Immediate constituent analysis

Last updated

In linguistics, Immediate Constituent Analysis (ICA) is a syntactic theory which focuses on the hierarchical structure of sentences by isolating and identifying the constituents. While the idea of breaking down sentences into smaller components can be traced back to early psychological and linguistic theories, ICA as a formal method was developed in the early 20th century. It was influenced by Wilhelm Wundt's psychological theories of sentence structure but was later refined and formalized within the framework of structural linguistics by Leonard Bloomfield. The method gained traction in the distributionalist tradition through the work of Zellig Harris and Charles F. Hockett, who expanded and applied it to sentence analysis. Additionally, ICA was further explored within the context of glossematics by Knud Togeby. These contributions helped ICA become a central tool in syntactic analysis, focusing on the hierarchical relationships between sentence constituents.

Contents

In its simplest form, ICA proposes that sentences can be divided into smaller, meaningful units, known as immediate constituents, which are further broken down until the atomic units are uncovered, like individual words. These immediate constituents are typically arranged in a binary branching structure, forming a hierarchical organization of the sentence. The process of ICA can vary based on the underlying syntactic framework being employed. In phrase structure grammars (or constituency grammars), the analysis is based on the idea that the fundamental units of syntax are phrases, and these phrases combine in a hierarchical way to form sentences. In contrast, dependency grammars focus on the relationships between individual words, treating words as nodes that are linked by dependency relations rather than phrasal constituents.

History and Development

Immediate Constituent Analysis (ICA) has played a crucial role in the evolution of syntactic theory, shaping our understanding of sentence structure from its early structuralist roots to contemporary linguistic applications. Emerging in the early 20th century, ICA was developed as a method for breaking down sentences into their smallest meaningful components, influencing key linguistic theories like generative grammar and distributionalism. Although no longer at the forefront of modern syntactic theory, ICA continues to be a valuable tool in both theoretical linguistics and practical applications, such as language teaching and computational syntax.

Origin of Structuralism (late 19th and early 20th century)

The work of early structuralist linguists, particularly in the early 20th century, resulted in the development of structural linguistics and subsequently the differentiation of the smallest units of meaning. Ferdinand de Saussure, a Swiss linguist who contributed groundwork to structural linguistics, which later contributed to the developments in syntactic analysis, even though his work focused more on the structural relationship between elements of a language rather than formal syntactic structures (Jensen 2002, pg. 24).

However, ICA as a formal method began to emerge in the United States in the 1930s, largely as a part of American Structuralism. Linguist Leonard Bloomfield, dubbed the father of distributionalism, introduced the distributional analysis method, which focused on providing a structure for syntax, which later influenced ICA's development. Wilhelm Wundt, a German psychologist, had earlier proposed a similar method of dividing sentences into components for psychological analysis, but it was Leonard Bloomfield, known as the father of distributionalism, who formally introduced distributional analysis as a linguistic methodology. Bloomfield’s work on syntactic structures laid the foundation for the ICA approach by emphasizing the identification and classification of linguistic elements in a sentence, which could then be analyzed for their distributional properties.

Early ICA: from Distributionalism to Generative Grammar (mid 20th century)

The method of Immediate Constituent Analysis is most closely associated with the work of Zellig Harris. Harris expanded on Bloomfield's distributional analysis by providing a more formal approach to syntactic structure, specifically in English sentence analysis. In the 1940s and 1950s, Harris introduced the concept of immediate constituents as the parts of a sentence that can be directly combined to form larger units, such as noun phrases (NPs) and verb phrases (VPs) (Harris 1951, pg. 52).

Harris's ICA method involved continuously dividing a sentence into two immediate constituents, which can then be further subdivided until reaching the smallest meaningful units. Harris's work was foundational in the development of syntactic theory, and his ICA approach set the stage for later advancements in generative grammar (Harris 1955, pg. 363).

Charles F. Hockett, another key figure in structural linguistics, also contributed to the development of ICA. He built upon Harris’s work, incorporating the idea of constituent structure and the distributional analysis of sentence components. Hockett’s contributions focused on the role of syntax in understanding language’s formal structure and its relationship to meaning.

In the 1960s, Noam Chomsky introduced generative grammar, which significantly expanded on the structuralist approaches like ICA. While Chomsky’s theories were more abstract and rule-based, ICA’s emphasis on constituent structure remained influential. Chomsky’s Transformational-Generative Grammar theorized that deep structures (abstract syntactic representations) could be transformed into surface structures (the actual sentences) using specific transformational rules. Though ICA was not directly incorporated into Chomsky's generative grammar, its focus on constituent analysis influenced Chomsky’s syntactic theory (Chomsky 1957, pg. 93).

Chomsky introduced more formalized syntactic structures, including phrase structure rules and X-bar theory, which were designed to explain the hierarchical structure of sentences in a more formal and rule-based manner. Even though ICA itself was not central to Chomsky’s theories, the core idea of breaking down sentences into hierarchical structures remained.

In the mid-20th century, ICA gained additional refinement through the work of Knud Togeby, who integrated structuralist principles into his own approach to sentence analysis. Togeby, working within the framework of glossematics, a European theory of structural linguistics, developed a more formalized version of ICA. His approach emphasized the importance of breaking down sentences into immediate constituents to reveal their hierarchical structure, thus contributing to the ongoing evolution of ICA.

Formalization (late 20th century)

As generative grammar evolved, linguists began to formalize structural analysis further, leading to the development of more sophisticated models like X-bar theory, binding theory, and later minimalist syntax. While ICA was criticized for being too simplistic in these later theoretical frameworks, its basic principles of constituent structure remained an important influence on syntactic theory (Chomsky 1981, pg. 96).

In addition, ICA found new relevance in the field of computational linguistics, particularly in the development of syntactic parsers and language-processing algorithms. ICA’s hierarchical decomposition proved useful for programming computers to analyze and generate syntactic structures automatically (Jurafsky & Martin, 2023).

Contemporary Applications

Today, ICA remains a useful method in both theoretical and applied linguistics. While it is no longer central to the major syntactic theories, ICA continues to be used in more practical, pedagogical contexts, such as teaching syntax and sentence parsing. ICA is still useful in explaining sentence structure in languages that do not rely on word order as heavily, such as languages with free word order or those that rely more on morphology.

Constituents and Application of ICA

A key aim of ICA is that it divides immediate constituents into smaller units. This process of breaking up sentences into smaller units until fully exhausted is seen in the hierarchical structure of syntax trees. When looking at a tree, a phrasal label, or node, lets us know that the set of substrings below that label behave in a distributionally coherent way. For example, within a sentence (S) a node that bears-the-label of a syntactic category, NP, dominates a set of substrings, and every string that precedes under this node NP, will behave similarly to an N.

Therefore, a constituent represents a dominance relationship where it includes a node and the sequence of symbols dominated by that node, forming a distributionally coherent unit. (See below for Constituent Tests). This means the symbols within the constituent function together as a single unit and behave in the same way as other elements of their category within the sentence structure. By following these relationships, it systematically breaks down the tree, starting from the root S and moving downward through its branches until all parts of the sentence are accounted for.

The Smallest Indivisible Units: Terminal Nodes

The units which no longer dominate anything else and cannot be further divided, are called terminal nodes. These are typically words or morphemes (the smallest meaningful unit), representing the final stage in a tree. A sentence, in turn, is formed as a sequence of symbols, beginning with a designated non-terminal symbol and culminating in a terminal string. Every terminal node is part of a constituent, as constituents form the interconnected structure leading from the root to the smallest, indivisible unit of the tree.

Harrisian's Structuralist Approach: Endocentric and Exocentric Constructions

Within Immediate Constituent Analysis, what has been described so far is that each node in a sentence dominates its preceding substrings, which are distributionally related to that node as a constituent. Within this framework, we have assumed that no node excludes another within the sentence; instead, all nodes are part of an interconnected hierarchical structure that extends to the terminal nodes. This configuration is referred to as an endocentric construction, where all sentence components are linked through dominance relations. The analysis concludes only when the tree has exhausted every unit.

However, Zellig Harris in his structuralist approach also allows for exocentric constructions where the category of the whole constituent is not the same as the category of its individual parts. In other words, the overall structure does not inherit the category of its components. To illustrate exocentric constructions, consider the rule S → NP VP. This rule states that when a NP and a VP are combined, the resulting structure is an S. However, S doesn't inherit the category of its components (NP and VP). In other words, the whole sentence is not categorized as a noun phrase or a verb phrase, but as a new unit—a sentence—which is an exocentric construction. The rule S → NP VP demonstrates how the combination of these parts creates a new structure that doesn’t directly reflect the properties of its individual components.

ICA in phrase structure grammars

Given a phrase structure grammar (= constituency grammar), ICA divides up a sentence into major parts or immediate constituents, and these constituents are in turn divided into further immediate constituents. [1] The process continues until irreducible constituents are reached, i.e., until each constituent consists of only a word or a meaningful part of a word. The end result of ICA is often presented in a visual diagrammatic form that reveals the hierarchical immediate constituent structure of the sentence at hand. These diagrams are usually represented as trees. For example:

E-ICA-01.jpg

This tree illustrates the manner in which the entire sentence is divided first into the two immediate constituents this tree and illustrates ICA according to the constituency relation; these two constituents are further divided into the immediate constituents this and tree, and illustrates ICA and according to the constituency relation; and so on.

More recent literature has come forth with the argument that generative grammar applies an "array-based" structure which is derived from, but no longer a form of, ICA. More contemporary phrase structure grammar (a subset of generative grammar) models such as Bare Phrase Structure and X-Bar Theory appear to cause an inconsistent interpretation of a "constituent" as posited by ICA, moreover forsaking the distributional class properties so fundamental to ICA. This stands as the basis for the argument that generative grammar, though built upon the principles of ICA, has now developed in a different direction. It is important to note that contemporary theories and their labels are becoming incompatible with ICA, as opposed to ICA no longer driving their development; that is, while the ICA thought process is foundational to many theories, a hindsight comparison of the development of these theories over time indicates a deviation from original ICA ideas. As such, there may be a need to reconsider new foundational analyses on which to build future grammar models. Krivochen's (2024) [2] array-based analysis is one such suggestion.

A syntax tree example under bare phrase structure The man refused BPS.png
A syntax tree example under bare phrase structure

This tree, represented by the more contemporary model Bare Phrase Structure, illustrates several arguments offered by Krivochen (2024) on the non-correspondence between modern generative grammar and ICA. According to ICA, the distributional properties of a category would apply to all nodes it dominates, creating supposed constituents. However, this would offer ill-formed predictions of constituents (e.g., the highest TP would, under IC-analysis, be able to select for The man has refused the present as a constituent, but this is intuitively an incorrect constituent label).

A syntax tree example under immediate constituent analysis The man refused ICA.png
A syntax tree example under immediate constituent analysis

The third tree in this section illustrates the same sentence, “The man refused the present.”, but with an ICA correspondence. As theories have developed, it is argued that tree structures and their implications on categories and divisions have gradually moved away from models compatible with ICA. Although this tree structure is commonly used in computational linguistics, the model on which this tree is based has been considered outdated in syntax since the development of functional categories, phrasal heads, and X-Bar schema, among others, as fundamental grammar concepts.

However, because phrase structure trees and structurally simpler trees are always able to derive one another from each other and are both still used today, ICA is still relevant in many contemporary theories.

An important aspect of ICA in phrase structure grammars is that each individual word is a constituent by definition. The process of ICA always ends when the smallest constituents are reached, which are often words (although the analysis can also be extended into the words to acknowledge the manner in which words are structured). The process is, however, different in dependency grammars, since many individual words do not end up as constituents in dependency grammars.

ICA in dependency grammars

As a rule, dependency grammars do not employ ICA, as the principle of syntactic ordering is not inclusion but, rather, asymmetrical dominance-dependency between words. When an attempt is made to incorporate ICA into a dependency-type grammar, the results are some kind of a hybrid system. In actuality, ICA is different in dependency grammars. [3] Since dependency grammars view the finite verb as the root of all sentence structure, they cannot and do not acknowledge the initial binary subject-predicate division of the clause associated with phrase structure grammars. What this means for the general understanding of constituent structure is that dependency grammars do not acknowledge a finite verb phrase (VP) constituent and many individual words also do not qualify as constituents, which means in turn that they will not show up as constituents in the ICA. Thus in the example sentence This tree illustrates ICA according to the dependency relation, many of the phrase structure grammar constituents do not qualify as dependency grammar constituents:

E-ICA-02.jpg

This ICA does not view the finite verb phrase illustrates ICA according to the dependency relation nor the individual words tree, illustrates, according, to, and relation as constituents.

While the structures that ICA identifies for dependency and constituency grammars differ in significant ways, as the two trees just produced illustrate, both views of sentence structure acknowledge constituents. The constituent is defined in a theory-neutral manner:

Constituent
A given word/node plus all the words/nodes that that word/node dominates

This definition is neutral with respect to the dependency vs. constituency distinction. It allows one to compare the ICA across the two types of structure. A constituent is always a complete tree or a complete subtree of a tree, regardless of whether the tree at hand is a constituency or a dependency tree.

Constituency tests

The ICA for a given sentence is arrived at usually by way of constituency tests. Constituency tests (e.g. topicalization, clefting, pseudoclefting, pro-form substitution, answer ellipsis, passivization, omission, coordination, etc.) identify the constituents, large and small, of English sentences. Two illustrations of the manner in which constituency tests deliver clues about constituent structure and thus about the correct ICA of a given sentence are now given. Consider the phrase The girl in the following trees:

Thegirlishappy.png

The acronym BPS stands for "bare phrase structure", which is an indication that the words are used as the node labels in the tree. Again, focusing on the phrase The girl, the tests unanimously confirm that it is a constituent as both trees show:

...the girl is happy - Topicalization (invalid test because test constituent is already at front of sentence)
It is the girl who is happy. - Clefting
(The one)Who is happy is the girl. - Pseudoclefting
She is happy. - Pro-form substitution
Who is happy? -The girl. - Answer ellipsis

Based on these results, one can safely assume that the noun phrase The girl in the example sentence is a constituent and should therefore be shown as one in the corresponding IC-representation, which it is in both trees. Consider next what these tests tell us about the verb string is happy:

*...is happy, the girl. - Topicalization
*It is is happy that the girl. - Clefting
*What the girl is is happy. - Pseudoclefting
*The girl so/that/did that. - Pro-form substitution
What is the girl? -*Is happy. - Answer ellipsis

The star * indicates that the sentence is not acceptable English. Based on data like these, one might conclude that the finite verb string is happy in the example sentence is not a constituent and should therefore not be shown as a constituent in the corresponding IC-representation. Hence this result supports the ICA in the dependency tree over the one in the constituency tree, since the dependency tree does not view is happy as a constituent.

Notes

  1. The basic concept of immediate constituents is widely employed in phrase structure grammars. See for instance Akmajian and Heny (1980:64), Chisholm (1981:59), Culicover (1982:21), Huddleston (1988:7), Haegeman and Guéron (1999:51).
  2. Krivochen, D. (2024). Constituents, arrays, and trees: two (more) models of grammatical description. Folia Linguistica, 58(3), 699-727. https://doi.org/10.1515/flin-2024-2025
  3. Concerning dependency grammars, see Ágel et al. (2003/6).

Related Research Articles

In linguistics, syntax is the study of how words and morphemes combine to form larger units such as phrases and sentences. Central concerns of syntax include word order, grammatical relations, hierarchical sentence structure (constituency), agreement, the nature of crosslinguistic variation, and the relationship between form and meaning (semantics). Diverse approaches, such as generative grammar and functional grammar, offer unique perspectives on syntax, reflecting its complexity and centrality to understanding human language.

A syntactic category is a syntactic unit that theories of syntax assume. Word classes, largely corresponding to traditional parts of speech, are syntactic categories. In phrase structure grammars, the phrasal categories are also syntactic categories. Dependency grammars, however, do not acknowledge phrasal categories.

In grammar, a phrase—called expression in some contexts—is a group of words or singular word acting as a grammatical unit. For instance, the English expression "the very happy squirrel" is a noun phrase which contains the adjective phrase "very happy". Phrases can consist of a single word or a complete sentence. In theoretical linguistics, phrases are often analyzed as units of syntactic structure such as a constituent. There is a difference between the common use of the term phrase and its technical use in linguistics. In common usage, a phrase is usually a group of words with some special idiomatic meaning or other significance, such as "all rights reserved", "economical with the truth", "kick the bucket", and the like. It may be a euphemism, a saying or proverb, a fixed expression, a figure of speech, etc.. In linguistics, these are known as phrasemes.

Phrase structure rules are a type of rewrite rule used to describe a given language's syntax and are closely associated with the early stages of transformational grammar, proposed by Noam Chomsky in 1957. They are used to break down a natural language sentence into its constituent parts, also known as syntactic categories, including both lexical categories and phrasal categories. A grammar that uses phrase structure rules is a type of phrase structure grammar. Phrase structure rules as they are commonly employed operate according to the constituency relation, and a grammar that employs phrase structure rules is therefore a constituency grammar; as such, it stands in contrast to dependency grammars, which are based on the dependency relation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parse tree</span> Tree in formal language theory

A parse tree or parsing tree is an ordered, rooted tree that represents the syntactic structure of a string according to some context-free grammar. The term parse tree itself is used primarily in computational linguistics; in theoretical syntax, the term syntax tree is more common.

Lexical semantics, as a subfield of linguistic semantics, is the study of word meanings. It includes the study of how words structure their meaning, how they act in grammar and compositionality, and the relationships between the distinct senses and uses of a word.

In linguistics, X-bar theory is a model of phrase-structure grammar and a theory of syntactic category formation that was first proposed by Noam Chomsky in 1970 reformulating the ideas of Zellig Harris (1951), and further developed by Ray Jackendoff, along the lines of the theory of generative grammar put forth in the 1950s by Chomsky. It attempts to capture the structure of phrasal categories with a single uniform structure called the X-bar schema, basing itself on the assumption that any phrase in natural language is an XP that is headed by a given syntactic category X. It played a significant role in resolving issues that phrase structure rules had, representative of which is the proliferation of grammatical rules, which is against the thesis of generative grammar.

In linguistics, a verb phrase (VP) is a syntactic unit composed of a verb and its arguments except the subject of an independent clause or coordinate clause. Thus, in the sentence A fat man quickly put the money into the box, the words quickly put the money into the box constitute a verb phrase; it consists of the verb put and its arguments, but not the subject a fat man. A verb phrase is similar to what is considered a predicate in traditional grammars.

In linguistics, a determiner phrase (DP) is a type of phrase headed by a determiner such as many. Controversially, many approaches take a phrase like not very many apples to be a DP, headed, in this case, by the determiner many. This is called the DP analysis or the DP hypothesis. Others reject this analysis in favor of the more traditional NP analysis where apples would be the head of the phrase in which the DP not very many is merely a dependent. Thus, there are competing analyses concerning heads and dependents in nominal groups. The DP analysis developed in the late 1970s and early 1980s, and it is the majority view in generative grammar today.

In linguistics, the head or nucleus of a phrase is the word that determines the syntactic category of that phrase. For example, the head of the noun phrase boiling hot water is the noun water. Analogously, the head of a compound is the stem that determines the semantic category of that compound. For example, the head of the compound noun handbag is bag, since a handbag is a bag, not a hand. The other elements of the phrase or compound modify the head, and are therefore the head's dependents. Headed phrases and compounds are called endocentric, whereas exocentric ("headless") phrases and compounds lack a clear head. Heads are crucial to establishing the direction of branching. Head-initial phrases are right-branching, head-final phrases are left-branching, and head-medial phrases combine left- and right-branching.

In linguistics, branching refers to the shape of the parse trees that represent the structure of sentences. Assuming that the language is being written or transcribed from left to right, parse trees that grow down and to the right are right-branching, and parse trees that grow down and to the left are left-branching. The direction of branching reflects the position of heads in phrases, and in this regard, right-branching structures are head-initial, whereas left-branching structures are head-final. English has both right-branching (head-initial) and left-branching (head-final) structures, although it is more right-branching than left-branching. Some languages such as Japanese and Turkish are almost fully left-branching (head-final). Some languages are mostly right-branching (head-initial).

Dependency grammar (DG) is a class of modern grammatical theories that are all based on the dependency relation and that can be traced back primarily to the work of Lucien Tesnière. Dependency is the notion that linguistic units, e.g. words, are connected to each other by directed links. The (finite) verb is taken to be the structural center of clause structure. All other syntactic units (words) are either directly or indirectly connected to the verb in terms of the directed links, which are called dependencies. Dependency grammar differs from phrase structure grammar in that while it can identify phrases it tends to overlook phrasal nodes. A dependency structure is determined by the relation between a word and its dependents. Dependency structures are flatter than phrase structures in part because they lack a finite verb phrase constituent, and they are thus well suited for the analysis of languages with free word order, such as Czech or Warlpiri.

The term phrase structure grammar was originally introduced by Noam Chomsky as the term for grammar studied previously by Emil Post and Axel Thue. Some authors, however, reserve the term for more restricted grammars in the Chomsky hierarchy: context-sensitive grammars or context-free grammars. In a broader sense, phrase structure grammars are also known as constituency grammars. The defining character of phrase structure grammars is thus their adherence to the constituency relation, as opposed to the dependency relation of dependency grammars.

In generative grammar, non-configurational languages are languages characterized by a flat phrase structure, which allows syntactically discontinuous expressions, and a relatively free word order.

<i>Syntactic Structures</i> Book by Noam Chomsky

Syntactic Structures is an important work in linguistics by American linguist Noam Chomsky, originally published in 1957. A short monograph of about a hundred pages, it is recognized as one of the most significant and influential linguistic studies of the 20th century. It contains the now-famous sentence "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously", which Chomsky offered as an example of a grammatically correct sentence that has no discernible meaning, thus arguing for the independence of syntax from semantics.

In theoretical linguistics, a distinction is made between endocentric and exocentric constructions. A grammatical construction is said to be endocentric if it fulfils the same linguistic function as one of its parts, and exocentric if it does not. The distinction reaches back at least to Bloomfield's work of the 1930s, who based it on terms by Pāṇini and Patañjali in Sanskrit grammar. Such a distinction is possible only in phrase structure grammars, since in dependency grammars all constructions are necessarily endocentric.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lucien Tesnière</span> French linguist (1893–1954)

Lucien Tesnière was a prominent and influential French linguist. He was born in Mont-Saint-Aignan on May 13, 1893. As a senior lecturer at the University of Strasbourg (1924) and later professor at the University of Montpellier (1937), he published many papers and books on Slavic languages. However, his importance in the history of linguistics is based mainly on his development of an approach to the syntax of natural languages that would become known as dependency grammar. He presented his theory in his book Éléments de syntaxe structurale, published posthumously in 1959. In the book he proposes a sophisticated formalization of syntactic structures, supported by many examples from a diversity of languages. Tesnière died in Montpellier on December 6, 1954.

The linguistics wars were extended disputes among American theoretical linguists that occurred mostly during the 1960s and 1970s, stemming from a disagreement between Noam Chomsky and several of his associates and students. The debates started in 1967 when linguists Paul Postal, John R. Ross, George Lakoff, and James D. McCawley —self-dubbed the "Four Horsemen of the Apocalypse"—proposed an alternative approach in which the relation between semantics and syntax is viewed differently, which treated deep structures as meaning rather than syntactic objects. While Chomsky and other generative grammarians argued that meaning is driven by an underlying syntax, generative semanticists posited that syntax is shaped by an underlying meaning. This intellectual divergence led to two competing frameworks in generative semantics and interpretive semantics.

Merge is one of the basic operations in the Minimalist Program, a leading approach to generative syntax, when two syntactic objects are combined to form a new syntactic unit. Merge also has the property of recursion in that it may be applied to its own output: the objects combined by Merge are either lexical items or sets that were themselves formed by Merge. This recursive property of Merge has been claimed to be a fundamental characteristic that distinguishes language from other cognitive faculties. As Noam Chomsky (1999) puts it, Merge is "an indispensable operation of a recursive system ... which takes two syntactic objects A and B and forms the new object G={A,B}" (p. 2).

Syntactic movement is the means by which some theories of syntax address discontinuities. Movement was first postulated by structuralist linguists who expressed it in terms of discontinuous constituents or displacement. Some constituents appear to have been displaced from the position in which they receive important features of interpretation. The concept of movement is controversial and is associated with so-called transformational or derivational theories of syntax. Representational theories, in contrast, reject the notion of movement and often instead address discontinuities with other mechanisms including graph reentrancies, feature passing, and type shifters.

References