, R.; Barf, R.R.; Hut, R.A.; Nijholt, I.M.; Jacobs, E.H.; Gerkema, M.P. (2008). "Circadian time-place learning in mice depends on Cry genes". Current Biology. 18 (11): 844–848. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.077.
  20. Mulder, C.K.; Papantoniou, C.; Gerkema, M.P.; Van der Zee, E.A. (2014). "Neither the SCN nor the adrenals are required for circadian time-place learning in mice". Chronobiology International. 31 (9): 1075–1092. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.944975. PMC   4219850 . PMID   25083974.