Excitation-transfer theory, based heavily on psychology, psychophysiology, and biochemistry, is a psychological theory that originated in the field of social psychology and effects studies pertaining to communication. [1] In the context of communication, this theory suggests that the emotional response to a particular message or stimulus can be influenced by the residual, or remaining, arousal from a previous experience. [2] Excitation-transfer theory was first proposed by Dolf Zillmann in the 1970s to explain the emotional and physiological processes involved in the transfer of arousal from one situation to another. [1]
This theory, which applies elements of the three-factor theory of emotions, states that left over, or residual, excitation from the initial stimulus will amplify the excitatory response or reaction to another stimulus, regardless of the hedonic valences or potential experience one has had with the emotions felt from the stimuli. [3] [4] Hedonic valence, in particular, refers to the emotional tone or affective quality of an experience, stimulus, or object. [3] In addition, the excitation-transfer process is not limited to a single emotion, as the initial, residual, and excitatory emotional reactions do not have to be related. [4]
The process of excitation-transfer occurs when the feelings of arousal, or another emotion of excitation, that stem from one stimulus is converted, or misattributed, into a different action or behavior due to a secondary stimulus. [5] In addition, the transfer of one emotion to another will result in the second emotion directed toward the additional stimulus being felt more intensely than if the emotion caused by the first stimulus was not felt. [5] Components including dispositional and excitatory emotional responses related to the three-factor theory of emotions are also correlated to the excitation-transfer process. [1] [4]
Developed research and applied studies in which this theory has been tested has led to the development of specific conditions required for the excitation-transfer process to occur. These conditions include time, shift of attention and hedonic assimilation. [1] Examples of how the theory is applied are also provided.
In addition, research has also found limitations of excitation-transfer theory, which are noted as areas requiring further research. [6]
Dolf Zillmann began developing excitation-transfer theory in the late 1960s through the early 1970s and continued to refine it into the 21st century. [1] The theory itself is based largely on Clark Hull's notion of residual excitation (i.e., drive theory), Stanley Schachter's two factor theory of emotion, and the application of the three-factor theory of emotions. [1] [7] Specifically, Zillmann noticed a fault within the two factor theory of emotion regarding the lack of information that addressed the relationship between cognitive behaviors and cues within the theory. [1]
In these theories working as the starting point, excitation-transfer theory is able to make assumptions regarding how the transition of one emotion to another cognitively occurs. [4] One of the assumptions states that excitation responses are, for the most part, ambiguous and are differentiated only by what emotions the brain assigns to them in that specific instance. [8] This theory also helps to explain the variability of emotional arousal (including how it is possible for fear to be transferred into relief, anger into delight, etc.) and how the reaction to one stimulus can intensify the reaction to another. [8]
During the development of excitation-transfer theory, the public became concerned about the potential real-world effects violent media content could have on consumers. [1] As a result, excitation-transfer theory became one of the dominant theoretical foundations used for predicting, testing, and explaining the potential effects of media such as violent films, pornography, and music onto the population. [1]
Furthermore, Zillmann also expressed that excitation transfer is not limited to face-to-face communication stimuli, as the process can occur from an array of stimuli including mediated messages. [9] Research focusing on this theory has found a correlation between cognitive levels of arousal and behavioral actions as a result of the media being consumed. [1]
The three-factor theory of emotions works as a framework of application that allows excitation-transfer theory to categorize the emotional behaviors experienced through the excitation-transfer process, which include dispositional, excitatory and experimental. [1] [4] [8] [10]
The three-factory theory of emotions suggests that physiological arousal comes before and leads into both outward expressions of emotions (dispositional) and the subjective experience of emotions (experiential). [1] [10] The components of the theory include:
The way in which excitation-transfer theory applies the concepts of the three-factor theory of emotions are that it highlights the association of physiological arousal, dispositional behaviors, and subjective experiences across different emotional contexts. [1] [4] [11] In addition, the utilization of the three-factory theory of emotions provides insight regarding how dynamic and influential emotional, arousing responses can be when transferred from one situation to another. [1] [4] [11]
Three major concepts of the excitation-transfer theory consist of:
Arousal refers to a heightened state of physiological and psychological activity. It can be triggered by various stimuli, including emotional experiences (such as excitement or fear), physical activities (such as exercise), or intense situations (such as viewing thrilling media content). [3] [9] [12]
Excitation, within the excitation-transfer framework, refers to the residual physiological arousal left over from a previous event or experience. [13] Furthermore, the lingering feelings of arousal can persist for a while after the initial stimulus has ended. [4] [12]
Transfer occurs when the residual arousal, or excitation, from one event influences the emotional response to a following and seemingly unrelated event. This implies that the emotional intensity experienced in the first situation would be carried over and effect the reactions felt toward the second situation. [4] [13] The arousal transferred through this process can also amplify the emotional reaction to the new stimulus. [3]
The excitation-transfer process requires the presence of these three conditions:
Two additional components of excitation-transfer theory consist of:
The requirements for the occurrence of excitation-transfer, particularly related to time, shift of attention, and hedonic assimilation, are important aspects of understanding how physiological arousal from one stimulus may affect the response to a secondary stimulus. [1] Together, these factors provide a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms involved in excitation transfer.
Time plays an essential, yet complex role in excitation transfer theory. [1] Specific terms used to explain the importance of time in the emotional transfer process include:
The concept of time in this theory emphasizes that the transfer of excitation is typically more effective when there is a relatively short time interval between the initial arousal-inducing event and the subsequent stimulus. [1] [4]
The recency of the arousal is crucial in the sense that if the initial arousal is recent, the physiological activation it is associated with is more likely to be present during the encounter with the subsequent stimulus. This recency contributes to the transfer of excitation from one situation to another.l [1] [4]
As time elapses, the physiological arousal from the initial event tends to disappear. The theory suggests that the transfer effect is most potent when the residual arousal is still present during the encounter with the second stimulus. [1] [4]
Response latency refers to the time it takes for an individual to respond to a stimulus. In content of this theory, response latency may be able to enhance the likelihood of the excitation-transfer process occurring. For example, a short response latency may enhance the likelihood of the transfer of excitatory emotions. [1] [4]
Excitation delay is the amount of time in between the initial arousal and the creation of the emotional response. This factor can influence the emotional transfer process, as a shorter excitation delay may result in a more effective transfer of arousal to a subsequent stimulus. [1] [4]
Time of recovery refers to the duration of time it takes for physiological arousal to return to the baseline, or initially felt, emotion after experiencing the stimuli. To elaborate, a high time of recovery would imply that the physiological arousal persists for a longer timeframe without returning to the baseline emotion. This would imply that there is higher potential for the excitation-transfer process to take place and influence following situations. In addition, a low time of recovery would suggest a quicker return to the baseline emotion. This would diminish the potential for the excitation-transfer process to take place and influence preceding situations. [1] [4]
For excitation-transfer theory, attentional shift guides the cognitive processing and selective attention that contribute to the transfer of emotions. [1] [16] Specific terms used to explain the importance of the shift of attention in the emotional process include:
Excitation-transfer theory and hedonic assimilation are related concepts that both pertain to the influence of prior experiences on current emotional responses. [3] While excitation-transfer theory focuses on the transfer of arousal from one situation to another, hedonic assimilation is more specific in addressing how the affective tone (pleasant or unpleasant) of a prior experience can influence the evaluation of a subsequent and potentially unrelated stimulus. [1] [3] [4] Zillmann proposed the idea that hedonic assimilation may serve as precondition needed in order for the excitation-transfer process of emotions to take place. [1]
While both excitation-transfer theory and hedonic assimilation involve the influence of prior experiences on current emotional responses, they differ in the sense that excitation-transfer theory focuses more on the transfer of physiological arousal, while hedonic assimilation is most concerned with the transfer of the hedonic (emotional) tones. [1] [4]
Using examples of how the excitation-transfer theory process works is beneficial, as it helps conceptualized the components and tangible effects of the theory.
Example - Watching a movie within the genre of thriller, suspense or horror.
Explanation - The initial stimulus would be the act or situation of watching a movie within one of these genres. Physical responses including an increased heart rate and elevated levels of adrenaline would be the physiological arousal components of the theory. If the heightened emotions from the physiology arousal transfer from watching the movie to an additional or subsequent situation, this experience would fall into the transfer of physiological arousal category. The transfer of emotions could appear as the viewer holding residual feeling of arousal that came from watching the movie that are then reflected onto another action or situation. The secondary action or situation may include answering a phone call with increased enthusiasm or excitement. This process would be considered misattribution of excitation.
Example - Riding a roller coster ride at an amusement park.
Explanation - The initial stimulus would be the act of riding the roller coaster. Physical responses including an elevated heart rate and an adrenaline rush while on the ride are considered the physiological arousal components of the theory. If the heightened emotions from the physiology arousal transfer from the riding the roller coaster to an additional or subsequent situation, this experience would fall into the transfer of physiological arousal category. The transfer of emotions could appear as the rider holding residual feeling of arousal from riding the roller coaster that are then reflected onto another action or situation. This subsequent action or situation may include feeling extremely happy to see a friend after getting off of the ride, which would be considered the misattribution of excitation.
In both examples, the excitation-transfer process involves an initial stimulus (watching a thrilling, suspenseful, or horror-filled movie, or riding a roller coaster) that induces physiological arousal. This arousal transfers to a subsequent stimulus (answering a phone call or meeting a friend) and is misattributed to the new situation. As a result, this misattribution had the ability to influence a person's emotional and behavioral responses.
Researchers have found there to be a relation between excitation-transfer and the effect of media violence. This concept is explained in the sense that when media violence is observed by a viewer, the first stimulus, it will cause them to be emotionally aroused. When approached with a second stimulus soon after being aroused by the media violence, the emotional reaction to said stimulus will be far more intense because of the arousal from the initial stimulus. [7] [15]
Excitation-transfer theory, while offering insights, is just one perspective on the complex effects sexual media can have on audience members. The application of this theory to sexual media involves examining how physiological arousal generated by exposure to sexual content can transfer to subsequent situations that may influence emotional or behavioral responses. Research has also studied the potential effects of sexual media in terms of relationship dynamics, sexual attitudes, and the misattribution of arousal. [7] [12]
Additional areas where excitation-transfer theory can and have be applied through research include advertisements, humor and empathy. [1] [17] [18] [19]
Today, excitation-transfer theory remains a key component of the theoretical framework of studies focusing on communication and emotion. However, many current studies have run into limitations of the theory that require additional research to potentially build out the components of excitation-transfer theory. [17] [18]
Limitations of excitation-transfer theory include:
Based on previously conducted studies using this theory as a theoretical framework, areas where excitation-transfer theory can potentially be built out include:
Emotions are physical and mental states brought on by neurophysiological changes, variously associated with thoughts, feelings, behavioral responses, and a degree of pleasure or displeasure. There is no scientific consensus on a definition. Emotions are often intertwined with mood, temperament, personality, disposition, or creativity.
Arousal is the physiological and psychological state of being awoken or of sense organs stimulated to a point of perception. It involves activation of the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) in the brain, which mediates wakefulness, the autonomic nervous system, and the endocrine system, leading to increased heart rate and blood pressure and a condition of sensory alertness, desire, mobility, and reactivity.
Motivational salience is a cognitive process and a form of attention that motivates or propels an individual's behavior towards or away from a particular object, perceived event or outcome. Motivational salience regulates the intensity of behaviors that facilitate the attainment of a particular goal, the amount of time and energy that an individual is willing to expend to attain a particular goal, and the amount of risk that an individual is willing to accept while working to attain a particular goal.
The two-factor theory of emotion posits when an emotion is felt, a physiological arousal occurs and the person uses the immediate environment to search for emotional cues to label the physiological arousal.
Opponent-process theory is a psychological and neurological model that accounts for a wide range of behaviors, including color vision. This model was first proposed in 1878 by Ewald Hering, a German physiologist, and later expanded by Richard Solomon, a 20th-century psychologist.
Affect, in psychology, is the underlying experience of feeling, emotion, attachment, or mood. It encompasses a wide range of emotional states and can be positive or negative. Affect is a fundamental aspect of human experience and plays a central role in many psychological theories and studies. It can be understood as a combination of three components: emotion, mood, and affectivity. In psychology, the term affect is often used interchangeably with several related terms and concepts, though each term may have slightly different nuances. These terms encompass: emotion, feeling, mood, emotional state, sentiment, affective state, emotional response, affective reactivity, disposition. Researchers and psychologists may employ specific terms based on their focus and the context of their work.
Media psychology is the branch and specialty field in psychology that focuses on the interaction of human behavior with media and technology. Media psychology is not limited to mass media or media content; it includes all forms of mediated communication and media technology-related behaviors, such as the use, design, impact, and sharing behaviors. This branch is a relatively new field of study because of advancement in technology. It uses various methods of critical analysis and investigation to develop a working model of a user's perception of media experience. These methods are employed for society as a whole and on an individual basis. Media psychologists are able to perform activities that include consulting, design, and production in various media like television, video games, films, and news broadcasting. Media psychologists are not considered to be those who are featured in media, rather than those who research, work or contribute to the field. Mediacology is a new term used as a collaborative word of Media and Psychology.
Emotionality is the observable behavioral and physiological component of emotion. It is a measure of a person's emotional reactivity to a stimulus. Most of these responses can be observed by other people, while some emotional responses can only be observed by the person experiencing them. Observable responses to emotion do not have a single meaning. A smile can be used to express happiness or anxiety, while a frown can communicate sadness or anger. Emotionality is often used by experimental psychology researchers to operationalize emotion in research studies.
Stanley Schachter was an American social psychologist best known for his development of the two factor theory of emotion in 1962 along with Jerome E. Singer. In his theory he states that emotions have two ingredients: physiological arousal and a cognitive label. A person's experience of an emotion stems from the mental awareness of the body's physical arousal and the explanation one attaches to this arousal. Schachter also studied and published many works on the subjects of obesity, group dynamics, birth order and smoking. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Schachter as the seventh most cited psychologist of the 20th century.
Appraisal theory is the theory in psychology that emotions are extracted from our evaluations of events that cause specific reactions in different people. Essentially, our appraisal of a situation causes an emotional, or affective, response that is going to be based on that appraisal. An example of this is going on a first date. If the date is perceived as positive, one might feel happiness, joy, giddiness, excitement, and/or anticipation, because they have appraised this event as one that could have positive long-term effects, i.e. starting a new relationship, engagement, or even marriage. On the other hand, if the date is perceived negatively, then our emotions, as a result, might include dejection, sadness, emptiness, or fear. Reasoning and understanding of one's emotional reaction becomes important for future appraisals as well. The important aspect of the appraisal theory is that it accounts for individual variability in emotional reactions to the same event.
In psychology, desensitization is a treatment or process that diminishes emotional responsiveness to a negative, aversive, or positive stimulus after repeated exposure. Desensitization can also occur when an emotional response is repeatedly evoked when the action tendency associated with the emotion proves irrelevant or unnecessary. The process of desensitization was developed by psychologist Mary Cover Jones and is primarily used to assist individuals in unlearning phobias and anxieties. Joseph Wolpe (1958) developed a hierarchal list of anxiety-evoking stimuli in order of intensity, which allows individuals to undergo adaptation. Wolpe's "reciprocal inhibition" desensitization process is based on well-known psychology theories such as Hull's "drive-reduction" theory and Sherrington's concept of "reciprocal inhibition." Although medication is available for individuals with anxiety, fear, or phobias, empirical evidence supports desensitization with high rates of cure, particularly in clients with depression or schizophrenia.
Mood management theory posits that the consumption of messages, particularly entertaining messages, is capable of altering prevailing mood states, and that the selection of specific messages for consumption often serves the regulation of mood states. Mood management theory now belongs to a larger group of theoretical approaches which speak on media content and how it plays a role in mood management. Mood optimization is a current affective sate of media mood management.
In psychology, misattribution of arousal is the process whereby people make a mistake in assuming what is causing them to feel aroused. For example, when actually experiencing physiological responses related to fear, people mislabel those responses as romantic arousal. The reason physiological symptoms may be attributed to incorrect stimuli is because many stimuli have similar physiological symptoms such as increased blood pressure or shortness of breath.
Affect displays are the verbal and non-verbal displays of affect (emotion). These displays can be through facial expressions, gestures and body language, volume and tone of voice, laughing, crying, etc. Affect displays can be altered or faked so one may appear one way, when they feel another. Affect can be conscious or non-conscious and can be discreet or obvious. The display of positive emotions, such as smiling, laughing, etc., is termed "positive affect", while the displays of more negative emotions, such as crying and tense gestures, is respectively termed "negative affect".
The self-regulation of emotion or emotion regulation is the ability to respond to the ongoing demands of experience with the range of emotions in a manner that is socially tolerable and sufficiently flexible to permit spontaneous reactions as well as the ability to delay spontaneous reactions as needed. It can also be defined as extrinsic and intrinsic processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions. The self-regulation of emotion belongs to the broader set of emotion regulation processes, which includes both the regulation of one's own feelings and the regulation of other people's feelings.
Emotion can have a powerful effect on humans and animals. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.
Sexual arousal describes the physiological and psychological responses in preparation for sexual intercourse or when exposed to sexual stimuli. A number of physiological responses occur in the body and mind as preparation for sexual intercourse, and continue during intercourse. Male arousal will lead to an erection, and in female arousal, the body's response is engorged sexual tissues such as nipples, clitoris, vaginal walls, and vaginal lubrication.
Emotion perception refers to the capacities and abilities of recognizing and identifying emotions in others, in addition to biological and physiological processes involved. Emotions are typically viewed as having three components: subjective experience, physical changes, and cognitive appraisal; emotion perception is the ability to make accurate decisions about another's subjective experience by interpreting their physical changes through sensory systems responsible for converting these observed changes into mental representations. The ability to perceive emotion is believed to be both innate and subject to environmental influence and is also a critical component in social interactions. How emotion is experienced and interpreted depends on how it is perceived. Likewise, how emotion is perceived is dependent on past experiences and interpretations. Emotion can be accurately perceived in humans. Emotions can be perceived visually, audibly, through smell and also through bodily sensations and this process is believed to be different from the perception of non-emotional material.
Expressive suppression is defined as the intentional reduction of the facial expression of an emotion. It is a component of emotion regulation.
Dolf Zillmann is dean emeritus, and professor of information sciences, communication and psychology at the University of Alabama (UA). Zillmann predominantly conducted research in media psychology, a branch of psychology focused on the effects of media consumption on human affect, developing and expanding a range of theories within media psychology and communication. His work centred on the relation between aggression, emotion, and arousal through media consumption, predominantly in pornography and violent genres of movie and television. His research also includes the effects of music consumption, video games, and sports.
This article needs additional or more specific categories .(December 2023) |