Sensory trap hypothesis

Last updated

The sensory trap hypothesis describes an evolutionary idea that revolves around mating behavior and female mate choice. It is a model of female preference and male sexual trait evolution through what is known as sensory exploitation. [1] [2] Sensory exploitation, or a sensory trap is an event that occurs in nature where male members of a species perform behaviors or display visual traits that resemble a non-sexual stimulus which females are responsive to. This tricks females into engaging with the males, thus creating more mating opportunities for males. [2] [3] What makes it a sensory trap is that these female responses evolved in a non-sexual context, and the male produced stimulus exploits the female response which would not otherwise occur without the mimicked stimulus. [2] [4]

Contents

Limitations

The term "trap" indicates that these sensory trap events may be detrimental to female mating success, but they may not always be costly. In fact, there are circumstances where not responding to the stimulus itself can be costly, as females may ignore the actual stimulus in the correct context, and lose the fitness benefits that come with it. [2] There are also circumstances where these traps can actually be beneficial in the context of mate choice, where the females who are responding to the trap end up gaining high-quality males to mate with. [2] [3]

While these sensory traps can be quite successful when they appropriately mimic the non-sexual stimulus, they often become exaggerated as a result of excessive selection to the point where they are no longer useful. [2] This is due to the trait or behavior becoming imperceptible or no longer resembling the original stimulus. [2]

Sensory traps in nature

References

  1. Arnqvist, Göran (2006-02-28). "Sensory exploitation and sexual conflict". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 361 (1466): 375–386. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1790. ISSN   0962-8436. PMC   1569614 . PMID   16612895.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Christy, John H. (1995). "Mimicry, Mate Choice, and the Sensory Trap Hypothesis" . The American Naturalist. 146 (2): 171–181. Bibcode:1995ANat..146..171C. doi:10.1086/285793. ISSN   0003-0147. JSTOR   2463056. S2CID   84199588.
  3. 1 2 3 Buchinger, Tyler J.; Scott, Anne M.; Fissette, Skye D.; Brant, Cory O.; Huertas, Mar; Li, Ke; Johnson, Nicholas S.; Li, Weiming (2020-03-31). "A pheromone antagonist liberates female sea lamprey from a sensory trap to enable reliable communication". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (13): 7284–7289. Bibcode:2020PNAS..117.7284B. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1921394117 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   7132252 . PMID   32184327.
  4. 1 2 Proctor, Heather C. (1992-10-01). "Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour: a cladistic test using water mites (Acari: Parasitengona)" . Animal Behaviour. 44 (4): 745–752. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   54426553.
  5. Herzner, Gudrun; Schmitt, Thomas; Linsenmair, K. Eduard; Strohm, Erhard (2005-12-01). "Prey recognition by females of the European beewolf and its potential for a sensory trap" . Animal Behaviour. 70 (6): 1411–1418. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.032. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53269076.
  6. Stålhandske, S. (2002-05-07). "Nuptial gifts of male spiders function as sensory traps". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 269 (1494): 905–908. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1917. PMC   1690977 . PMID   12028772.
  7. Bilde, Trine; Tuni, Cristina; Elsayed, Rehab; Pekar, Stano; Toft, Søren (2007). "Nuptial gifts of male spiders: sensory exploitation of the female's maternal care instinct or foraging motivation?" . Animal Behaviour. 2 (73): 267–273. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.05.014. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53193415.
  8. Córdoba-Aguilar, A. (2002-11-01). "Sensory Trap as the Mechanism of Sexual Selection in a Damselfly Genitalic Trait (Insecta: Calopterygidae)" . The American Naturalist. 160 (5): 594–601. Bibcode:2002ANat..160..594C. doi:10.1086/342819. ISSN   0003-0147. PMID   18707510. S2CID   28598530.
  9. Hasegawa, Masaru; Arai, Emi; Watanabe, Mamoru; Nakamura, Masahiko (2013-11-01). "Male nestling-like courtship calls attract female barn swallows, Hirundo rustica gutturalis" . Animal Behaviour. 86 (5): 949–953. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.08.012. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53154712.
  10. Christy, John H; Baum, Julia K; Backwell, Patricia R. Y (2003-07-01). "Attractiveness of sand hoods built by courting male fiddler crabs, Uca musica: test of a sensory trap hypothesis" . Animal Behaviour. 66 (1): 89–94. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2196. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53150753.
  11. Christy, John H.; Backwell, Patricia R.; Schober, Ursula (2003-01-01). "Interspecific attractiveness of structures built by courting male fiddler crabs: experimental evidence of a sensory trap" . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 53 (2): 84–91. Bibcode:2003BEcoS..53...84C. doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0553-4. ISSN   1432-0762. S2CID   34001395.
  12. Backwell, Patricia R. Y.; Jennions, Michael D.; Christy, John H.; Schober, Ursula (1995). "Pillar Building in the Fiddler Crab Uca beebei: Evidence for a Condition-Dependent Ornament" . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 36 (3): 185–192. Bibcode:1995BEcoS..36..185B. doi:10.1007/BF00177795. ISSN   0340-5443. JSTOR   4601062. S2CID   7795347.
  13. Garcia, Constantino Macías; Ramirez, Elvia (March 2005). "Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals" . Nature. 434 (7032): 501–505. Bibcode:2005Natur.434..501G. doi:10.1038/nature03363. ISSN   1476-4687. PMID   15791255. S2CID   4401680.
  14. Martín, José; López, Pilar (2008-10-01). "Female sensory bias may allow honest chemical signaling by male Iberian rock lizards" . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (12): 1927–1934. Bibcode:2008BEcoS..62.1927M. doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0624-2. ISSN   1432-0762. S2CID   23521697.
  15. Edwards, David P.; Yu, Douglas W. (2007-03-02). "The roles of sensory traps in the origin, maintenance, and breakdown of mutualism" . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 61 (9): 1321–1327. Bibcode:2007BEcoS..61.1321E. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0369-3. ISSN   0340-5443. S2CID   43863247.