Karenleigh A. Overmann

Last updated

Karenleigh A. Overmann is a cognitive archaeologist known for her work on how ancient societies became numerate and literate. [1] She currently directs the Center for Cognitive Archaeology at the University of Colorado, Colorado Springs. Before becoming an academic researcher, Overmann served 25 years of active duty in the U.S. Navy. [2]

Contents

Education

Overmann completed her master's in psychology in 2013 at the University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, under the supervision of Frederick L. Coolidge and Thomas G. Wynn and her doctorate in archaeology in 2016 at the University of Oxford as a Clarendon Scholar [2] under the supervision of Lambros Malafouris and Chris Gosden. [3] From 2018 to 2020, Overmann was a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Actions (MSCA) postdoctoral research fellow at the University of Bergen (project 785793). [4]

Work on numeracy

Overmann has published several works showing how numbers are realized and elaborated through the use of material forms; these make the innate sense of number tangible and tractable to manipulation. [3] [5] [6] She describes the role of “the material devices used to represent and manipulate numbers—fingers, tallies, tokens, and written notations—[as] the mechanism for their elaboration and the source of their various properties. With this model, cross-cultural variability becomes a simple matter of whether material devices are used, which devices are used, and how those devices are used.” [7] :5–6 The “technological layering of material forms [is] systematized by device affordances and limitations that predictably and reliably emerge from the interaction of numerosity, material form, behaviors, and social needs.” [8] :461 This research has been highlighted as "a naturalistically plausible account of the emergence of the modern natural number concept" [9] :530 and "a 'Copernican Revolution' in the way we understand the relationship between numbers and the material devices we use to record and manipulate them." [10] :1

She has analyzed counting artifacts from the Ancient Near East, including fingers (as indicated by Sumerian vocabulary), tallies (as archaeologically attested), tokens (archaeologically and textually attested), and notations (textually attested). She later expanded the catalogue of Near Eastern tokens published by Denise Schmandt-Besserat in 1992 [11] with over 2,300 new entries. [3] [12] [13] The results of analyzing the updated token catalogue were published in 2019 as a component of Overmann's book, The Material Origin of Numbers.

Overmann has also investigated the traditional counting methods used in Oceania, particularly Polynesia; this research solved two mysteries of several centuries' standing: what was meant by the claim that Māori counted by "elevens" and why the Hawaiian word for twenty, iwakalua, meant "nine and two"; both are related to the method of counting by sorting used in Polynesia. [4] [14] She coined the term "ephemeral abacus " to refer to temporary material forms with inherent place value (exponential structure), including collaborative finger-counting and counting by sorting. [4] In 2021, she published a detailed comparison of Polynesian and Mesopotamian numbers, both of which use object-specified counting sequences; the more recent counting practices of Polynesia provide a new way to understand how such counting would have worked in ancient times. [15]

Work on early writing systems and literacy

Overmann has analyzed early writing in Mesopotamia, showing how script and literacy emerged from the practice of handwriting small pictures over the course of about 15 centuries of time, [16] [17] [18] [19] and extended the analysis to examine writing as an example of extended cognition. [20] In 2024, she described this as the development of complementarity: “The material component of writing complements the psychological component of literacy, and vice versa. Following Menary, I use the term complementarity to mean that ‘bodily internal and external processes coordinate with one another in the completion of cognitive tasks’. [21] :20 However, I want to go beyond this sense to emphasize that the literacy we enjoy today is the result of the material form that is writing changing in its form and functions and the brain reorganizing neurologically to interact with it. That is, the material and psychological components have both changed in ways that maximize the efficiency of their interaction with each other.” [22] :15 Often working in conjunction with archaeologist Thomas G. Wynn, she has applied insights gained from her analyses of the way writing changes over time to stone tools in the Lower Paleolithic. [23] [24] [25] [26]

Other work

Beyond her work in numeracy and literacy, Overmann has analyzed the material component of timekeeping for its effects on the conceptualization of time. [27] With her colleagues Thomas Wynn and Frederick L. Coolidge, she has written about the cognitive differences between Neandertals and contemporary Homo sapiens and the implications for Neandertal extinction. [28] [29] She has also analyzed Jane Austen's novel Emma as a gender-reversed version of Pride and Prejudice [30] and written about conceptions of the mind and madness in the Regency era. [31]

Selected works

Authored books

Edited volumes

Special journal issues

Articles on numbers

Articles on early writing systems

Book chapters

See also

References

  1. "UCCS Center for Cognitive Archaeology Faculty". University of Colorado, Colorado Springs. 2021. Retrieved June 3, 2021.
  2. 1 2 "From the US Navy to an Archaeology DPhil at Oxford…". University of Oxford. November 15, 2013. Retrieved June 3, 2021.
  3. 1 2 3 Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2019). The Material Origin of Numbers: Insights from the Archaeology of the Ancient Near East. Piscataway, NJ: Gorgias Press. ISBN   9781463207434.
  4. 1 2 3 Overmann, Karenleigh A (2020). "The Curious Idea that Māori Once Counted by Elevens, and the Insights It Still Holds for Cross-Cultural Numerical Research". Journal of the Polynesian Society. 129 (1): 59–84. doi: 10.15286/jps.129.1.59-84 . Retrieved 24 July 2020.
  5. Barras, Colin (2021). "How Did Neanderthals and Other Ancient Humans Learn to Count?". Nature. 594 (7861): 22–25. Bibcode:2021Natur.594...22B. doi: 10.1038/d41586-021-01429-6 . ISSN   0028-0836. PMID   34079134. S2CID   235322556 . Retrieved June 6, 2021.
  6. "On the Origin of Numbers". 2021. Retrieved June 6, 2021.
  7. Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2025). Cultural Number Systems: A Sourcebook. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. ISBN   978-3-031-83382-3.
  8. Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2021). "Numerical origins: The critical questions". Journal of Cognition and Culture. 21 (5): 449–468. doi:10.1163/15685373-12340121.
  9. Zahidi, Karim (2021). "Radicalizing Numerical Cognition" . Synthese. 198 (1): 529–545. doi:10.1007/s11229-020-02956-x. S2CID   229433621.
  10. Dos Santos, César (2023). "Review of Karenleigh A. Overmann's The Materiality of Numbers". Qeios: 1–10. doi: 10.32388/MBBSHS . Retrieved 31 August 2023.
  11. Schmandt-Besserat, Denise (1992). Before Writing: From Counting to Cuneiform, Vol. II. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. ISBN   9780292707832.
  12. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2016), Catalogue of Ancient Near Eastern Tokens, Unpublished, retrieved November 2, 2021
  13. Wilding, Denise; Rowan, Clare; Maurer, Bill; Schmandt-Besserat, Denise (2017). "Tokens, writing and (ac)counting: A conversation with Denise Schmandt-Besserat and Bill Maurer". Exchanges: The Interdisciplinary Research Journal. 5 (1): 1–14. doi: 10.31273/eirj.v5i1.196 . Retrieved January 6, 2022.
  14. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2020). "Counting by "Elevens" and Why Nine and Two make Twenty: The Material Roots of Polynesian Numbers". Journal of Mathematics and Culture. 15 (3): 1–32. Retrieved June 3, 2021.
  15. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2021). "A New Look at Old Numbers, and What It Reveals about Numeration". Journal of Near Eastern Studies. 80 (2): 291–321. doi:10.1086/715767. S2CID   239028709 . Retrieved October 23, 2021.
  16. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2016). "Beyond Writing: The Development of Literacy in the Ancient Near East". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 26 (2): 285–303. doi:10.1017/S0959774316000019. S2CID   163840618.
  17. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2021). "A Cognitive Archaeology of Writing: Concepts, Models, Goals". In Boyes, Philip; Steele, Philippa; Astoreca, Natalia Elvira (eds.). The Social and Cultural Contexts of Historic Writing Practices. Oxford: Oxbow. pp. 55–72. ISBN   9781789254785.
  18. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2021). "Writing System Transmission and Change: A Neurofunctional Perspective". In Gabriel, Gösta; Overmann, Karenleigh A; Payne, Annick (eds.). Signs – Sounds – Semantics. Nature and Transformation of Writing Systems in the Ancient Near East. Wiener Offene Orientalistik 13. Wein: Ugarit-Verlag. pp. 93–116.
  19. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2022). "Early Writing: A Cognitive Archaeological Perspective on Literacy and Numeracy". Visible Language. 56 (1): 8–44. doi:10.34314/vl.v56i1.4934.
  20. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2024). "Writing as an extended cognitive system". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences: 1–21. doi:10.1007/s11097-023-09955-6.
  21. Menary, Richard (2010). "Introduction: The extended mind in focus". In Menary, Richard (ed.). The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 1–25. ISBN   978-3-031-83382-3.
  22. Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2024). "Writing as an extended cognitive system". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences: 1–21. doi:10.1007/s11097-023-09955-6.
  23. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2021). "The Material Difference in Human Cognition". Adaptive Behavior. 29 (2): 123–136. doi: 10.1177/1059712320930738 . hdl: 11250/2766177 . S2CID   225657095.
  24. Overmann, Karenleigh A; Wynn, Thomas (2019). "Materiality and human cognition". Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory. 26 (2): 457–478. doi:10.1007/s10816-018-9378-y. S2CID   149605602.
  25. Overmann, Karenleigh A; Wynn, Thomas (2019). "On Tools Making Minds: An Archaeological Perspective on Human Cognitive Evolution". Journal of Cognition and Culture. 19 (1–2): 39–58. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12340047 . hdl: 1956/23288 . S2CID   181503453.
  26. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2025). "On a creativity that is mundane, cooperative, and material". Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 37 (4): 383–396. doi:10.1080/20445911.2025.2471446.
  27. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2024). "The beginning of time". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 34 (4): 693–709.
  28. Wynn, Thomas; Overmann, Karenleigh A; Coolidge, Frederick L (2016). "The false dichotomy: A refutation of the Neandertal indistinguishability claim" (PDF). Journal of Anthropological Sciences. 94 (94): 201–221. PMID   26708102.
  29. Coolidge, Frederick L.; Wynn, Thomas; Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2024). "The Expert Neandertal Mind and Brain, Revisited". In Wynn, Thomas; Overmann, Karenleigh A.; Coolidge, Frederick L. (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Archaeology. Oxford University Press. ISBN   9780192895950.
  30. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2009). "Darcy and Emma: Jane Austen's ironic meditation on gender". Persuasions: The Jane Austen Journal. 31: 222–235. Retrieved June 5, 2020.
  31. Overmann, Karenleigh A (2009). "Cartesian Dualism, Real and Literary Madness in the Regency, and the Mind and Madness in Austen's Novels". Persuasions: The Jane Austen Journal. 35: 109–128. Retrieved June 5, 2020.