Text world theory

Last updated

Text world theory is a cognitive model of language processing which aims to explain how people construct meaning from language. [1] Text world theory and schema theory seek to help people understand how we process language and create mental representations when we read or listen to something. [1] This theory figuratively describes a piece of language (such as a text, a speech or conversation) as a "world" that the reader, hearer or interlocutor must "build" in their mind. [2] Text world theory was first developed by Paul Werth in the 1980s, [1] and has subsequently been used as in education as a method for pupils to engage with literature. [3]

Contents

Origin and development

Text world theory is a branch of cognitive linguistics that was first developed by Professor Paul Werth in the 1980s and 1990s. Werth was a text linguist working within generative linguistics who developed his text world theory as a professor of linguistics at the University of Amsterdam. [4] [5] Werth claimed to have created an approach that accounted for all aspects of human communication, but his monograph Text Worlds: Representing Conceptual Space in Discourse was not completed by the time of his death in 1995. Mick Short completed and edited the text between 1995 and 1998, and it was published in 1999.

These ideas have been expanded upon by contemporary researchers like Joanna Gavins. [1] Since Werth's conception of text world theory, the theory has been incorporated into the study of linguistics, stylistics, cognitive poetics, narratology and literary theory. While Werth's study was largely limited to short extracts, text world theory has been expanded and tested against many different discourse types, including personal ads, poetry, recipes and radio programmes. [1] [6] [7]

Types

Text world theory is structured into three conceptual levels: the discourse-world, the text-worlds and world-switches.

Discourse worlds

The discourse world [2] [1] [6] is the immediate environment a person is in when they are communicating. Discourse worlds can either be shared or split. A shared discourse world would include a face-to-face conversation, whereas a split discourse world would include an author and reader, where the two participants do not share a common environment. An individual's discourse world influences how they interpret language due to their background knowledge, thoughts and feelings. [2] [8] [1] Rather than being an "objective set of physical surroundings", [8] the context of a discourse world is a mental process carried out as part of a negotiation between discourse-world participants. [9]

Text worlds

Within these discourse worlds, humans create "mental representations" [1] which allow the language they are encountering to be understood. In Text worlds a person uses the linguistic stimuli of the text/speech and expands upon it using their background knowledge, perspectives and emotions. [2] [1] Language acts as a means of refining and determining the schema the reader draws upon. [1] For instance "the house" draws up no schema other than houses the individual reader has experienced in their lifetime, whereas "the abandoned house surrounded by dead trees" likely calls up the linguistic schema of Gothic fiction and its associated features.

World-switches

World-switches are departures from the initial text world through a shift in space or time. [2] These involve the reader constructing a mental model of a new text world through which the new time or place can be conceptualised. For instance, "Gavin stared at the crowd of revellers at the staff party..." plants the reader in one text world. However, if the sentence continued "...and he recalled the school discos of his lonely childhood", this would constitute a world shift that requires readers to call upon new schema quite differentiated from the original text world.

Participant-accessible and enactor-accessible worlds

Worlds that are accessible to every enactor within that world are known as participant accessible. This means that multiple characters in the text are having the same experience and express their own judgment of it. [1] Enactor accessible means that only one enactor can access that world. One example of this might be an individual's imagination or inner feelings. [1]

Modal worlds are distinct from world-switches. Changes in time and space are world-switches, while shifts in perception are described as modal worlds. [1] They express modes of approaching the world and are only accessible to the enactors of the text experiencing these shifts.

  • In the perception modality, the enactor's sub-world is believed to be factual and true: "surely she was wrong about the price of the book."
  • In the deontic modality, the character expresses a form of duty or a requirement they must fulfill: "it was required that he feed the animals."
  • The boulomaic modality refers to a particular desire the character has, such as a fantasy or ideal and is often demonstrated through verbs like 'wish', 'hope' and 'desire'. [1]

Example

The first clause of Italo Calvino's short story 'The Man Who Shouted Teresa' is "I stepped off the pavement". [3] When this is read, a split discourse world occurs due to the separation between Calvino and the reader. The reader will locate their text world in the past due to the past tense verb "stepped", and will identify a character (presumably a protagonist) with whom the reader is expected to identify through the first person pronoun, "I." The word "pavement" makes the reader use their background knowledge in order to build a text world in which this action takes place, and this will differ between readers. While many readers might construct an urban environment based on this word, each reader's text world would be informed by how their background knowledge and lived experience has influenced their understanding of the word "pavement". [3]

Approach

Text world scholars often use diagrams as initial points of entry for analysing different texts. [1] [8] This is to provide a visual way of understanding the different text worlds at play, as well as highlight the key elements of the schema the reader is being asked to call upon. They are used to demonstrate complex structures in a relatively simple way. A basic diagram of a single text-world from the first paragraphs of The Old Man and the Sea is shown below. The material actions or verbs are denoted with the arrow symbol →, while descriptions are marked with the = symbol.

TEXT-WORLD 1
Time:

End of fortieth day without a catch

Location:

Gulf Stream

On a boat

Enactors:

Old man (Santiago) → refusing to let boy join again

The boy → helps Santiago with lines, other equipment

Objects:

skiff = empty

scars → running down Santiago's face

sea → reflecting the sun

sail = furled, like a flag of defeat

lines = coiled

The diagram demonstrates how the first few paragraphs could trigger schemas around poverty, manual labour, conflict with nature, and loneliness. These diagrams can become significantly more sophisticated, with links to multiple text-worlds demonstrating when world-shifts occur. Text world scholars typically use diagrams as an entry point into the reader's experience and the cognitive experience of reading the text, which they elaborate on through in-depth analysis. [1] [8]

Use in education

Text world theory has been used in schools in order to encourage a personal engagement with texts from pupils and help teachers to understand the "nature of communicative interaction and literary transaction." [8] Louise Rosenblatt argues two types of reading are efferent reading (reading with an outcome in mind, such as reading a cookbook in order to make a meal) and aesthetic reading (reading while examining the emotional and intellectual experience of the reader). She argues that pupils are implicitly encouraged to read poetry in an efferent way. To show this, she gives an example of a third-grade textbook wherein a poem about a cow is followed by the question, "what facts does this poem teach you?". This literal-mindedness, she argues, had removed the agency of the pupil as a reader, which leads to a reduced engagement with literature. [10]

This aspect of Rosenblatt's writing, part of her work on reader response theory, has been linked to text world theory. Text world theory provides a model through which pupils can build an "authentic" text world, and monitor their cognitive experience while reading a text. It also models the "creative nature" inherent in the reading process as pupils construct their unique text worlds. [2]

In a classroom setting, discourse-worlds are complicated by the fact that there are multiple participants engaging with the discourse world, namely the other pupils and the educational staff. Moreover, in a classroom setting, there is often an unequal distribution of power. [11] Marcello Giovanelli argues that this unequal distribution of power can often lead to pupils believing that their interpretations of literature are less valuable than their teachers, which, in turn, leads to pupils failing to build an independent "text world". [8] One of the solutions that Giovanelli gives to this issue is the importance of pictorial representations of texts as related to the work in education through semiotics by Charles Suhor. [12] [8]

Several researchers have observed the benefits of using approaches informed by text world theory in the classroom. They both give pupils an understanding of the importance of multiple interpretations of texts, and give teachers a model for how pupils construct meaning during the reading process. [2] [3] [8]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Concept</span> Mental representation or an abstract object

A concept is defined as an abstract idea. It is understood to be a fundamental building block underlying principles, thoughts, and beliefs. Concepts play an important role in all aspects of cognition. As such, concepts are studied within such disciplines as linguistics, psychology, and philosophy, and these disciplines are interested in the logical and psychological structure of concepts, and how they are put together to form thoughts and sentences. The study of concepts has served as an important flagship of an emerging interdisciplinary approach, cognitive science.

Natural language processing (NLP) is an interdisciplinary subfield of computer science and artificial intelligence. It is primarily concerned with providing computers the ability to process data encoded in natural language and is thus closely related to information retrieval, knowledge representation and computational linguistics, a subfield of linguistics. Typically data is collected in text corpora, using either rule-based, statistical or neural-based approaches of machine learning and deep learning.

In cognitive linguistics, conceptual metaphor, or cognitive metaphor, refers to the understanding of one idea, or conceptual domain, in terms of another. An example of this is the understanding of quantity in terms of directionality or the understanding of time in terms of money.

Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are considered as psychologically real, and research in cognitive linguistics aims to help understand cognition in general and is seen as a road into the human mind.

Social constructivism is a sociological theory of knowledge according to which human development is socially situated, and knowledge is constructed through interaction with others. Like social constructionism, social constructivism states that people work together to actively construct artifacts. But while social constructivism focuses on cognition, social constructionism focuses on the making of social reality.

A graphic organizer, also known as a knowledge map, concept map, story map, cognitive organizer, advance organizer, or concept diagram, is a pedagogical tool that uses visual symbols to express knowledge and concepts through relationships between them. The main purpose of a graphic organizer is to provide a visual aid to facilitate learning and instruction.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Image schema</span> Recurring structure in cognitive processes

An image schema is a recurring structure within our cognitive processes which establishes patterns of understanding and reasoning. As an understudy to embodied cognition, image schemas are formed from our bodily interactions, from linguistic experience, and from historical context. The term is introduced in Mark Johnson's book The Body in the Mind; in case study 2 of George Lakoff's Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: and further explained by Todd Oakley in The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics; by Rudolf Arnheim in Visual Thinking; by the collection From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics edited by Beate Hampe and Joseph E. Grady.

Semantic memory refers to general world knowledge that humans have accumulated throughout their lives. This general knowledge is intertwined in experience and dependent on culture. New concepts are learned by applying knowledge learned from things in the past.

Narratology is the study of narrative and narrative structure and the ways that these affect human perception. The term is an anglicisation of French narratologie, coined by Tzvetan Todorov. Its theoretical lineage is traceable to Aristotle (Poetics) but modern narratology is agreed to have begun with the Russian formalists, particularly Vladimir Propp, and Mikhail Bakhtin's theories of heteroglossia, dialogism, and the chronotope first presented in The Dialogic Imagination (1975).

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mental model</span> Way of representing external reality within ones mind

A mental model is an internal representation (model) of external reality: that is, a way of representing reality within one's mind. Such models are hypothesized to play a major role in cognition, reasoning and decision-making. The term for this concept was coined in 1943 by Kenneth Craik, who suggested that the mind constructs "small-scale models" of reality that it uses to anticipate events.

Force dynamics is a semantic category that describes the way in which entities interact with reference to force. Force Dynamics gained a good deal of attention in cognitive linguistics due to its claims of psychological plausibility and the elegance with which it generalizes ideas not usually considered in the same context. The semantic category of force dynamics pervades language on several levels. Not only does it apply to expressions in the physical domain like leaning on or dragging, but it also plays an important role in expressions involving psychological forces. Furthermore, the concept of force dynamics can be extended to discourse. For example, the situation in which speakers A and B argue, after which speaker A gives in to speaker B, exhibits a force dynamic pattern.

The term conceptual model refers to any model that is formed after a conceptualization or generalization process. Conceptual models are often abstractions of things in the real world, whether physical or social. Semantic studies are relevant to various stages of concept formation. Semantics is fundamentally a study of concepts, the meaning that thinking beings give to various elements of their experience.

Cognitive poetics is a school of literary criticism that applies the principles of cognitive science, particularly cognitive psychology, to the interpretation of literary texts. It has ties to reader-response criticism, and also has a grounding in modern principles of cognitive linguistics. The research and focus on cognitive poetics paves way for psychological, sociocultural and indeed linguistic dimensions to develop in relation to stylistics.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Diagrammatic reasoning</span>

Diagrammatic reasoning is reasoning by means of visual representations. The study of diagrammatic reasoning is about the understanding of concepts and ideas, visualized with the use of diagrams and imagery instead of by linguistic or algebraic means.

Multiliteracy is an approach to literacy theory and pedagogy coined in the mid-1990s by the New London Group. The approach is characterized by two key aspects of literacy – linguistic diversity and multimodal forms of linguistic expressions and representation. It was coined in response to two major changes in the globalized environment. One such change was the growing linguistic and cultural diversity due to increased transnational migration. The second major change was the proliferation of new mediums of communication due to advancement in communication technologies e.g the internet, multimedia, and digital media. As a scholarly approach, multiliteracy focuses on the new "literacy" that is developing in response to the changes in the way people communicate globally due to technological shifts and the interplay between different cultures and languages.

Cognitive sociolinguistics is an emerging field of linguistics that aims to account for linguistic variation in social settings with a cognitive explanatory framework. The goal of cognitive sociolinguists is to build a mental model of society, individuals, institutions and their relations to one another. Cognitive sociolinguists also strive to combine theories and methods used in cognitive linguistics and sociolinguistics to provide a more productive framework for future research on language variation. This burgeoning field concerning social implications on cognitive linguistics has yet received universal recognition.

Raymond W. Gibbs Jr. is a former psychology professor and researcher at the University of California, Santa Cruz. His research interests are in the fields of experimental psycholinguistics and cognitive science. His work concerns a range of theoretical issues, ranging from questions about the role of embodied experience in thought and language, to looking at people's use and understanding of figurative language. Raymond Gibbs's research is especially focused on bodily experience and linguistic meaning. Much of his research is motivated by theories of meaning in philosophy, linguistics, and comparative literature.

In linguistics, psychology, and literary theory, the concepts of deictic field and deictic shift are sometimes deployed in the study of narrative media. These terms provide a theoretical framework for helping literary analysts to conceptualize the ways in which readers redirect their attention away from their immediate surroundings as they become immersed in the reality generated by the text.

The usage-based linguistics is a linguistics approach within a broader functional/cognitive framework, that emerged since the late 1980s, and that assumes a profound relation between linguistic structure and usage. It challenges the dominant focus, in 20th century linguistics, on considering language as an isolated system removed from its use in human interaction and human cognition. Rather, usage-based models posit that linguistic information is expressed via context-sensitive mental processing and mental representations, which have the cognitive ability to succinctly account for the complexity of actual language use at all levels. Broadly speaking, a usage-based model of language accounts for language acquisition and processing, synchronic and diachronic patterns, and both low-level and high-level structure in language, by looking at actual language use.

Cognitive discourse analysis (CODA) is a research method which examines natural language data in order to gain insights into patterns in (verbalisable) thought. The term was coined by Thora Tenbrink to describe a kind of discourse analysis that had been carried out by researchers in linguistics and other fields. As it is limited to examining verbalisable thought, CODA studies are often triangulated against other research methods. The method is theoretically neutral, and can therefore be used alongside a range of different models of cognition and grammar.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Gavins, Joanna (2007-03-07). Text World Theory An Introduction. Edinburgh University Press. doi:10.3366/edinburgh/9780748622993.001.0001. ISBN   978-0-7486-2299-3.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Cushing, Ian (2018-01-02). "'Suddenly, I am part of the poem': texts as worlds, reader-response and grammar in teaching poetry". English in Education. 52 (1): 7–19. doi:10.1080/04250494.2018.1414398. ISSN   0425-0494. S2CID   149068617.
  3. 1 2 3 4 Giovanelli, Marcello; Mason, Jessica (March 2015). "'Well I don't feel that': Schemas, worlds and authentic reading in the classroom". English in Education. 49 (1): 41–55. doi:10.1111/eie.12052. ISSN   0425-0494.
  4. "The Text World Theory Special Collection". Text World Theory. 2017-09-11. Retrieved 2024-03-01.
  5. Werth, Paul (1999). Text worlds: representing conceptual space in discourse. Textual explorations (1. publ ed.). Harlow: Longman. ISBN   978-0-582-22914-3.
  6. 1 2 Werth, Paul (1999). Text Worlds: Representing Conceptual Space in Discourse. Longman. ISBN   978-0-582-22914-3.
  7. "About Text World Theory". Text World Theory. 2015-02-10. Retrieved 2024-03-01.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Giovanelli, Marcello (2016-08-16). "Readers building fictional worlds: visual representations, poetry and cognition". Literacy. 51 (1): 26–35. doi:10.1111/lit.12091. ISSN   1741-4350.
  9. Edwards, Derek; Mercer, Neil (1987), "Communication and control", Common Knowledge (Routledge Revivals), Routledge, doi:10.4324/9780203095287-7, ISBN   978-0-203-09528-7 , retrieved 2024-02-18
  10. Rosenblatt, Louise Michelle (1998). The reader, the text, the poem: the transactional theory of the literary work ; with a new preface and epilogue (Paperback ed., [Nachdr.] ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. ISBN   978-0-8093-1805-6.
  11. Fairclough, Norman (2001), "Language and power 2000", Language and Power (2 ed.), Routledge, pp. 215–230, doi:10.4324/9781315838250-15, ISBN   978-1-315-83825-0 , retrieved 2024-02-18
  12. Suhor, Charles (July 1984). "Towards a Semiotics-based Curriculum". Journal of Curriculum Studies. 16 (3): 247–257. doi:10.1080/0022027840160304. ISSN   0022-0272.