Calming signals

Last updated

Calming signals is a term conceived by Norwegian dog trainer and canine ethologist, Turid Rugaas, to describe the patterns of behavior used by dogs when interacting with each other in environments that cause heightened stress and when conveying their desires or intentions. [1] [2] The term has been used interchangeably with "appeasement signals." [3] [4] Calming signals, or appeasement signals, are communicative cues used by dogs to de-escalate aggressive encounters or to prevent the development of aggressive encounters completely. [5] Calming signals are performed by one dog (the sender) and directed towards one or more individual(s) (the recipient(s)), which could be dogs or individuals of other species, such as humans. [4] When calming signals are ignored, a dog may display warning signals of aggression, and this has the potential to escalate to outright conflict between individuals. [6]

Contents

The domestication of dogs by humans has significantly altered the behavioral patterns observed in ancestral species, such as the wolf (C. lupis). [4] Dogs have developed changes in body language, as well as changes in auditory and olfactory displays over the course of some 30,000 years, and many of these modified behavioral patterns, or calming signals, can differ in meaning depending on the intended signal receiver's species. [7] Calming signals can be released by an individual voluntarily, or they can be an involuntary response to environmental stimuli as a result of stress-induced changes to body chemistry, such as the release of an odour from the body when anxious. [7]

History

In the past, studies on social behavior in wolves have been used to provide insight on social behavior patterns in domesticated dogs. [4] Although the domesticated dog (C. familiaris) share a common ancestor with the wolves and may present certain similarities, distinct differences in morphology and in the environment in which the two species evolved can cause the transpiration of inaccurate conclusions about communication behavior patterns in domesticated dogs when applying knowledge gained by the study of wolves. [4] Thus, the grouping of ancestral and descendant species is not found to be an appropriate method for studying calming signals in domesticated species of dogs. [4]

The threshold for aggressive behavior in domestic dogs varies from that of wolves. [4] Most domestic dog breeds are less likely to engage in aggressive behavior than their ancestral counterparts, and are therefore more likely to display calming signals to defuse conflict. [4] In dog breeds that differ greatly from wolf morphology, like pugs, some visual signals will be absent or highly modified as they no longer have the physical capacity or means to convey these signals. [4] Neoteny can also account for the loss of certain visual signals in domestic dogs and the retention of novel signals over subsequent generations. [4] [6]

Types of Calming Signals

Dogs use visual, auditory, and olfactory indicators to communicate with both conspecifics and other species, such as humans. [7] The majority and most well-studied calming signals are visual, and are sometimes accompanied by auditory cues (i.e. a sharp whine accompanying a yawn). [4]

Examples of Visual Calming Signals

Examples of behaviors classified as calming signals: [1] [2] [4]

A dog displaying the lip/nose licking behavior. Dog body language licking nose (no food) drooped ears.jpg
A dog displaying the lip/nose licking behavior.

The behavioral patterns listed above have the potential to be used as calming signals, but are only classified as such in the appropriate context. [4] [6] For example, a dog lying down when resting would not be considered a calming signal, but a dog lying down when approached by another dog would be considered a calming signal. Thus, calming signals are context-dependent behavioral responses to a dog's environment. [6]

Not all calming signals have the same efficacy in de-escalating aggressive encounters or conveying a dog's intention and dogs preferentially display certain calming signals over others depending on external factors such as distance between the sender and the recipient of the signal, and familiarity of the recipient to the sender. [4] [6] A dog is most likely to display a calming signal when it is directly interacting with another dog and when the dogs are separated by a small distance. [4] Lip-licking is a calming signal whose use is noted to increase in frequency as the distance between the sender and the recipient decreases. [4] [6] However, sniffing the ground and yawning, which are both considered calming signals, are most often displayed when the distance between the sender and the recipient increases. [4] Calming signals that are most commonly displayed by dogs overall are freezing, licking of the nose, and turning of the body away from the source of the escalation (i.e. a dog baring its teeth or growling). [4]

Dog-Human Interactions

Domestic dogs display interspecific signaling, specifically towards humans. [6] [7] Because domestic dogs co-inhabit the same environment as their owners/handlers, humans are their principle social partners, [6] and there is a great level of interaction between the two. [7] Licking of the lips and looking away are calming signal-categorized behaviors that are used by dogs in both conspecific and heterospecific interactions, and in both instances are thought to be used to appease the recipient. [6] Lip licking is used as a greeting behavior to establish a peaceful basis for future interactions. [6]

Understanding canine calming signals is crucial to experiencing positive interactions with dogs. [8] [9] Children under the age of six are least likely to correctly interpret auditory and visual calming signals displayed by dogs and, in the US, younger children have a greater probability of becoming victims of a dog attack. [8] In cases where children cannot appropriately interpret and respond to calming signals, the dog-human interaction is likely to escalate, and the dog may exhibit aggressive behaviors, such as biting. [8]

Conspecific Interactions

Two dogs meeting. The brown dog is displaying calming behaviors (softening the eyes, reducing body size, etc.). 2008-06-18 Dogs meeting.jpg
Two dogs meeting. The brown dog is displaying calming behaviors (softening the eyes, reducing body size, etc.).
The dominant male shows peaceful intentions by licking his lips. Beschwichtigungssignal Hund.jpg
The dominant male shows peaceful intentions by licking his lips.

Calming signals are often used by dogs post-conflict to diffuse aggressive behaviors and to regain a peaceful social environment. [10] Dogs have evolved peacemaking social mechanisms to alleviate, prevent, or resolve conflicts. [10] Some of these behavior mechanisms are calming signals. [1] [2] [4] [10] Social groups of dogs display two types of post-conflict calming signal behavior patterns: the two opponents of the conflict display the calming signals (reconciliation), or between a third-party member of the social group and one opponent (third-party initiated post-conflict affiliation). [10]

Familiarity and distance between two individuals affects the frequency of use of calming signals and the types of calming signals used. [4] [6] Calming signals are used between dogs to prevent the escalation of an agonistic encounter. [3] [6] Intraspecific calming signals can be voluntary, such as licking the lips, or involuntary, such as the release of odors from glands during high-stress interactions. [7] In both cases, the recipient receives the signal, understands its meaning, and acts on this information, often taking action to mitigate the stressful environment by changing their body language or demeanor. [4]

Calming signals are not displayed in intraspecific interactions when the level of aggression or threat exceeds the aggression threshold of the sender. [6] In these cases, dogs are more likely to rely on submissive behaviors than calming signals. [3] [6] Calming signals are only useful to a dog when there is a great enough probability that the direction of the encounter can be changed to de-escalate aggression. [6]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog communication</span> Communication of dogs with other dogs and as well as humans

Dog communication is the transfer of information between dogs, as well as between dogs and humans. Behaviors associated with dog communication are categorized into visual and vocal. Visual communication includes mouth shape and head position, licking and sniffing, ear and tail positioning, eye gaze, facial expression, and body posture. Dog vocalizations, or auditory communication, can include barks, growls, howls, whines and whimpers, screams, pants and sighs. Dogs also communicate via gustatory communication, utilizing scent and pheromones.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog training</span> Practice of teaching behaviors to dogs

Dog training is a kind of animal training, the application of behavior analysis which uses the environmental events of antecedents and consequences to modify the dog behavior, either for it to assist in specific activities or undertake particular tasks, or for it to participate effectively in contemporary domestic life. While training dogs for specific roles dates back to Roman times at least, the training of dogs to be compatible household pets developed with suburbanization in the 1950s.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog aggression</span> Behavior in dogs

Dog aggression expressed by dogs is considered to be normal behaviour and various types of aggression are influenced by a dog's environment and genetic predisposition. Dogs commonly display possessive aggression when defending resources or themselves.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bark (sound)</span> Sound mainly produced by Dogs

A bark is a sound most often produced by dogs. Other animals that make this noise include, but are not limited to, wolves, coyotes, foxes, seals, and barking owls. The most common onomatopoeia in the English language for this sound is "woof.""Bark" is also a verb that describes the sound of many canids.

The alpha roll is a dog training technique that is considered outdated by many modern-day dog trainers. The theory behind the training method is that dogs are hierarchical animals. The technique is used to teach the dog that the trainer or owner of the dog is the pack leader. Methods include when a dog misbehaves to pin the dog on its back and held in that position, sometimes by the throat.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lick granuloma</span> Medical condition

A lick granuloma, also known as acral lick dermatitis, is a skin disorder found most commonly in dogs, but also in cats. In dogs, it results typically from the dog's urge to lick the lower portion of one of their legs.

The dog is a domesticated descendant of the wolf. Also called the domestic dog, it is derived from extinct Pleistocene wolves, and the modern wolf is the dog's nearest living relative. Dogs were the first species to be domesticated by hunter-gatherers over 15,000 years ago before the development of agriculture. Due to their long association with humans, dogs have expanded to a large number of domestic individuals and gained the ability to thrive on a starch-rich diet that would be inadequate for other canids.

Canine reproduction is the process of sexual reproduction in domestic dogs, wolves, coyotes and other canine species.

Growling is a low, guttural vocalization produced by animals as an aggressive warning but can also be found in other contexts such as playful behaviors or mating. Different animals will use growling in specific contexts as a form of communication. In humans, low or dull rumbling noises may also be emitted when they are discontent with something or they are angry, although this human sound is often termed "groaning".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog behavior</span> Internally coordinated responses of dogs to internal and external stimuli

Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli. It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles. As a result of this physical and social evolution, dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans. Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social-cognitive abilities in domestic dogs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Laughter in animals</span> Overview of humor in animals

Laughter in animals other than humans describes animal behavior which resembles human laughter.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Separation anxiety in dogs</span>

Separation anxiety in dogs describes a condition in which a dog exhibits distress and behavior problems when separated from its handler. Separation anxiety typically manifests within minutes of departure of the handler. It is not fully understood why some dogs suffer from separation anxiety and others do not. The diagnosis process often leads to a misdiagnosis as it is difficult to differentiate from other medical and behavioral problems. The behavior may be secondary to an underlying medical condition. With chronic stress, impairments to physiological health can manifest. Increased stress in the dog alters hormone levels, thus decreasing natural immunity to various health problems.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Free-ranging dog</span> Dog not confined to a yard or house

A free-ranging dog is a dog that is not confined to a yard or house. Free-ranging dogs include street dogs, village dogs, stray dogs, feral dogs, etc., and may be owned or unowned. The global dog population is estimated to be 900 million, of which around 20% are regarded as owned pets and therefore restrained.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human–canine bond</span> Interspecies relationship

Human–canine bonding is the relationship between dogs and humans. This relationship can be traced back to at least 15,000 years ago, to the Bonn-Oberkassel dog, who was found buried alongside two humans. For centuries, dogs have been considered man's best friend. This is most evident in western countries, such as the United States, where over 48% of households have a pet dog.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat–dog relationship</span> Interaction between the main house pets

Cats and dogs have a range of interactions. The natural instincts of each species lead towards antagonistic interactions, though individual animals can have non-aggressive relationships with each other, particularly under conditions where humans have socialized non-aggressive behaviors.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Puppy cat</span> Behavioral pattern found in domestic cats

Puppy cat is a term used to refer to specific breeds of domestic cats that have unusual behavioral tendencies that are reminiscent of young domestic dogs. These are within the scope of feline behavior, and may be enhanced through selective breeding. These behaviors, not specific to any breed, include following people around from room to room, the desire to receive frequent moments of physical affection such as being held and petted, a lack of aggression toward some fellow animals, and a placid nature. While these attributes are found desirable for owners interacting with their cats, problems can occur when the felines are exposed to dogs and people who wish to harm them, with the cats possibly being too trusting and too friendly for their own good. Breeds known for these canine behaviors include the Abyssinian, Burmese, Maine Coon, Manx, and Ragdoll, though dog behavior traits can appear regardless of breed.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Turid Rugaas</span> Norwegian dog trainer

Turid Rugaas is a Norwegian dog trainer. Rugaas got her first dog in 1948 and has been training since 1969. She set up her dog school Hagan Hundeskole in 1984 on her farm on overlooking the fjords of Norway. She started educating other dog trainers and giving seminars 1992. Since then conducted workshops in 12 different countries with students from 24 countries. She is now retired from actual dog training, but continues to conduct seminars and workshops internationally.

The body language of dogs is one form of non-verbal communication whereby dogs can express emotions and intentions through bodily movements. It refers to the interpretation of posture and behaviour of species in the genus Canis. This form of visual communication is generally used for identifying emotions and intentions of domestic dogs, though it can also be applied to wild canines such as wolves. Understanding the body language of dogs is particularly important in preventing dog bites, especially of children. This communication can occur between dogs, or during a dog-human interaction. Such movements primarily involve the tail, the ears, and the head/body. Tail-wagging is a common tail movement used by dogs to communicate. Additionally, ear flattening or heightening are typical movements made using the ears. In terms of the head/body, it is of interest to study turning of the head, as well as the overall posture of the dog.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fly biting</span>

Fly biting refers to episodes of intentional focused biting at the air, as if the dog is biting at imaginary flies. Cavalier King Charles Spaniels appear to be predisposed to fly catching syndrome, though it has been documented in many different breeds and mixes. Age of onset is varied.

References

  1. 1 2 3 Rugaas, Turid. "Calming Signals - The Art of Survival".
  2. 1 2 3 Rugaas, Turid (2006). On Talking Terms with Dogs: Calming Signals (2nd ed.). Wenatchee, Wash.: Dogwise Pub. ISBN   1929242360.
  3. 1 2 3 Overall, Karen (2017). "Appeasement, calming signals, and information capture: how do our subjects tell us what matters to them?". Journal of Veterinary Behaviour . 19: v–viii. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2017.04.001 via Elsevier Science Direct.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 Mariti, Chiara; Falaschi, Caterina; Zilocchi, Marcella; Fatjó, Jaume; Sighieri, Claudio; Ogi, Asahi; Gazzano, Angelo (2017). "Analysis of the intraspecific visual communication in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris): A pilot study on the case of calming signals". Journal of Veterinary Behaviour . 18: 49–55. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2016.12.009. hdl: 11568/835281 . S2CID   151850637 via Elsevier.
  5. Mariti, C.; Falaschi, C.; Zilocchi, M.; Carlone, B.; Gazzano, A. (November 2014). "Analysis of calming signals in domestic dogs: Are they signals and are they calming?". Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 9 (6): e1–e2. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.008.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Firnkes, Angelika; Bartels, Angela; Bidoli, Emilie; Erhard, Michael (2017-05-01). "Appeasement signals used by dogs during dog–human communication". Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 19: 35–44. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2016.12.012. ISSN   1558-7878. S2CID   151351008.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Siniscalchi, Marcello; d’Ingeo, Serenella; Minunno, Michele; Quaranta, Angelo (2018-07-31). "Communication in Dogs". Animals. 8 (8): 131. doi: 10.3390/ani8080131 . ISSN   2076-2615. PMC   6116041 . PMID   30065156.
  8. 1 2 3 Eretová, Petra; Chaloupková, Helena; Hefferová, Marcela; Jozífková, Eva (2020-01-13). "Can Children of Different Ages Recognize Dog Communication Signals in Different Situations?". International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health . 17 (2): 506. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17020506 . ISSN   1660-4601. PMC   7014456 . PMID   31941151.
  9. Hall, Sophie S.; Brown, Beverley J.; Mills, Daniel S. (2019-09-26). "Developing and Assessing the Validity of a Scale to Assess Pet Dog Quality of Life: Lincoln P-QoL". Frontiers in Veterinary Science . 6: 326. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00326 . ISSN   2297-1769. PMC   6775215 . PMID   31616681.
  10. 1 2 3 4 Cools, Annemieke K.A.; Van Hout, Alain J.-M.; Nelissen, Mark H. J. (2008-01-08). "Canine Reconciliation and Third-Party-Initiated Postconflict Affiliation: Do Peacemaking Social Mechanisms in Dogs Rival Those of Higher Primates?: Canine Reconciliation and Third-Party-Initiated Postconflict Affiliation". Ethology. 114 (1): 53–63. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01443.x.