Gesticulation in Italian

Last updated

Hand gestures are used in regions of Italy and in the Italian language as a form of nonverbal communication and expression. The gestures within the Italian lexicon are dominated by movements of the hands and fingers, but may also include movements of facial features such as eyebrows, the mouth and the cheeks. [1]

Contents

Theories persist as to the exact origin of hand gestures as a method of communication in Italy, though they likely emerged through necessity as a universal, non-verbal method of communicating across different Italian local languages and dialects. [2] Despite the majority of today's Italian population speaking Italian, hand gestures have persisted as a method of expression to accompany verbal speech in many regions of Italy.

Around 251 specific hand gestures have been identified, with the belief that they developed during a period of occupation in which seven main groups are believed to have taken root in Italy: the Germanic tribes (Vandals, Ostrogoths and Lombards), Moors, Normans, French, Spaniards, and Austrians. Given that there was no common language, rudimentary sign language may have developed, forming the basis of modern-day hand gestures.[ citation needed ]

History

The precise origin of hand gestures as a popular component of Italian communication is still contested. De Jorio interprets the endurance of hand gestures in Southern Italy in particular as a cultural legacy of the Romans, who used the art of chironomia in everyday communication and in oratory. [3]

The development of hand gestures is closely connected with a communicative phenomenon, [4] and as a non-verbal communication system, could not have been formed within a short period. Amid early urbanization in Italy, [5] the increasing demand for communication stimulated the wide use of hand gestures in Italy, as did colonization and the resulting cultural fusion and language barrier. Ancient Greek colonization of the Mediterranean coast, including southern Italy, can be traced back via research to the early eighth century BC. [6] After the fall of the Western Roman Empire, local languages did not assume a dominant position over one another due to the arrival of new immigrants and colonizers from other regions. [7] Data indicates seven main groups: the Carolingians, the Visigoths, the Normans, the Saracens, the Germanic tribes, the French, and the Austrians. [7] Additionally, an extant funeral stone from the 5th century BC (currently in the Pergamon Museum in Berlin) depicts two soldiers shaking hands. [8] This is believed to be proof of gestures in Ancient Greek culture, which passed to and affected Italian non-verbal communication through generations even more than spoken language. To the 21st century, around 250 hand gestures Italians use in everyday conversation have been identified. [9]

The irreplaceable role of gestures in medieval and Renaissance societies is referred to as "une civilisation du geste" ("a civilisation of gesture") by Jacques Le Goff. [10] [ page needed ] As the Renaissance emphasized the restoration of human nature in the classical era with the aim of breaking the shackles of religion, [11] people became more willing to express themselves and attract others' attention. [12] Hand gestures allowed those using them to express themselves to a more satisfactory degree, making the Renaissance a vital era for the development of Italian hand gestures.

Hand gestures were particularly conspicuous in Italy during the early modern period. [3] This may be due to the emergence of large, highly populated city-states throughout Italy, such as Florence and Naples, in which people were compelled to make themselves more understood. [13] The more frequent usage of hand gestures by individuals living in, or raised in, Italian cities is still observable in a contemporary context.

Gesture frontier

A "gesture frontier" exists in Italy which separates the gestures used commonly in Southern Italy from those used in Northern Italy. [3] This frontier is evident in the differing meaning of the 'chin flick' gesture. In Northern Italy, this gesture generally means 'get lost', whereas in Southern Italy it simply means 'no'. [14] According to Morris, [14] this is due to the ancient Greek colonisation of Southern Italy, as Greeks also use the 'chin flick' gesture to mean 'no'. A study conducted in central Italy proved this gesture frontier to be true; despite the mobility of the Italian population and the existence of nationwide media, the majority of the northern Roman population used the 'chin flick' with the Northern meaning, and the southern Neapolitan population used the Southern meaning. [14]

This separation is evident between Northern and Southern Europe as well as within Italy; for instance, speakers of English and Dutch generally use gesticulations considerably less in their speech than Italians and Greeks. [3] The heavy use of gestures in communication has historically been considered indicative of a lack of civilisation in Southern European cultures by Northern Europeans. [3]

Role of gestures in communication

The continuation of hand gestures as a part of the Italian lexicon can be best understood as a form of cultural coding, as Italian children unconsciously imitate their parents and peers' behaviours, causing them to develop gesticulating during conversation as an involuntary habit. [15]

The use of hand gestures has always served a dual purpose in Italian culture; a substantive purpose which contributes expression to verbal communication and indicates emotion, and a pragmatic purpose which can serve as a substitute to verbal communication. [2]

In a contemporary context, hand gestures are primarily used amongst Italians as a form of expression to accompany conversation rather than a substitute for verbal communication. [16] The prevalence of hand gestures in communication in large Italian cities is thought to be due to competition, as individuals unconsciously wish to be more visible and take up more space in a busy urban setting by adding physical elements to their communication. [1]

Communication versus information

A pointing Uncle Sam is an example of strong non-verbal communication; this gesture is seen as impolite in many countries Uncle Sam (pointing finger).png
A pointing Uncle Sam is an example of strong non-verbal communication; this gesture is seen as impolite in many countries

Communication

Communicative gestures could also be referred to as "active" gestures, since they are performed on purpose by individuals. For example, a speaker who is enthusiastic to deliver important information to their audience might emphasize hand gestures rather than speech. [17] An example is J.M. Flagg's poster of Uncle Sam, who points his index finger directly towards the viewer in what is seen as a strong expression. [18]

Information

Informative or "passive" gestures refer to hand movements that are not necessary or meaningful to the conversation, such as scratching, adjusting one's clothing, and tapping. [17] Since this part of gesture does not focus on communication, it usually does not involve extra verbal communication.

Classification of gestures

There are two main ways to classify Italian hand gestures. The first way is to distinguish them via their use cases, such as during religious rites, at gladiatorial arenas, and in daily conversation. [19] Another way is to differentiate communicative and informative hand gestures in the Italian language. [20] These two types of gestures might occur automatically, whereas the informative-communicative dichotomy is used to explore the actual intent of use behind the conversation. [20]

Icons of the Annunciation Ohrid annunciation icon.jpg
Icons of the Annunciation

Religious rites

In the oldest surviving images of the Annunciation, the Archangel Gabriel is generally found raising his hand before mentioning something important, a gesture widely adopted by Roman rhetoricians for the same purpose. [21]

Gladiatorial arenas

Pollice Verso (1872), which popularized the thumbs-down gesture. Oil on canvas, Phoenix Art Museum Jean-Leon Gerome Pollice Verso.jpg
Pollice Verso (1872), which popularized the thumbs-down gesture. Oil on canvas, Phoenix Art Museum

More recent research suggests that the thumbs-up and thumbs-down gestures originated from Roman gladiatorial arenas, being used to decide the destiny of the loser of a fight. [22] The loser would beg for mercy to the crowd, who would decide his fate by pointing their thumbs up or down. If the gladiator received more thumbs up than thumbs down, then he was to be spared. A majority of thumbs down, on the other hand, signified execution. [23] However, there is still controversy amongst scholars about the exact meanings of thumbs-up and thumbs-down in ancient Rome.

Daily routines

The habit of talking with one's hands in Italy has been reported to address and reinforce the meanings of various expressions. In normal conversation, gesturing helps in delivering the meaning and receiving information, an example being up-down movement of the hand. When begging for help, an Italian may press their palms together as if praying. Due to differences in local context and cultural background, Italian hand gestures vary among regions.

Basic gestures

Michael Pena performing the "Che vuoi?" gesture at Lucca Comics & Games (2018) Diego Luna and Michael Pena - Lucca Comics & Games 2018 04.jpg
Michael Peña performing the "Che vuoi?" gesture at Lucca Comics & Games (2018)

The following section introduces some common and useful gestures used regularly in Italian conversation with words described. [24]

Benefits

Gesticulation is often regarded as having a positive impact on expression for the speaker and understanding for the listener. [27] Hand gestures often mirror and illustrate the speaker’s internal thoughts. Neuroimaging research shows that areas involved in language production, such as Broca’s area, are co-activated during gesture, which suggests neurological link between gesture and cognitive processes. [28] [29] Gestures also render communication more effective and support comprehension by embodying action and structuring ideas, which has many benefits, particularly in the context of education. [30] Cognitive psychology research indicates that congruent gestures can deepen understanding by visually representing the sequence and causality of concepts and thus effectively acts as a "second language" that complements speech. [31]

Longitudinal studies demonstrate that the number of distinct gesture meanings conveyed by toddlers at around 14 months of age significantly predicts vocabulary size at 42 to 54 months. For instance, one study found that gesture variety at 14 months explained an additional 32% of variance in vocabulary at age 3.5, even after accounting for early spoken words. [32] [33] Another study confirmed that more gesturing at 14 months was linked to increased school readiness and greater vocabulary at 54 months. [34] Research studying a group of children aged from 18 to 42 months found that early gesture-speech combinations (e.g. pointing while naming an object) was a better predictor of sentence complexity, whereas gesture variety was a more accurate predictor of vocabulary growth. [35] Pointing and other referential gestures (deictic gestures) appear as early as 8–10 months and consistently predict later language development, including the onset of first words and two-word combinations. [36] [37] In addition, research shows that gesture use is influenced by caregiver behaviour and family background. Children from higher socioeconomic households tend to gesture more, which correlates with larger vocabulary size; gesture use mediates about 40% of the disparity in vocabulary due to socioeconomic status. [38]

Socially, people who gesture in conversation are often associated with enthusiasm and sociable personality. Gesturing plays an important role in public speaking, particularly in emphasising ideas and influencing the audience's perception of the speaker. [39]

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Cocozza, Paula (2013-07-02). "A crash course in Italian hand gestures". The Guardian. Retrieved 2019-04-09.
  2. 1 2 Kendon, Adam (March 1995). "Gestures as illocutionary and discourse structure markers in Southern Italian conversation". Journal of Pragmatics. 23 (3): 247–279. doi:10.1016/0378-2166(94)00037-F.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 Bremmer, Jan; Roodenburg, Herman (1993). A cultural history of gesture. Polity Press. ISBN   0745607861. OCLC   466066031.
  4. Blake, Joanna; Vitale, Grace; Osborne, Patricia; Olshansky, Esthe (2005). "A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Communicative Gestures in Human Infants during the Transition to Language". Gesture. 5 (1–2): 201–217. doi:10.1075/gest.5.1-2.14bla. ISSN   1568-1475.
  5. Attema, P. A. J (2004). Centralization, early urbanization, and colonization in first millennium B.C.Greece and Italy. Leuven; Dudley, MA: Peeters.
  6. Rathmann, H.; Semerari, G. Saltini; Harvati, K. (2017). "Evidence for Migration Influx into the Ancient Greek Colony of Metaponto: A Population Genetics Approach Using Dental Nonmetric Traits". International Journal of Osteoarchaeology. 27 (3): 453–464. doi:10.1002/oa.2569. ISSN   1099-1212.
  7. 1 2 "Italian Hand Gestures: Speaking Like a Native, Without Words". GRAND VOYAGE ITALY. Retrieved 2019-05-13.
  8. "From the handshake to the high-five: a brief history of gestures". History Extra. Retrieved 2019-05-07.
  9. Brennan, Linda L. (2000-09-30). "Let Your Fingers Do the Talking: Conducting Class with "Chat"". Journal of Educational Technology Systems. 29 (1): 21–30. doi:10.2190/5qgv-efkp-9ygl-kqfa. ISSN   0047-2395. S2CID   62765932.
  10. Revel, Jacques; Schmitt, Jean Claude, eds. (1998). L'ogre historien: autour de Jacques Le Goff (in French). Editions Gallimard. ISBN   978-2-07-075089-4. OCLC   803411090.
  11. "The Italian View of Renaissance Italy", Renaissance Essays, Bloomsbury Academic, 1988, doi:10.5040/9781472599735.ch-018, ISBN   9781472599735
  12. S. Celenza, Christopher (2013-01-15). "What Counted as Philosophy in the Italian Renaissance? The History of Philosophy, the History of Science, and Styles of Life". Critical Inquiry. 39 (2): 367–401. doi:10.1086/668530. ISSN   0093-1896. S2CID   224798297.
  13. Donadio, Rachel (2013-06-30). "When Italians Chat, Hands and Fingers Do the Talking". The New York Times . ISSN   0362-4331 . Retrieved 2019-03-30.
  14. 1 2 3 Morris, Desmond (1977). People Watching: The Guide to Body Language. Great Britain: Random House. p. 73. ISBN   9780099429784.
  15. Salinas, Ricardo B. (2013-07-23). "The Culture Code". HuffPost. Retrieved 2019-05-08.
  16. Poggi, Isabella (2001). "The Lexicon and the Alphabet of Gesture, Gaze, and Touch". In de Antonio, Angélica; Aylett, Ruth; Ballin, Daniel (eds.). Intelligent Virtual Agents. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 2190. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 235–236. doi:10.1007/3-540-44812-8_20. ISBN   9783540448129.
  17. 1 2 Krauss, Robert M.; Chen, Yihsiu; Chawla, Purnima (1996), "Nonverbal Behavior and Nonverbal Communication: What do Conversational Hand Gestures Tell Us?", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Elsevier, pp. 389–450, doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60241-5, ISBN   9780120152285
  18. Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Brentari, Diane (2015-10-05). "Gesture, sign, and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 40: e46. doi:10.1017/s0140525x15001247. ISSN   0140-525X. PMC   4821822 . PMID   26434499.
  19. Corbeill, Anthony (5 June 2018). Nature Embodied: Gesture in Ancient Rome. Princeton University Press. ISBN   9780691187808. OCLC   1076414916.
  20. 1 2 Abner, Natasha; Cooperrider, Kensy; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2015). "Gesture for Linguists: A Handy Primer". Language and Linguistics Compass. 9 (11): 437–451. doi:10.1111/lnc3.12168. ISSN   1749-818X. PMC   4721265 . PMID   26807141.
  21. Esparza, Daniel (2016-06-12). "What Do the Hand Gestures in Icons Mean?". Aleteia — Catholic Spirituality, Lifestyle, World News, and Culture. Retrieved 2019-05-07.
  22. Corbeill, Anthony (1997). "Thumbs in Ancient Rome: "Pollex" as Index". Memoirs of the American Academy in Rome. 42: 1–21. doi:10.2307/4238745. ISSN   0065-6801. JSTOR   4238745.
  23. "V. From Eternity to Five-Year Plans", A Very Brief History of Eternity, Princeton University Press, 2010, pp. 157–219, doi:10.1515/9781400831876-007, ISBN   9781400831876
  24. gonomadtravel (2010-06-01). "Italian Hand Gestures in Conversation". GoNOMAD Travel. Retrieved 2019-05-07.
  25. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Marchetti, Silvia (2015-05-29). "Italian hand gestures everyone should know". CNN. Retrieved 2019-05-09.
  26. "Scalia's Italian gesture confused with obscenity". NBC News. March 28, 2006. Retrieved 17 May 2022.
  27. Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Alibali, Martha Wagner (2013). "Gesture's role in speaking, learning, and creating language". Annual Review of Psychology. 64: 257–283. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143802. ISSN   0066-4308. PMC   3642279 . PMID   22830562.
  28. Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Beilock, Sian (2010-12-07). "Action's influence on thought: The case of gesture". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 5 (6): 664–674. doi:10.1177/1745691610388764. ISSN   1745-6916. PMC   3093190 . PMID   21572548.
  29. Gregoire, Carolyn (2016-02-05). "The Fascinating Science Behind 'Talking' With Your Hands". Huffington Post. Retrieved 2019-05-27.
  30. Kang, Seokmin; Tversky, Barbara (2016-09-22). "From hands to minds: Gestures promote understanding". Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 1 (1): 4. doi: 10.1186/s41235-016-0004-9 . ISSN   2365-7464. PMC   5256437 . PMID   28180155.
  31. MORETT, LAURA M. (2014). "When Hands Speak Louder Than Words: The Role of Gesture in the Communication, Encoding, and Recall of Words in a Novel Second Language". The Modern Language Journal. 98 (3): 834–853. doi:10.1111/modl.12125. ISSN   0026-7902. JSTOR   43651822.
  32. Marklund, Ulrika; Marklund, Ellen; Gustavsson, Lisa; Samuelsson, Christina (April 2025). "Relationship Between Gestures and Vocabulary in 14-Month-Old Swedish-Learning Children". Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 66 (2): 219–230. doi:10.1111/sjop.13077. ISSN   1467-9450. PMC   11886030 . PMID   39444153.
  33. Rowe, Meredith L.; Özçalışkan, Şeyda; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2008). "Learning words by hand: Gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development". First Language. 28 (2): 182–199. doi:10.1177/0142723707088310. ISSN   0142-7237. PMC   2745165 . PMID   19763249.
  34. Rowe, Meredith L.; Ozçalişkan, Seyda; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2008-01-01). "Learning words by hand: Gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development". First Language. 28 (2): 182–199. doi:10.1177/0142723707088310. ISSN   0142-7237. PMC   2745165 . PMID   19763249.
  35. Rowe, Meredith L.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan (January 2009). "Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning". Developmental Science. 12 (1): 182–187. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.x. ISSN   1467-7687. PMC   2677374 . PMID   19120426.
  36. Özçalışkan, Şeyda; Adamson, Lauren B.; Dimitrova, Nevena (August 2016). "Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism". Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. 20 (6): 754–763. doi:10.1177/1362361315605921. ISSN   1461-7005. PMC   4846586 . PMID   26503989.
  37. Kuhn, Laura J.; Willoughby, Michael T.; Wilbourn, Makeba Parramore; Vernon-Feagans, Lynne; Blair, Clancy B.; Family Life Project Key Investigators (2014). "Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood". Child Development. 85 (5): 1898–1914. doi:10.1111/cdev.12249. ISSN   1467-8624. PMC   4165687 . PMID   24773289.
  38. "Babies' gestures partly explain link between wealth and vocabulary". Science. 2025-07-01. Retrieved 2025-07-01.
  39. Goman, Carol Kinsey (2011). "Great Leaders Talk With Their Hands". Forbes. Retrieved 2019-05-27.