Karenleigh A. Overmann is a cognitive archaeologist known for her work on how ancient societies became numerate and literate. [1] She currently directs the Center for Cognitive Archaeology at the University of Colorado, Colorado Springs. Before becoming an academic researcher, Overmann served 25 years of active duty in the U.S. Navy. [2]
Overmann completed her master's in psychology in 2013 at the University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, under the supervision of Frederick L. Coolidge and Thomas G. Wynn and her doctorate in archaeology in 2016 at the University of Oxford as a Clarendon Scholar [2] under the supervision of Lambros Malafouris and Chris Gosden. [3] From 2018 to 2020, Overmann was a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Actions (MSCA) postdoctoral research fellow at the University of Bergen (project 785793). [4]
Overmann has published several works showing how numbers are realized and elaborated through the use of material forms; these make the innate sense of number tangible and tractable to manipulation. [3] [5] [6] She describes the role of “the material devices used to represent and manipulate numbers—fingers, tallies, tokens, and written notations—[as] the mechanism for their elaboration and the source of their various properties. With this model, cross-cultural variability becomes a simple matter of whether material devices are used, which devices are used, and how those devices are used.” [7] : 5–6 The “technological layering of material forms [is] systematized by device affordances and limitations that predictably and reliably emerge from the interaction of numerosity, material form, behaviors, and social needs.” [8] : 461 This research has been highlighted as "a naturalistically plausible account of the emergence of the modern natural number concept" [9] : 530 and "a 'Copernican Revolution' in the way we understand the relationship between numbers and the material devices we use to record and manipulate them." [10] : 1
She has analyzed counting artifacts from the Ancient Near East, including fingers (as indicated by Sumerian vocabulary), tallies (as archaeologically attested), tokens (archaeologically and textually attested), and notations (textually attested). She later expanded the catalogue of Near Eastern tokens published by Denise Schmandt-Besserat in 1992 [11] with over 2,300 new entries. [3] [12] [13] The results of analyzing the updated token catalogue were published in 2019 as a component of Overmann's book, The Material Origin of Numbers.
Overmann has also investigated the traditional counting methods used in Oceania, particularly Polynesia; this research solved two mysteries of several centuries' standing: what was meant by the claim that Māori counted by "elevens" and why the Hawaiian word for twenty, iwakalua, meant "nine and two"; both are related to the method of counting by sorting used in Polynesia. [4] [14] She coined the term "ephemeral abacus " to refer to temporary material forms with inherent place value (exponential structure), including collaborative finger-counting and counting by sorting. [4] In 2021, she published a detailed comparison of Polynesian and Mesopotamian numbers, both of which use object-specified counting sequences; the more recent counting practices of Polynesia provide a new way to understand how such counting would have worked in ancient times. [15]
Overmann has analyzed early writing in Mesopotamia, showing how script and literacy emerged from the practice of handwriting small pictures over the course of about 15 centuries of time, [16] [17] [18] [19] and extended the analysis to examine writing as an example of extended cognition. [20] In 2024, she described this as the development of complementarity: “The material component of writing complements the psychological component of literacy, and vice versa. Following Menary, I use the term complementarity to mean that ‘bodily internal and external processes coordinate with one another in the completion of cognitive tasks’. [21] : 20 However, I want to go beyond this sense to emphasize that the literacy we enjoy today is the result of the material form that is writing changing in its form and functions and the brain reorganizing neurologically to interact with it. That is, the material and psychological components have both changed in ways that maximize the efficiency of their interaction with each other.” [22] : 15 Often working in conjunction with archaeologist Thomas G. Wynn, she has applied insights gained from her analyses of the way writing changes over time to stone tools in the Lower Paleolithic. [23] [24] [25] [26]
Beyond her work in numeracy and literacy, Overmann has analyzed the material component of timekeeping for its effects on the conceptualization of time. [27] With her colleagues Thomas Wynn and Frederick L. Coolidge, she has written about the cognitive differences between Neandertals and contemporary Homo sapiens and the implications for Neandertal extinction. [28] [29] She has also analyzed Jane Austen's novel Emma as a gender-reversed version of Pride and Prejudice [30] and written about conceptions of the mind and madness in the Regency era. [31]