Priming (psychology)

Last updated

Priming is a concept in psychology to describe how exposure to one stimulus may influence a response to a subsequent stimulus, without conscious guidance or intention. [1] [2] [3] The priming effect is the positive or negative effect of a rapidly presented stimulus (priming stimulus) on the processing of a second stimulus (target stimulus) that appears shortly after. Generally speaking, the generation of priming effect depends on the existence of some positive or negative relationship between priming and target stimuli. For example, the word nurse might be recognized more quickly following the word doctor than following the word bread. Priming can be perceptual, associative, repetitive, positive, negative, affective, semantic, or conceptual. Priming effects involve word recognition, semantic processing, attention, unconscious processing, and many other issues, and are related to differences in various writing systems. How quickly this effect occurs is contested; [4] [5] some researchers claim that priming effects are almost instantaneous. [6]

Contents

Priming works most effectively when the two stimuli are in the same modality. For example, visual priming works best with visual cues and verbal priming works best with verbal cues. But priming also occurs between modalities, [7] or between semantically related words such as "doctor" and "nurse". [8] [9]

In 2012, a great amount of priming research was thrown into doubt as part of the replication crisis. Many of the landmark studies that found effects of priming were unable to be replicated in new trials using the same mechanisms. [10] The experimenter effect may have allowed the people running the experiments to subtly influence them to reach the desired result, and Publication bias tended to mean that shocking and positive results were seen as interesting and more likely to be published than studies that failed to show any effect of priming. The result is that the efficacy of priming may have been greatly overstated in earlier literature, or have been entirely illusory. [11] [12]

Types

Positive and negative priming

The terms positive and negative priming refer to when priming affects the speed of processing. A positive prime speeds up processing, while a negative prime lowers the speed to slower than un-primed levels. [13] Positive priming is caused by simply experiencing the stimulus, [14] while negative priming is caused by experiencing the stimulus, and then ignoring it. [13] [15] Positive priming effects happen even if the prime is not consciously perceived. [14] The effects of positive and negative priming are visible in event-related potential (ERP) readings. [16]

Positive priming is thought to be caused by spreading activation. [14] This means that the first stimulus activates parts of a particular representation or association in memory just before carrying out an action or task. The representation is already partially activated when the second stimulus is encountered, so less additional activation is needed for one to become consciously aware of it.

Negative priming is more difficult to explain. Many models have been hypothesized, but currently the most widely accepted are the distractor inhibition and episodic retrieval models. [13] In the distractor inhibition model, the activation of ignored stimuli is inhibited by the brain. [13] The episodic retrieval model hypothesizes that ignored items are flagged 'do-not-respond' by the brain. Later, when the brain acts to retrieve this information, the tag causes a conflict. The time taken to resolve this conflict causes negative priming. [13] Although both models are still valid, recent scientific research has led scientists to lean away from the distractor inhibitor model. [13]

Perceptual and conceptual priming

The difference between perceptual and conceptual types of priming is whether items with a similar form or items with a similar meaning are primed, respectively.

Perceptual priming is based on the form of the stimulus and is enhanced by the match between the early and later stimuli. Perceptual priming is sensitive to the modality and exact format of the stimulus. An example of perceptual priming is the identification of an incomplete word in a word-stem completion test. The presentation of the visual prime does not have to be perfectly consistent with later testing presentations in order to work. Studies have shown that, for example, the absolute size of the stimuli can vary and still provide significant evidence of priming. [17]

Conceptual priming is based on the meaning of a stimulus and is enhanced by semantic tasks. For example, the word table will show conceptual priming effects on the word chair, because the words belong to the same category. [18]

Repetition priming

Repetition priming, also called direct priming, is a form of positive priming. When a stimulus is experienced, it is also primed. This means that later experiences of the stimulus will be processed more quickly by the brain. [19] This effect has been found on words in the lexical decision task. There are multiple theories and models that reason why repetition priming might exist. For example, facilitation suggests that when a stimulus overlaps with existing or previously seen representation than information will travel faster. [20]

Semantic priming

This image shows a priming web built from different types of priming. The lines in this web indicate associations that an individual might have. If two words are more closely linked in the web, then they are more likely to be more quickly recognized when primed with a nearby word. The dotted lines indicate morpheme primes, or primes from words that sound similar to each other, while the straight lines indicate semantic primes or words that have meanings or associations that relate to each other. Priming Web Diagram.svg
This image shows a priming web built from different types of priming. The lines in this web indicate associations that an individual might have. If two words are more closely linked in the web, then they are more likely to be more quickly recognized when primed with a nearby word. The dotted lines indicate morpheme primes, or primes from words that sound similar to each other, while the straight lines indicate semantic primes or words that have meanings or associations that relate to each other.

In semantic priming, the prime and the target are from the same semantic category and share features. [21] For example, the word dog is a semantic prime for wolf, because the two are similar animals. Semantic priming is theorized to work because of spreading activation within associative networks. [14] When a person thinks of one item in a category, similar items are stimulated by the brain. Even if they are not words, morphemes can prime for complete words that include them. [22] An example of this would be that the morpheme 'psych' can prime for the word 'psychology'.

In support with further detail, when an individual processes a word sometimes that word can be affected when the prior word is linked semantically. Previous studies have been conducted, focusing on priming effects having a rapid rise time and a hasty decay time. For example, an experiment by Donald Foss researched the decay time of semantic facilitation in lists and sentences. Three experiments were conducted and it was found that semantic relationships within words differs when words occur in sentences rather than lists. Thus, supporting the ongoing discourse model. [23]

Associative priming

In associative priming, the target is a word that has a high probability of appearing with the prime, and is "associated" with it but not necessarily related in semantic features. The word dog is an associative prime for cat, since the words are closely associated and frequently appear together (in phrases like "raining cats and dogs"). [24] A similar effect is known as context priming. Context priming works by using a context to speed up processing for stimuli that are likely to occur in that context. A useful application of this effect is reading written text. [25] The grammar and vocabulary of the sentence provide contextual clues for words that will occur later in the sentence. These later words are processed more quickly than if they had been read alone, and the effect is greater for more difficult or uncommon words. [25]

Response priming

In the psychology of visual perception and motor control, the term response priming denotes a special form of visuomotor priming effect. The distinctive feature of response priming is that prime and target are presented in quick succession (typically, less than 100 milliseconds apart) and are coupled to identical or alternative motor responses. [26] [27] When a speeded motor response is performed to classify the target stimulus, a prime immediately preceding the target can thus induce response conflicts when assigned to a different response as the target. These response conflicts have observable effects on motor behavior, leading to priming effects, e.g., in response times and error rates. A special property of response priming is its independence from visual awareness of the prime: For example, response priming effects can increase under conditions where visual awareness of the prime is decreasing. [28] [29]

Masked priming

The masked priming paradigm has been widely used in the last two decades in order to investigate both orthographic and phonological activations during visual word recognition. The term "masked" refers to the fact that the prime word or pseudoword is masked by symbols such as ###### that can be presented in a forward manner (before the prime) or a backward manner (after the prime). These masks enable to diminish the visibility of the prime. The prime is usually presented less than 80 ms (but typically between 40-60 ms) in this paradigm. In all, the short SOA (Stimuli Onset Asynchrony, i.e. the time delay between the onset of the mask and the prime) associated with the masking make the masked priming paradigm a good tool to investigate automatic and irrespective activations during visual word recognition. [30] Forster has argued that masked priming is a purer form of priming, as any conscious appreciation of the relationship between the prime and the target is effectively eliminated, and thus removes the subject's ability to use the prime strategically to make decisions. Results from numerous experiments show that certain forms of priming occur that are very difficult to occur with visible primes. One such example is form-priming, where the prime is similar to, but not identical to the target (e.g., the words nature and mature). [31] [32] Form priming is known to be affected by several psycholinguistic properties such as prime-target frequency and overlap. If a prime is higher frequency than the target, lexical competition occurs, whereas if the target has a higher frequency than the prime, then the prime pre-activates the target [33] and if the prime and target differ by one letter and one phoneme, the prime competes with the target, leading to lexical competition. [34] Not only is it affected by the prime and target, but also by individual differences such that people with well-established lexical representations are more likely to show lexical competition than people with less-established lexical representation. [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] [40]

Kindness priming

Kindness priming is a specific form of priming that occurs when a subject experiences an act of kindness and subsequently experiences a lower threshold of activation when subsequently encountering positive stimuli. A unique feature of kindness priming is that it causes a temporarily increased resistance to negative stimuli in addition to the increased activation of positive associative networks. [41] This form of priming is closely related to affect priming.

Affective priming

Affective or affect priming entails the evaluation of people, ideas, objects, goods, etc., not only based on the physical features of those things, but also on affective context. Most research and concepts about affective priming derive from the affective priming paradigm where people are asked to evaluate or respond to a stimuli following positive, neutral, or negative primes. Some research suggests that valence (positive vs. negative) has a stronger effect than arousal (low vs. high) on lexical-decision tasks. [42] Affective priming might also be more diffuse and stronger when the prime barely enters conscious awareness. [43] Evaluation of emotions can be primed by other emotions as well. Thus, neutral pictures, when presented after unpleasant pictures, are perceived as more pleasant than when presented after pleasant pictures. [44]

Cultural priming

1122-WAS-The White House.JPG
Beijing-forbidden4.jpg
Priming with different cultural knowledge alters the way people think.

Cultural priming is a technique employed in the field of cross-cultural psychology and social psychology to understand how people interpret events and other concepts, like cultural frame switching and self-concept. [45] :270 [46] For example, Hung and his associate displayed to participants a different set of culture related images, like U.S. Capitol building vs Chinese temple, and then watch a clip of fish swimming ahead of a group of fishes. [47] When exposed to the latter one, Hong Kong participants are more likely to reason in a collectivistic way. [48] :187 In contrast, their counterparts who view western images are more likely to give a reverse response and focus more on that individual fish. [49] :787 [50] People from bi-culture society when primed with different cultural icons, they are inclined to make cultural activated attribution. [45] :327 One method is the Pronoun circling task, a type of cultural priming task, which involves asking participants to consciously circle pronouns like "We", "us", "I", and "me" during paragraph reading. [51] [52] :381

Anti-priming

Anti-priming is a measurable impairment in processing information owing to recent processing of other information when the representations of information overlap and compete. Strengthening one representation after its usage causes priming for that item but also anti-priming for some other, non-repeated items. [53] For example, in one study, identification accuracy of old Chinese characters was significantly higher than baseline measurements (i.e., the priming effect), while identification accuracy of novel characters was significantly lower than baseline measurements (i.e., the anti-priming effect). [54] Anti-priming is said to be the natural antithesis of repetition priming, and it manifests when two objects share component features, thereby having overlapping representations. [55] However, one study failed to find anti-priming effects in a picture-naming task even though repetition priming effects were observed. Researchers argue that anti-priming effects may not be observed in a small time-frame. [55]

Replicability controversy

The replicability and interpretation of priming findings has become controversial. [10] Studies in 2012 failed to replicate findings, including age priming, [12] with additional reports of failure to replicate this and other findings such as social-distance also reported. [56] [57]

Priming is often considered to play a part in the success of sensory branding of products and connected to ideas like crossmodal correspondencies and sensation transference. Known effects are e.g. consumers perceiving lemonade suddenly as sweeter when the logo of the drink is more saturated towards yellow, [58] although this result has not yet been replicated by an independent study.

Although semantic, associative, and form priming are well established, [59] some longer-term priming effects were not replicated in further studies, casting doubt on their effectiveness or even existence. [60] Nobel laureate and psychologist Daniel Kahneman has called on priming researchers to check the robustness of their findings in an open letter to the community, claiming that priming has become a "poster child for doubts about the integrity of psychological research." [61] In 2022, Kahneman described behavioral priming research as "effectively dead." [62] Other critics have asserted that priming studies suffer from major publication bias, [11] experimenter effect [12] and that criticism of the field is not dealt with constructively. [63]

Measuring the effects of priming

Priming effects can be found with many of the tests of implicit memory. Tests such as the word-stem completion task, and the word fragment completion task measure perceptual priming. In the word-stem completion task, participants are given a list of study words, and then asked to complete word "stems" consisting of 3 letters with the first word that comes to mind. A priming effect is observed when participants complete stems with words on the study list more often than with novel words. The word fragment completion task is similar, but instead of being given the stem of a word, participants are given a word with some letters missing. The lexical decision task can be used to demonstrate conceptual priming. [8] [64] In this task, participants are asked to determine if a given string is a word or a nonword. Priming is demonstrated when participants are quicker to respond to words that have been primed with semantically-related words, e.g., faster to confirm "nurse" as a word when it is preceded by "doctor" than when it is preceded by "butter". Other evidence has been found through brain imaging and studies from brain injured patients. Another example of priming in healthcare research was studying if safety behaviors of nurses could be primed by structuring change of shift report. [65] A pilot simulation study found that there is early evidence to show that safety behaviors can be primed by including safety language into report. [65]

Effects of brain injuries

Amnesia

Patients with amnesia are described as those who have suffered damage to their medial temporal lobe, resulting in the impairment of explicit recollection of everyday facts and events. Priming studies on amnesic patients have varying results, depending on both the type of priming test done, as well as the phrasing of the instructions.

Amnesic patients do as well on perceptual priming tasks as healthy patients, [66] however they show some difficulties completing conceptual priming tasks, depending on the specific test. For example, they perform normally on category instance production tasks, but show impaired priming on any task that involves answering general knowledge questions. [67] [68]

Phrasing of the instructions associated with the test has had a dramatic impact on an amnesic's ability to complete the task successfully. When performing a word-stem completion test, patients were able to successfully complete the task when asked to complete the stem using the first word that came to mind, but when explicitly asked to recall a word to complete the stem that was on the study list, patients performed at below-average levels. [69]

Overall, studies from amnesic patients indicate that priming is controlled by a brain system separate from the medial temporal system that supports explicit memory.

Aphasia

Perhaps the first use of semantic priming in neurological patients was with stroke patients with aphasia. In one study, patients with Wernicke's aphasia who were unable to make semantic judgments showed evidence of semantic priming, while patients with Broca's aphasia who were able to make semantic judgments showed less consistent priming than those with Wernicke's aphasia or normal controls. This dissociation was extended to other linguistic categories such phonology and syntactic processing by Blumstein, Milberg and their colleagues. [70]

Dementia

Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, have been studied extensively as far as priming goes. Results are conflicting in some cases, but overall, patients with AD show decreased priming effects on word-stem completion and free association tasks, while retaining normal performance on lexical decision tasks. [71] These results suggest that AD patients are impaired in any sort of priming task that requires semantic processing of the stimuli, while priming tasks that require visuoperceptual interpretation of stimuli are unaffected by Alzheimers.

Focal cortical lesions

Patient J.P., who suffered a stroke in the left medial/temporal gyrus, resulting in auditory verbal agnosia - the inability to comprehend spoken words, but maintaining the ability to read and write, and with no effects to hearing ability. J.P. showed normal perceptual priming, but his conceptual priming ability for spoken words was, expectedly, impaired. [72] Another patient, N.G., who suffered from prosopanomia (the inability to retrieve proper names) following damage to his left temporal lobe, was unable to spontaneously provide names of persons or cities, but was able to successfully complete a word-fragment completion exercise following priming with these names. This demonstrated intact perceptual priming abilities. [73]

Cognitive neuroscience

Perceptual priming

The extrastriate cortex (shown in orange and red) is believed to be involved in perceptual priming. Brodmann areas 17 18 19.png
The extrastriate cortex (shown in orange and red) is believed to be involved in perceptual priming.

Priming while improving performance decreases neural processing in the cerebral cortex of sensory stimuli with stimulus repetition. This has been found in single-cell recordings [74] and in electroencephalography (EEG) upon gamma waves, [75] with PET [76] and functional MRI. [77] This reduction is due to representational sharpening in the early sensory areas which reduces the number of neurons representing the stimulus. This leads to a more selective activation of neurons representing objects in higher cognitive areas. [78]

Conceptual priming

Conceptual priming has been linked to reduced blood flow in the left prefrontal cortex. [79] The left prefrontal cortex is believed to be involved in the semantic processing of words, among other tasks. [80]

The view that perceptual priming is controlled by the extrastriate cortex while conceptual priming is controlled by the left prefrontal cortex is undoubtedly an oversimplified view of the process, and current work is focused on elucidating the brain regions involved in priming in more detail. [81]

In daily life

Priming is thought to play a large part in the systems of stereotyping. [82] This is because attention to a response increases the frequency of that response, even if the attended response is undesired. [82] [83] The attention given to these response or behaviors primes them for later activation. [82] Another way to explain this process is automaticity. If trait descriptions, for instance "stupid" or "friendly", have been frequently or recently used, these descriptions can be automatically used to interpret someone's behavior. An individual is unaware of this, and this may lead to behavior that may not agree with their personal beliefs. [84]

This can occur even if the subject is not conscious of the priming stimulus. [82] An example of this was done by John Bargh et al. in 1996. Subjects were implicitly primed with words related to the stereotype of elderly people (example: Florida, forgetful, wrinkle). While the words did not explicitly mention speed or slowness, those who were primed with these words walked more slowly upon exiting the testing booth than those who were primed with neutral stimuli. [82] Similar effects were found with rude and polite stimuli: those primed with rude words were more likely to interrupt an investigator than those primed with neutral words, and those primed with polite words were the least likely to interrupt. [82] A 2008 study showed that something as simple as holding a hot or cold beverage before an interview could result in pleasant or negative opinion of the interviewer. [85]

These findings have been extended to therapeutic interventions. For example, a 2012 study suggested that presented with a depressed patient who "self-stereotypes herself as incompetent, a therapist can find ways to prime her with specific situations in which she had been competent in the past... Making memories of her competence more salient should reduce her self-stereotype of incompetence." [86]

See also

Related Research Articles

The mere-exposure effect is a psychological phenomenon by which people tend to develop liking or disliking for things merely because they are familiar with them. In social psychology, this effect is sometimes called the familiarity principle. The effect has been demonstrated with many kinds of things, including words, Chinese characters, paintings, pictures of faces, geometric figures, and sounds. In studies of interpersonal attraction, the more often people see a person, the more pleasing and likeable they find that person.

The spacing effect demonstrates that learning is more effective when study sessions are spaced out. This effect shows that more information is encoded into long-term memory by spaced study sessions, also known as spaced repetition or spaced presentation, than by massed presentation ("cramming").

The Levels of Processing model, created by Fergus I. M. Craik and Robert S. Lockhart in 1972, describes memory recall of stimuli as a function of the depth of mental processing. More analysis produce more elaborate and stronger memory than lower levels of processing. Depth of processing falls on a shallow to deep continuum. Shallow processing leads to a fragile memory trace that is susceptible to rapid decay. Conversely, deep processing results in a more durable memory trace. There are three levels of processing in this model. Structural processing, or visual, is when we remember only the physical quality of the word. Phonemic processing includes remembering the word by the way it sounds. Lastly, we have semantic processing in which we encode the meaning of the word with another word that is similar or has similar meaning. Once the word is perceived, the brain allows for a deeper processing.

The N400 is a component of time-locked EEG signals known as event-related potentials (ERP). It is a negative-going deflection that peaks around 400 milliseconds post-stimulus onset, although it can extend from 250-500 ms, and is typically maximal over centro-parietal electrode sites. The N400 is part of the normal brain response to words and other meaningful stimuli, including visual and auditory words, sign language signs, pictures, faces, environmental sounds, and smells.

The lexical decision task (LDT) is a procedure used in many psychology and psycholinguistics experiments. The basic procedure involves measuring how quickly people classify stimuli as words or nonwords.

Semantic satiation is a psychological phenomenon in which repetition causes a word or phrase to temporarily lose meaning for the listener, who then perceives the speech as repeated meaningless sounds. Extended inspection or analysis in place of repetition also produces the same effect.

The generation effect is a phenomenon whereby information is better remembered if it is generated from one's own mind rather than simply read. Researchers have struggled to fully explain why generated information is better recalled than read information, as no single explanation has been comprehensive.

Repetition priming refers to improvements in a behavioural response when stimuli are repeatedly presented. The improvements can be measured in terms of accuracy or reaction time and can occur when the repeated stimuli are either identical or similar to previous stimuli. These improvements have been shown to be cumulative, so as the number of repetitions increases the responses get continually faster up to a maximum of around seven repetitions. These improvements are also found when the repeated items are changed slightly in terms of orientation, size and position. The size of the effect is also modulated by the length of time the item is presented for and the length time between the first and subsequent presentations of the repeated items.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Negative priming</span> Initial stimulus inhibits response to subsequent stimulus

Negative priming is an implicit memory effect in which prior exposure to a stimulus unfavorably influences the response to the same stimulus. It falls under the category of priming, which refers to the change in the response towards a stimulus due to a subconscious memory effect. Negative priming describes the slow and error-prone reaction to a stimulus that is previously ignored. For example, a subject may be imagined trying to pick a red pen from a pen holder. The red pen becomes the target of attention, so the subject responds by moving their hand towards it. At this time, they mentally block out all other pens as distractors to aid in closing in on just the red pen. After repeatedly picking the red pen over the others, switching to the blue pen results in a momentary delay picking the pen out. The slow reaction due to the change of the distractor stimulus to target stimulus is called the negative priming effect.

Indirect memory tests assess the retention of information without direct reference to the source of information. Participants are given tasks designed to elicit knowledge that was acquired incidentally or unconsciously and is evident when performance shows greater inclination towards items initially presented than new items. Performance on indirect tests may reflect contributions of implicit memory, the effects of priming, a preference to respond to previously experienced stimuli over novel stimuli. Types of indirect memory tests include the implicit association test, the lexical decision task, the word stem completion task, artificial grammar learning, word fragment completion, and the serial reaction time task.

In psychology, implicit memory is one of the two main types of long-term human memory. It is acquired and used unconsciously, and can affect thoughts and behaviours. One of its most common forms is procedural memory, which allows people to perform certain tasks without conscious awareness of these previous experiences; for example, remembering how to tie one's shoes or ride a bicycle without consciously thinking about those activities.

Task switching, or set-shifting, is an executive function that involves the ability to unconsciously shift attention between one task and another. In contrast, cognitive shifting is a very similar executive function, but it involves conscious change in attention. Together, these two functions are subcategories of the broader cognitive flexibility concept.

Subliminal stimuli are any sensory stimuli below an individual's threshold for conscious perception, in contrast to supraliminal stimuli.

The mental lexicon is a component of the human language faculty that contains information regarding the composition of words, such as their meanings, pronunciations, and syntactic characteristics. The mental lexicon is used in linguistics and psycholinguistics to refer to individual speakers' lexical, or word, representations. However, there is some disagreement as to the utility of the mental lexicon as a scientific construct.

Distributed practice is a learning strategy, where practice is broken up into a number of short sessions over a longer period of time. Humans and other animals learn items in a list more effectively when they are studied in several sessions spread out over a long period of time, rather than studied repeatedly in a short period of time, a phenomenon called the spacing effect. The opposite, massed practice, consists of fewer, longer training sessions and is generally a less effective method of learning. For example, when studying for an exam, dispersing your studying more frequently over a larger period of time will result in more effective learning than intense study the night before.

Bilingual lexical access is an area of psycholinguistics that studies the activation or retrieval process of the mental lexicon for bilingual people.

Anthony Marcel is a British psychologist who contributed to the early debate on the nature of unconscious perceptual processes in the 1970s and 1980s. Marcel argued in favour of an unconscious mind that "…automatically re-describe(s) sensory data into every representational form and to the highest levels of description available to the organism.” Marcel sparked controversy with his claim to have demonstrated unconscious priming. As of 2013 Marcel was working at the University of Hertfordshire and Cambridge University where his research focused on consciousness and phenomenological experience.

In cognitive psychology, intertrial priming is an accumulation of the priming effect over multiple trials, where "priming" is the effect of the exposure to one stimulus on subsequently presented stimuli. Intertrial priming occurs when a target feature is repeated from one trial to the next, and typically results in speeded response times to the target. A target is the stimulus participants are required to search for. For example, intertrial priming occurs when the task is to respond to either a red or a green target, and the response time to a red target is faster if the preceding trial also has a red target.

In psychology, the transposed letter effect is a test of how a word is processed when two letters within the word are switched.

The bi-directional hypothesis of language and action proposes that the sensorimotor and language comprehension areas of the brain exert reciprocal influence over one another. This hypothesis argues that areas of the brain involved in movement and sensation, as well as movement itself, influence cognitive processes such as language comprehension. In addition, the reverse effect is argued, where it is proposed that language comprehension influences movement and sensation. Proponents of the bi-directional hypothesis of language and action conduct and interpret linguistic, cognitive, and movement studies within the framework of embodied cognition and embodied language processing. Embodied language developed from embodied cognition, and proposes that sensorimotor systems are not only involved in the comprehension of language, but that they are necessary for understanding the semantic meaning of words.

References

  1. Weingarten E, Chen Q, McAdams M, Yi J, Hepler J, Albarracín D (May 2016). "From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words". Psychological Bulletin. 142 (5): 472–97. doi:10.1037/bul0000030. PMC   5783538 . PMID   26689090.
  2. Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (2000). "Studying the Mind in the Middle: A Practical Guide to Priming and Automaticity Research". In Reis H, Judd C (eds.). Handbook of Research Methods in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–39.
  3. Shapir OM, Shapir-Tidhar MH, Shtudiner Z (October 2023). "Priming effect across framing, culture, and gender: Evidence from the academia". Managerial and Decision Economics. 44 (7): 3758–3768. doi:10.1002/mde.3918. SSRN   4387810.
  4. Tulving E, Schacter DL, Stark HA (1982). "Priming Effects in Word Fragment Completion are independent of Recognition Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 8 (4): 336–342. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.8.4.336. S2CID   143013790.
  5. Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Chai A, Barndollar K, Trötschel R (December 2001). "The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (6): 1014–27. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1014. PMC   3005626 . PMID   11761304.
  6. Ben-Haim MS, Chajut E, Hassin RR, Algom D (April 2015). "Speeded naming or naming speed? The automatic effect of object speed on performance". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 144 (2): 326–38. doi:10.1037/a0038569. PMID   25559652.
  7. Several researchers, for example, have used cross-modal priming to investigate syntactic deficits in individuals with damage to Broca's area of the brain. See the following:
  8. 1 2 Meyer DE, Schvaneveldt RW (1971). "Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 90 (2): 227–234. doi:10.1037/h0031564. PMID   5134329. S2CID   36672941.
  9. Friederici AD, Steinhauer K, Frisch S (May 1999). "Lexical integration: sequential effects of syntactic and semantic information". Memory & Cognition. 27 (3): 438–53. doi: 10.3758/BF03211539 . PMID   10355234. Semantic priming refers to the finding that word recognition is typically faster when the target word (e.g., doctor) is preceded by a semantically related prime word (e.g., nurse).
  10. 1 2 B. Bower. (2012). The hot and cold of priming: Psychologists are divided on whether unnoticed cues can influence behavior. Science News, 181, The Hot and Cold of Priming Archived September 28, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  11. 1 2 Bower B. "The Hot and Cold of Priming". Science News. Archived from the original on March 13, 2012. Retrieved October 12, 2012.
  12. 1 2 3 Doyen S, Klein O, Pichon CL, Cleeremans A (2012). "Behavioral priming: it's all in the mind, but whose mind?". PLOS ONE. 7 (1): e29081. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...729081D. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029081 . PMC   3261136 . PMID   22279526.
  13. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Mayr S, Buchner A (2007). "Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon". Zeitschrift für Psychologie. 215 (1): 35–51. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.35.
  14. 1 2 3 4 Reisberg, Daniel: Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind (2007), page 255, 517.
  15. Neumann E, DeSchepper BG (November 1991). "Costs and benefits of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 17 (6): 1136–45. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.6.1136. PMID   1838386.
  16. Bentin S, McCarthy G, Wood CC (April 1985). "Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 60 (4): 343–55. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(85)90008-2. PMID   2579801.
  17. Biederman I, Cooper EE (1992). "Size Invariance in Visual Object Priming". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 18 (1): 121–133. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.121.
  18. Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JD, Monti LA, Tinklenberg JR, Yesavage JA (October 1999). "Dissociation between two forms of conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease" (PDF). Neuropsychology. 13 (4): 516–24. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.516. PMID   10527059. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 19, 2011.
  19. Forster KI, Davis C (1984). "Repetition Priming and Frequency Attenuation". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 10 (4): 680–698. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.680.
  20. James, Thomas W.; Gauthier, Isabel (January 2006). "Repetition-induced changes in BOLD response reflect accumulation of neural activity". Human Brain Mapping. 27 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1002/hbm.20165. ISSN   1065-9471. PMC   6871272 . PMID   15954142.
  21. Ludovic Ferrand and Boris New: Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon, found on: boris.new.googlepages.com/Semantic-final-2003.pdf
  22. Marslen-Wilson W, Tyler LK, Waksler R, Older L (1994). "Morphology and Meaning in the English Mental Lexicon". Psychological Review. 101 (1): 3–33. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.294.7035 . doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.3.
  23. Foss, Donald (April 23, 1982). "A discourse on semantic priming". Cognitive Psychology. 14 (4): 590–607. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(82)90020-2. PMID   7140212. S2CID   42931448.
  24. Matsukawa J, Snodgrass JG, Doniger GM (November 2005). "Conceptual versus perceptual priming in incomplete picture identification". Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 34 (6): 515–40. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-9162-5. hdl: 2297/1842 . PMID   16341912. S2CID   18396648.
  25. 1 2 Stanovich KE, West RF (March 1983). "On priming by a sentence context". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 112 (1): 1–36. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.112.1.1. PMID   6221061.
  26. Klotz W, Wolff P (1995). "The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus". Psychological Research. 58 (2): 92–101. doi:10.1007/bf00571098. PMID   7480512. S2CID   25422424.
  27. Klotz W, Neumann O (1999). "Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 25 (4): 976–992. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.976.
  28. Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 2003, p. 6275-6280.
  29. Schmidt, T., & Vorberg, D. Criteria for unconscious cognition: Three types of dissociation. In: Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 2006, p. 489-504.
  30. Forster KI, Davis C (1984). "Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical decision". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 10 (4): 680–698. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.680.
  31. Forster KI, Davis C (1984). "The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime". Journal of Memory and Language. 30 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90008-8.
  32. Forster KI, Davis C, Schoknecht C, Carter R (1987). "Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation?". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A. 39A (2): 211–251. doi:10.1080/14640748708401785. S2CID   144992122.
  33. Segui, Juan; Grainger, Jonathan (1990). "APA PsycNet". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 16 (1): 65–76. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.65. S2CID   27807746 . Retrieved September 30, 2021.
  34. Davis, C. J.; Lupker, S. J. (2006). "APA PsycNet". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 32 (3): 668–687. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.668. PMID   16822131 . Retrieved September 30, 2021.
  35. Andrews, S.; Hersch, J. (2010). "APA PsycNet". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 139 (2): 299–318. doi:10.1037/a0018366. PMID   20438253 . Retrieved September 30, 2021.
  36. Andrews, S.; Lo, S. (2012). "APA PsycNet". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 38 (1): 152–163. doi:10.1037/a0024953. PMID   21875252 . Retrieved September 30, 2021.
  37. Perfetti, Charles (September 26, 2007). "Reading Ability: Lexical Quality to Comprehension". Scientific Studies of Reading. 11 (4): 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730. ISSN   1088-8438. S2CID   62203167.
  38. Adelman, James S.; Johnson, Rebecca L.; McCormick, Samantha F.; McKague, Meredith; Kinoshita, Sachiko; Bowers, Jeffrey S.; Perry, Jason R.; Lupker, Stephen J.; Forster, Kenneth I.; Cortese, Michael J.; Scaltritti, Michele (December 1, 2014). "A behavioral database for masked form priming". Behavior Research Methods. 46 (4): 1052–1067. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y. hdl: 10026.1/10800 . ISSN   1554-3528. PMID   24488815. S2CID   935533.
  39. Elsherif, M.M; Wheeldon, L.R.; Frisson, S. (September 1, 2021). "Phonological precision for word recognition in skilled readers". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 75 (6): 1021–1040. doi:10.1177/17470218211046350. ISSN   1747-0218. PMC   9016675 . PMID   34467802.
  40. Elsherif, M. M.; Frisson, S.; Wheeldon, L. R. (August 25, 2022). "Orthographic precision for word naming in skilled readers". Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. 38 (2): 197–216. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2022.2108091 . hdl: 11250/3068156 . ISSN   2327-3798.
  41. Carlson, Charlin, Norman, http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/55/2/211/, Positive mood and helping behavior: A test of six hypotheses, 1989
  42. Yao, Zhao; Zhu, Xiangru; Luo, Wenbo (October 1, 2019). "Valence makes a stronger contribution than arousal to affective priming". PeerJ. 7: e7777. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7777 . ISSN   2167-8359. PMC   6777477 . PMID   31592186.
  43. Rotteveel, M.; de Groot, P.; Geutskens, A.; Phaf, R. H. (December 2001). "Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming?". Emotion. 1 (4): 348–364. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.4.348. ISSN   1528-3542. PMID   12901397.
  44. Kosonogov, Vladimir (December 30, 2020). "The effects of the order of picture presentation on the subjective emotional evaluation of pictures". Psicologia. 34 (2): 171–178. doi: 10.17575/psicologia.v34i2.1608 . ISSN   2183-2471.
  45. 1 2 Kitayama S, Cohen D (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN   9781593857325. OCLC   560675525.
  46. Liu Z, Cheng M, Peng K, Zhang D (September 30, 2015). "Self-construal priming selectively modulates the scope of visual attention". Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 1508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01508 . PMC   4588108 . PMID   26483747.
  47. Hong YY, Chiu CY, Kung TM (1997). "Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition". Progress in Asian Social Psychology. 1: 135–46.
  48. Myers DG (2010). Social psychology (Tenth ed.). New York, NY. ISBN   9780073370668. OCLC   667213323.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  49. Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET (2007). Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. Guilford Press. ISBN   9781572309180.
  50. Hong YY, Morris MW, Chiu CY, Benet-Martínez V (July 2000). "Multicultural minds. A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition". The American Psychologist. 55 (7): 709–20. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.55.7.709. PMID   10916861.
  51. Oyserman D, Sorensen N, Reber R, Chen SX (August 2009). "Connecting and separating mind-sets: culture as situated cognition". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 97 (2): 217–35. doi:10.1037/a0015850. hdl: 10397/15377 . PMID   19634972.
  52. Keith KD (April 12, 2018). Culture across the curriculum : a psychology teacher's handbook. New York. ISBN   9781107189973. OCLC   1005687090.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  53. Marsolek, Chad J. (May 2008). "What antipriming reveals about priming". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12 (5): 176–181. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.005. ISSN   1364-6613. PMID   18403251. S2CID   44554182.
  54. Zhang, Feng; Fairchild, Amanda J.; Li, Xiaoming (2017). "Visual Antipriming Effect: Evidence from Chinese Character Identification". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1791. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01791 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   5651016 . PMID   29089908.
  55. 1 2 Humphries, Ailsa; Chen, Zhe; Wiltshire, Jonathan (March 2020). "Repetition priming with no antipriming in picture identification". Vision Research. 168: 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2019.09.011 . ISSN   1878-5646. PMID   32044587. S2CID   211054740.
  56. Pashler H, Coburn N, Harris CR (2012). "Priming of social distance? Failure to replicate effects on social and food judgments". PLOS ONE. 7 (8): e42510. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...742510P. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042510 . PMC   3430642 . PMID   22952597.
  57. PsychFileDrawer, (2013). Replicability of Social and goal priming findings.
  58. Rajain P (September 2016). "Sensory Marketing Aspects: Priming, Expectations, Crossmodal Correspondences & More". Vikalpa. 41 (3): 264–6. doi: 10.1177/0256090916652045 .
  59. Meyer DE (August 2014). "Semantic priming well established". Science. 345 (6196): 523. Bibcode:2014Sci...345..523M. doi:10.1126/science.345.6196.523-b. PMID   25082691.
  60. Yong E. "Replication studies: Bad copy". Nature. Retrieved October 12, 2012.
  61. Kahneman D. "A proposal to deal with questions about priming effects" (PDF). Nature. Retrieved October 12, 2012.
  62. "Adversarial Collaboration: An EDGE Lecture by Daniel Kahneman | Edge.org". www.edge.org. Retrieved February 27, 2022.
  63. Yong, Ed (March 10, 2012). "A failed replication draws a scathing personal attack from a psychology professor". Discover Magazine. Archived from the original on March 13, 2012. Retrieved October 12, 2012.
  64. Schvaneveldt RW, Meyer DE (1973). "Retrieval and comparison processes in semantic memory". In Kornblum S (ed.). Attention and performance IV. New York: Academic Press. pp. 395–409.
  65. 1 2 Groves PS, Bunch JL, Cram E, Farag A, Manges K, Perkhounkova Y, Scott-Cawiezell J (November 2017). "Priming Patient Safety Through Nursing Handoff Communication: A Simulation Pilot Study". Western Journal of Nursing Research. 39 (11): 1394–1411. doi:10.1177/0193945916673358. PMID   28322631. S2CID   32696412.
  66. Cermak LS, Talbot N, Chandler K, Wolbarst LR (1985). "The perceptual priming phenomenon in amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 23 (5): 615–22. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90063-6. PMID   4058707. S2CID   30545942.
  67. Shimamura AP, Squire LR (1984). "Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: a neuropsychological approach". J. Exp. Psychol. 113 (4): 556–570. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.554.414 . doi:10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.556. PMID   6240522.
  68. Blaxton TA (September 1992). "Dissociations among memory measures in memory-impaired subjects: evidence for a processing account of memory". Memory & Cognition. 20 (5): 549–62. doi: 10.3758/BF03199587 . PMID   1453972.
  69. Graf P, Squire LR, Mandler G (January 1984). "The information that amnesic patients do not forget". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 10 (1): 164–78. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.164. PMID   6242734.
  70. "Aphasia". The British Medical Journal. 2 (296): 258–261. 1866. ISSN   0007-1447. JSTOR   25205881.
  71. Carlesimo GA, Oscar-Berman M (June 1992). "Memory deficits in Alzheimer's patients: a comprehensive review". Neuropsychology Review. 3 (2): 119–69. doi:10.1007/BF01108841. PMID   1300219. S2CID   19548915.
  72. Schacter DL, McGlynn SM, Milberg WP, Church BA (1993). "Spared priming despite impaired comprehension: implicit memory in a case of word meaning deafness". Neuropsychology. 7 (2): 107–118. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.7.2.107. S2CID   3813751.
  73. Geva A, Moscovitch M, Leach L (April 1997). "Perceptual priming of proper names in young and older normal adults and a patient with prosopanomia". Neuropsychology. 11 (2): 232–42. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.11.2.232. PMID   9110330.
  74. Li L, Miller EK, Desimone R (June 1993). "The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology. 69 (6): 1918–29. doi:10.1152/jn.1993.69.6.1918. PMID   8350131. S2CID   6482631.
  75. Gruber T, Müller MM (May 2002). "Effects of picture repetition on induced gamma band responses, evoked potentials, and phase synchrony in the human EEG". Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 13 (3): 377–92. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00130-6. PMID   11919002.
  76. Squire LR, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Videen TO, Raichle ME (March 1992). "Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 89 (5): 1837–41. Bibcode:1992PNAS...89.1837S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.5.1837 . PMC   48548 . PMID   1542680.
  77. Wig GS, Grafton ST, Demos KE, Kelley WM (September 2005). "Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming". Nature Neuroscience. 8 (9): 1228–33. doi:10.1038/nn1515. PMID   16056222. S2CID   470957.
  78. Moldakarimov S, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski TJ (March 2010). "Perceptual priming leads to reduction of gamma frequency oscillations". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (12): 5640–5. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.5640M. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907525107 . PMC   2851786 . PMID   20212165.
  79. Demb JB, Desmond JE, Wagner AD, Vaidya CJ, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (September 1995). "Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity". The Journal of Neuroscience. 15 (9): 5870–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-05870.1995. PMC   6577672 . PMID   7666172.
  80. Gabrieli JD, Poldrack RA, Desmond JE (February 1998). "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 95 (3): 906–13. Bibcode:1998PNAS...95..906G. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.906 . PMC   33815 . PMID   9448258.
  81. Dehaene S, Naccache L, Le Clec'H G, Koechlin E, Mueller M, Dehaene-Lambertz G, van de Moortele PF, Le Bihan D (October 1998). "Imaging unconscious semantic priming" (PDF). Nature. 395 (6702): 597–600. Bibcode:1998Natur.395..597D. doi:10.1038/26967. PMID   9783584. S2CID   4373890. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 6, 2011.
  82. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Bargh JA, Chen M, Burrows L (August 1996). "Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (2): 230–44. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.318.3898 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230. PMID   8765481. S2CID   6654763.
  83. (see Ironic process theory for a more in-depth discussion of this phenomenon)
  84. Bargh JA, Williams EL (February 2006). "The Automaticity of Social Life". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (1): 1–4. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00395.x. PMC   2435044 . PMID   18568084.
  85. Williams LE, Bargh JA (October 2008). "Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth". Science. 322 (5901): 606–7. Bibcode:2008Sci...322..606W. doi:10.1126/science.1162548. PMC   2737341 . PMID   18948544.
  86. Cox WT, Abramson LY, Devine PG, Hollon SD (September 2012). "Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression: The Integrated Perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (5): 427–49. doi:10.1177/1745691612455204. PMID   26168502. S2CID   1512121.