Self-concept

Last updated

In the psychology of self, one's self-concept (also called self-construction, self-identity, self-perspective or self-structure) is a collection of beliefs about oneself. [1] [2] Generally, self-concept embodies the answer to the question "Who am I?". [3]

Contents

The self-concept is distinguishable from self-awareness, which is the extent to which self-knowledge is defined, consistent, and currently applicable to one's attitudes and dispositions. [4] Self-concept also differs from self-esteem: self-concept is a cognitive or descriptive component of one's self (e.g. "I am a fast runner"), while self-esteem is evaluative and opinionated (e.g. "I feel good about being a fast runner").

Self-concept is made up of one's self-schemas, and interacts with self-esteem, self-knowledge, and the social self to form the self as a whole. It includes the past, present, and future selves, where future selves (or possible selves) represent individuals' ideas of what they might become, what they would like to become, or what they are afraid of becoming. Possible selves may function as incentives for certain behaviour. [3] [5]

The perception people have about their past or future selves relates to their perception of their current selves. The temporal self-appraisal theory [6] argues that people have a tendency to maintain a positive self-evaluation by distancing themselves from their negative self and paying more attention to their positive one. In addition, people have a tendency to perceive the past self less favourably [7] (e.g. "I'm better than I used to be") and the future self more positively [8] (e.g. "I will be better than I am now").

History

Psychologists Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow had major influence in popularizing the idea of self-concept in the West. According to Rogers, everyone strives to reach an "ideal self." He believed that a person gets to self-actualize when they prove to themself that they are capable enough to achieve their goals and desires, but in order to attain their fullest potential, the person must have been raised in healthy surroundings which consist of "genuineness, acceptance, and empathy", however, the lack of relationships with people that have healthy personalities will stop the person from growing "like a tree without sunlight and water" and affect the individual's process to accomplish self- actualization. [9] Rogers also hypothesized that psychologically healthy people actively move away from roles created by others' expectations, and instead look within themselves for validation. On the other hand, neurotic people have "self-concepts that do not match their experiences. They are afraid to accept their own experiences as valid, so they distort them, either to protect themselves or to win approval from others." [10]

According to Carl Rogers, the self-concept has three different components: [11] [12]

Abraham Maslow applied his concept of self-actualization in his hierarchy of needs theory. In this theory, he explained the process it takes for a person to achieve self-actualization. He argues that for an individual to get to the "higher level growth needs", he must first accomplish "lower deficit needs". Once the "deficiency needs" have been achieved, the person's goal is to accomplish the next step, which is the "being needs". Maslow noticed that once individuals reach this level, they tend to "grow as a person" and reach self-actualization. However, individuals who experienced negative events while being in the lower deficit needs level prevents them from ascending in the hierarchy of needs. [13]

The self-categorization theory developed by John Turner states that the self-concept consists of at least two "levels": a personal identity and a social one. In other words, one's self-evaluation relies on self-perceptions and how others perceive them. Self-concept can alternate rapidly between one's personal and social identity. [14] Children and adolescents begin integrating social identity into their own self-concept in elementary school by assessing their position among peers. [15] By age five, acceptance from peers significantly affects children's self-concept, affecting their behaviour and academic success. [16]

Model

One's self-perception is defined by one's self-concept, self-knowledge, self-esteem, and social self. The constituent on one's self.png
One's self-perception is defined by one's self-concept, self-knowledge, self-esteem, and social self.

The self-concept is an internal model that uses self-assessments in order to define one's self-schemas. [17] Changes in self-concept can be measured by spontaneous self-report, where a person is prompted by a question like "Who are you?". [18] Often when measuring changes to the self-evaluation, whether a person has a positive or negative opinion of oneself, is measured instead of self-concept. [18]

Features such as personality, skills and abilities, occupation and hobbies, physical characteristics, gender, etc. are assessed and applied to self-schemas, which are ideas of oneself in a particular dimension (e.g., someone that considers themselves a geek will associate "geek-like" qualities to themselves). A collection of self-schemas makes up one's overall self-concept. For example, the statement "I am lazy" is a self-assessment that contributes to self-concept. Statements such as "I am tired", however, would not be part of someone's self-concept, since being tired is a temporary state and therefore cannot become a part of a self-schema. A person's self-concept may change with time as reassessment occurs, which in extreme cases can lead to identity crises.

Parts

Various theories identify different parts of the self include:

Development

Researchers debate over when self-concept development begins. Some assert that gender stereotypes and expectations set by parents for their children affect children's understanding of themselves by approximately age three. [21] However, at this developmental stage, children have a very broad sense of self; typically, they use words such as big or nice to describe themselves to others. [22] While this represents the beginnings of self-concept, others suggest that self-concept develops later, in middle childhood, alongside the development of self-control. [23] At this point, children are developmentally prepared to interpret their own feelings and abilities, as well as receive and consider feedback from peers, teachers, and family. [1] In adolescence, the self-concept undergoes a significant time of change. Generally, self-concept changes more gradually, and instead, existing concepts are refined and solidified. [24] However, the development of self-concept during adolescence shows a U-shaped curve, in which general self-concept decreases in early adolescence, followed by an increase in later adolescence. [25]

Romantic relationships can affect people's self-concept throughout a relationship. [26] :89 Self-expansion describes the addition of information to an individual's concept of self. [27] :30 Self-expansion can occur during relationships. Expansion of self-concept can occur during relationships, during new challenging experiences. [27]

Additionally, teens begin to evaluate their abilities on a continuum, as opposed to the "yes/no" evaluation of children. For example, while children might evaluate themselves "smart", teens might evaluate themselves as "not the smartest, but smarter than average." [28] Despite differing opinions about the onset of self-concept development, researchers agree on the importance of one's self-concept, which influences people's behaviors and cognitive and emotional outcomes including (but not limited to) academic achievement, levels of happiness, anxiety, social integration, self-esteem, and life-satisfaction. [15] [29] [30] [31]

Academic

Academic self-concept refers to the personal beliefs about their academic abilities or skills. [15] Some research suggests that it begins developing from ages three to five due to influence from parents and early educators. [21] By age ten or eleven, children assess their academic abilities by comparing themselves to their peers. [32] These social comparisons are also referred to as self-estimates. [33] Self-estimates of cognitive ability are most accurate when evaluating subjects that deal with numbers, such as math. [33] Self-estimates were more likely to be poor in other areas, such as reasoning speed. [33] [ clarification needed ]

Some researchers suggest that to raise academic self-concept, parents and teachers need to provide children with specific feedback that focuses on their particular skills or abilities. [34] Others also state that learning opportunities should be conducted in groups (both mixed-ability and like-ability) that downplay social comparison, as too much of either type of grouping can have adverse effects on children's academic self-concept and the way they view themselves in relation to their peers. [15] [29]

Physical

Physical self-concept is the individual's perception of themselves in areas of physical ability and appearance. Physical ability includes concepts such as physical strength and endurance, while appearance refers to attractiveness and body image. [35] Adolescents experience significant changes in general physical self-concept at the onset of puberty, about eleven years old for girls and about 15 years old for boys. The bodily changes during puberty, in conjunction with the various psychological changes of this period, makes adolescence especially significant for the development of physical self-concept. [36] An important factor of physical self-concept development is participation in physical activities. It has even been suggested that adolescent involvement in competitive sports increases physical self-concept. [37]

Gender identity

A person's gender identity is a sense of one's own gender. These ideas typically form in young children. [38] [39] According to the International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family, gender identity is developed at an early age when the child starts to communicate; by the age of eighteen months to two years is when the child begins to identify as a girl or a boy. [40] After this stage, some consider gender identity already formed, although some consider non-gendered identities more salient during that young of an age. Kohlberg noted gender constancy occurs by the ages of five to six, a child becomes well-aware of their gender identity. [41] [39] Both biological and social factors may influence identities such as a sense of individuality, identities of place as well as gendered identities. As part of environmental attitudes, some suggest women more than men care about the environment. [42] Forms of gender stereotyping is also important to consider in clinical settings. For example, a study at Kuwait University with a small sample of 102 individuals with gender dysphoria examined self-concept, masculinity and femininity. [43] Findings were that children who grew up on lower family bonds had lower self-concept. [43] Clearly, it is important to consider the context of social and political attitudes and beliefs before drawing any conclusions about gender identities in relation to personality, particularly about mental health and issues around acceptable behaviours. [43]

Measures

Since self-concept concerns one's awareness and perception of the external judgment from their social environment and the internal attributes of the self, self-reported questionnaires have been the most widely adopted measure for assessing self-concept. [44] Many of the existing questionnaires in the academia do not differentiate between the assessment of self-concept and self-esteem. The most commonly used questionnaires include the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory for Adults, the Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS) by Frydenberg and Lewis, as well as the Harter’s Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. [45]

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES)

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale is a two-dimensional scale developed in 1965 by Morris Rosenberg, originally designed for measuring the self-esteem of adolescents, but has gained wider use in other populations, such as adults. This inventory comprises ten Likert scale items that assess one's level of self-confidence (or personal satisfaction) and self-contempt (or personal devaluation). [45] With each item scaling from 1 to 4 points, a total of 30 points or above indicates a positive self-esteem/self-concept, 26-29 points indicating an intermediate self-esteem/self-concept, and those who get 25 points or below will be considered as having low self-esteem/self-concept.

The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (CSEI)

The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory is a self-report questionnaire developed in 1981 to measure attitudes toward the self among children, adolescents and adults. [46] The inventory comes in three forms: School Form (ages 8-15 years), Adult Form (ages 16 and above) and Short Form. Originally, the inventory was aimed at children in school (8-15 years) but later on, a revised version where 17 of the 58 items were rephrased to use with adults. The most commonly used version is the Adult Form. The inventory comprises 50 items and yields a total score alongside four separate scores which represent more specific aspects of self-esteem. [46] These aspects include: general self, social self-peers, home parents and school academic. With each item scored using a dichotomous scale, scores rage from 0 to 50, with higher scores reflecting higher self-esteem. [47]

The Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS)

The Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS) is an 80-item questionnaire developed in 1997 by Erica Frydenberg and Ramon Lewis. [48] The aim of the questionnaire is to measure adolescent coping strategies on a 5-point Likert scale. The ACS uses a self-report approach to subcategorize 79 items into 18 common coping mechanisms, which are then grouped into three broad coping styles: coping in relation to others, productive coping, and non-productive coping. [48] Examples of coping in relation to others include seeking professional help, social action, and social and spiritual support. Productive coping behaviours include focusing on the positive, finding solutions to the problem, and physical recreation, while non-productive mechanisms involve worrying, self blame, and wishful thinking. [48] [49] The final item, question 80, is an open-ended question that qualitatively assesses other mechanisms adolescents use to cope during difficult circumstances. The Adolescent Coping Scale gives respondents insight into their self concept by giving them an opportunity to engage in self-assessment and reflect on their behaviours and how they respond to a given situation. [49]

Harter's Self Perception Profile for Adolescents

The Self Perception Profile for Adolescents was developed by Susan Harter in 1988, comprising of nine sub-scales that each has five items. The measure assesses adolescents' perception of the self from nine aspects of their life, as listed below: physical appearance, romantic appeal, close friendship, social competence, behavioural conduct, scholastic competence, job competence, athletic competence, global self-worth. [45] During the administration of the test, adolescents will choose one sub-scale and indicate their perceived competence in the particular aspect. Each item in the sub-scale scores from 1 (low perceived competence) to 4 (high perceived competence).

Motivational properties

Self-concept can have motivational properties. There are four types of motives in particular that are most related to self-concept: [50]

Some of these motives may be more prominent depending on the situation. In Western societies, the most automatic is the self-enhancement motive, and may be dominant in some situations where motives contradict one another. [50] For example, the self-enhancement motive may contradict and dominate the self-assessment motive if one seeks out inaccurate compliments rather than honest feedback. Additionally, self-concept can motivate behavior because people tend to act in ways that reaffirm their self-concept, [51] which is consistent with the idea of the self-verification motive. In particular, if people perceive the self a certain way and receive feedback contrary to this perception, a tension is produced that motivates them to reestablish consistency between environmental feedback and self-concept. [51] For example, if someone believes herself to be outgoing, but someone tells her she is shy, she may be motivated to avoid that person or the environment in which she met that person because it is inconsistent with her self-concept of being an outgoing person. Further, another major motivational property of self-concept comes from the desire to eliminate the discrepancy between one's current self-concept and his or her ideal possible self. [51] This is parallel with the idea of the self-improvement motive. For example, if one's current self-concept is that she is a novice at piano playing, though she wants to become a concert pianist, this discrepancy will generate motivation to engage in behaviors (like practicing playing piano) that will bring her closer to her ideal possible self (being a concert pianist).

Cultural differences

Worldviews about one's self in relation to others differ across and within cultures. [52] Western cultures place particular importance on personal independence and on the expression of one's own attributes [53] (i.e. the self is more important than the group). This is not to say those in an independent culture do not identify and support their society or culture, there is simply a different type of relationship. [54] Non-Western cultures[ which? ] favor an interdependent view of the self: [52] Interpersonal relationships are more important than one's individual accomplishments, and individuals experience a sense of oneness with the group. [52] Such identity fusion can have positive and negative consequences. [52] Identity fusion can give people the sense that their existence is meaningful provided the person feels included within the society (for example, in Japan, the definition of the word for self (jibun) roughly translates to "one's share of the shared life space"). [54] Identity fusion can also harm one's self-concept because one's behaviors and thoughts must be able to change to continue to align with those of the overall group. [54] Non-interdependent self-concepts can also differ between cultural traditions. [55]

Additionally, one's social norms and cultural identities have a large effect on self-concept and mental well-being. [54] When a person can clearly define their culture's norms and how those play a part in their life, that person is more likely to have a positive self-identity, leading to better self-concept and psychological welfare. [54] One example of this is in regards to consistency. One of the social norms within a Western, independent culture is consistency, which allows each person to maintain their self-concept over time. [56] The social norm in a non-Western, interdependent culture[ which? ] has a larger focus on one's ability to be flexible and to change as the group and environment change. [56] If this social norm is not followed in either culture, this can lead to a disconnection with one's social identity, which affects personality, behavior, and overall self-concept. [54] Buddhists emphasize the impermanence of any self-concept. [57]

Anit Somech, an organizational psychologist and professor, who carried a small study in Israel showed that the divide between independent and interdependent self-concepts exists within cultures as well. Researchers compared mid-level merchants in an urban community with those in a kibbutz (collective community). [58] The managers from the urban community followed the independent culture. When asked to describe themselves, they primarily used descriptions of their own personal traits without comparison to others within their group. [58] When the independent, urban managers gave interdependent-type responses, most were focused on work or school, due to these being the two biggest groups identified within an independent culture. [58] The kibbutz managers followed the interdependent culture. They used hobbies and preferences to describe their traits, which is more frequently seen in interdependent cultures as these serve as a means of comparison with others in their society. There was also a large focus on residence, lending to the fact they share resources and living space with the others from the kibbutz. These types of differences were also seen in a study done with Swedish and Japanese adolescents. [59] Typically, these would both be considered[ by whom? ] non-Western cultures, but the Swedish showed more independent traits, while the Japanese followed the expected interdependent traits. [58]

Along with viewing one's identity as part of a group, another factor that coincides with self-concept is stereotype threat. Many working names have been used for this term: stigmatization, stigma pressure, stigma vulnerability and stereotype vulnerability. The terminology that was settled upon Claude Steele and Joshua Aronson to describe this "situational predicament was 'stereotype threat.' This term captures the idea of a situational predicament as a contingency of their [marginalized] group identity, a real threat of judgment or treatment in the person's environment that went beyond any limitations within." [60] Steele and Aronson described the idea of stereotype threat in their study of how this socio‐psychological notion affected the intellectual performance of African Americans. [61] Steele and Aronson tested a hypothesis by administering a diagnostic exam between two different groups: African American and White students. For one group a stereotype threat was introduced while the other served as a control. The findings were that academic performance of the African American students was significantly lower than their White counterparts when a stereotype threat was perceived after controlling for intellectual ability. Since the inception by Steele and Aronson of stereotype threat, other research has demonstrated the applicability of this idea to other groups. [61]

When one's actions could negatively influence general assumptions of a stereotype, those actions are consciously emphasized. Instead of one's individual characteristics, one's categorization into a social group is what society views objectively – which could be perceived as a negative stereotype, thus creating a threat. "The notion that stereotypes held about a particular group may create psychologically threatening situations associated with fears of confirming judgment about one's group, and in turn, inhibit learning and performance." [62]

The presence of stereotype threat perpetuates a "hidden curriculum" that further marginalized minority groups. Hidden curriculum refers to a covert expression of prejudice where one standard is accepted as the "set and right way to do things". More specifically, the hidden curriculum is an unintended transmission of social constructs that operate in the social environment of an educational setting or classroom. In the United States' educational system, this caters to dominant culture groups in American society. [63] "A primary source of stereotyping is often the teachers education program itself. It is in these programs that teachers learn that poor students and students of color should be expected to achieve less than their 'mainstream' counterparts." [64] These child-deficit assumptions that are built into the program that instructs teachers and lead to inadvertently testing all students on a "mainstream" standard that is not necessarily academic and that does not account for the social values and norms of non-"mainstream" students.[ clarification needed ]

For example, the model of "teacher as the formal authority" is the orthodox teaching role that has been perpetuated[ by whom? ] for many years until the 21st-century teaching model landed on the scene. As part of the 5 main teaching style proposed by Anthony Grasha, a cognitive and social psychologist until his death in 2003, the authoritarian style is described as believing that there are "correct, acceptable, and standard ways to do things". [65] [ relevant? ]

Gender issues

Some say, girls tend to prefer one-on-one (dyadic) interaction, forming tight, intimate bonds, while boys prefer group activities. [66] One study in particular found that boys performed almost twice as well in groups than in pairs, whereas girls did not show such a difference. [67] In early adolescence, the variations in physical self-concepts appear slightly stronger for boys than girls. This includes self-concepts about movement, body, appearance and other physical attributes. Yet during periods of physical change such as infancy, adolescence and ageing, it is particularly useful to compare these self-concepts with measured skills before drawing broad conclusions [68]

Some studies suggest self-concept of social behaviours are substantially similar with specific variations for girls and boys. For instance, girls are more likely than boys to wait their turn to speak, agree with others, and acknowledge the contributions of others. It seems boys see themselves as building larger group relationships based on shared interests, threaten, boast, and call names. [66] In mixed-sex pairs of children aged 33 months, girls were more likely to passively watch a boy play, and boys were more likely to be unresponsive to what the girls were saying. [69] In some cultures, such stereotypical traits are sustained from childhood to adulthood suggesting a strong influence of expectations by other people in these cultures. [66] The key impacts of social self-concepts on social behaviours and of social behaviours on social self-concepts is a vital area of ongoing research.

In contrast, research suggest overall similarities for gender groups in self-concepts about academic work. In general, any variations are systematically gender-based yet small in terms of effect sizes. Any variations suggest overall academic self-concept are slightly stronger for men than women in mathematics, science and technology and slightly stronger for women than men about language related skills. It is important to observe there is no link between self concepts and skills [i.e., correlations about r = 0.19 are rather weak if statistically significant with large samples]. Clearly, even small variations in perceived self-concepts tend to reflect gender stereotypes evident in some cultures . [70] In recent years, more women have been entering into the STEM field, working in predominantly mathematics, technology and science related careers. Many factors play a role in variations in gender effects on self-concept to accumulate as attitudes to mathematics and science; in particular, the impact other people's expectations rather than role-models on our self-concepts . [71]

Media

A commonly-asked question is "why do people choose one form of media over another?" According to the Galileo Model, there are different forms of media spread throughout three-dimensional space. [72] The closer one form of media is to another the more similar the source of media is to each other. The farther away from each form of media is in space, the least similar the source of media is. For example, mobile and cell phone are located closest in space where as newspaper and texting are farthest apart in space. The study further explained the relationship between self-concept and the use of different forms of media. The more hours per day an individual uses a form of media, the closer that form of media is to their self-concept.

Self-concept is related to the form of media most used. [72] If one considers oneself tech savvy, then one will use mobile phones more often than one would use a newspaper. If one considers oneself old fashioned, then one will use a magazine more often than one would instant message.

In this day and age, social media is where people experience most of their communication. With developing a sense of self on a psychological level, feeling as part of a greater body such as social, emotional, political bodies can affect how one feels about themselves. [73] If a person is included or excluded from a group, that can affect how they form their identities. [74] Growing social media is a place for not only expressing an already formed identity, but to explore and experiment with developing identities. In the United Kingdom, a study about changing identities revealed that some people believe that partaking in online social media is the first time they have felt like themselves, and they have achieved their true identities. They also revealed that these online identities transferred to their offline identities. [74]

A 2007 study was done on adolescents aged 12 to 18 to view the ways in which social media affects the formation of an identity. The study found that it affected the formation in three different ways: risk taking, communication of personal views, and perceptions of influences. [75] In this particular study, risk taking behavior was engaging with strangers. When it came to communication about personal views, half of the participants reported that it was easier to express these opinions online, because they felt an enhanced ability to be creative and meaningful. When it came to other's opinions, one subject reported finding out more about themselves, like openness to experience, because of receiving differing opinions on things such as relationships. [75]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Adolescence</span> Human transition from puberty to adult

Adolescence is a transitional stage of physical and psychological development that generally occurs during the period from puberty to adulthood. Adolescence is usually associated with the teenage years, but its physical, psychological or cultural expressions may begin earlier or end later. Puberty typically begins during preadolescence, particularly in females. Physical growth and cognitive development can extend past the teens. Age provides only a rough marker of adolescence, and scholars have not agreed upon a precise definition. Some definitions start as early as 10 and end as late as 30. The World Health Organization definition officially designates an adolescent as someone between the ages of 10 and 19.

Self-esteem is confidence in one's own worth, abilities, or morals. Self-esteem encompasses beliefs about oneself as well as emotional states, such as triumph, despair, pride, and shame. Smith and Mackie define it by saying "The self-concept is what we think about the self; self-esteem, is the positive or negative evaluations of the self, as in how we feel about it ."

In social psychology, an interpersonal relation describes a social association, connection, or affiliation between two or more persons. It overlaps significantly with the concept of social relations, which are the fundamental unit of analysis within the social sciences. Relations vary in degrees of intimacy, self-disclosure, duration, reciprocity, and power distribution. The main themes or trends of the interpersonal relations are: family, kinship, friendship, love, marriage, business, employment, clubs, neighborhoods, ethical values, support and solidarity. Interpersonal relations may be regulated by law, custom, or mutual agreement, and form the basis of social groups and societies. They appear when people communicate or act with each other within specific social contexts, and they thrive on equitable and reciprocal compromises.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Peer group</span> Primary group of people with similar interests, age, background, or social status

In sociology, a peer group is both a social group and a primary group of people who have similar interests (homophily), age, background, or social status. The members of this group are likely to influence the person's beliefs and behaviour.

Relational aggression, alternative aggression, or relational bullying is a type of aggression in which harm is caused by damaging someone's relationships or social status.

Confidence is the feeling of belief or trust that a person or thing is reliable. Self-confidence is trust in oneself. Self-confidence involves a positive belief that one can generally accomplish what one wishes to do in the future. Self-confidence is not the same as self-esteem, which is an evaluation of one's worth. Self-confidence is related to self-efficacy—belief in one's ability to accomplish a specific task or goal. Confidence can be a self-fulfilling prophecy, as those without it may fail because they lack it, and those with it may succeed because they have it rather than because of an innate ability or skill.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Looking-glass self</span> Sociological term

The term looking-glass self was created by American sociologist Charles Horton Cooley in 1902, and introduced into his work Human Nature and the Social Order. It is described as our reflection of how we think we appear to others. Essentially, people use others as a mirror to observe themselves, which is accomplished by imagining how one appears to others, then internalizing those imagined images. This can lead to one's self-evaluations becoming consistent with how other people perceive them.

Identity formation, also called identity development or identity construction, is a complex process in which humans develop a clear and unique view of themselves and of their identity.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Belongingness</span> Human emotional need

Belongingness is the human emotional need to be an accepted member of a group. Whether it is family, friends, co-workers, a religion, or something else, some people tend to have an 'inherent' desire to belong and be an important part of something greater than themselves. This implies a relationship that is greater than simple acquaintance or familiarity.

Self-enhancement is a type of motivation that works to make people feel good about themselves and to maintain self-esteem. This motive becomes especially prominent in situations of threat, failure or blows to one's self-esteem. Self-enhancement involves a preference for positive over negative self-views. It is one of the three self-evaluation motives along with self-assessment and self-verification . Self-evaluation motives drive the process of self-regulation, that is, how people control and direct their own actions.

Laurie A. Rudman is a social psychology feminist professor as well as the Director of the Rutgers University Social Cognition Laboratory who has contributed a great deal of research to studies on implicit and explicit attitudes and stereotypes, stereotype maintenance processes, and the media's effects on attitudes, stereotypes, and behavior on the Feminism movement. She was awarded the 1994 Gordon Allport Intergroup Relations Prize for her research examining the effects of sexist advertising on men's behavior toward female job applicants.

The psychology of self and identity is a subfield of Psychology that moves psychological research “deeper inside the conscious mind of the person and further out into the person’s social world.” The exploration of self and identity subsequently enables the influence of both inner phenomenal experiences and the outer world in relation to the individual to be further investigated. This is particularly necessary following the topic's prevalence within the domain of social psychology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Self-image</span> Mental picture of self that comes from different sources

Self-image is the mental picture, generally of a kind that is quite resistant to change, that depicts not only details that are potentially available to an objective investigation by others, but also items that have been learned by persons about themselves, either from personal experiences or by internalizing the judgments of others. In some formulations, it is a component of self-concept.

In social psychology, self-stereotyping is a process by which an individual integrates and internalizes commonly held characterizations of an in-group into their self-concept. It is described as part of social identity theory (SIT) and, more specifically, self-categorization theory (SCT).

Ethnic identity development includes the identity formation in an individual's self-categorization in, and psychological attachment to, (an) ethnic group(s). Ethnic identity is characterized as part of one's overarching self-concept and identification. It is distinct from the development of ethnic group identities.

Self-concealment is a psychological construct defined as "a predisposition to actively conceal from others personal information that one perceives as distressing or negative". Its opposite is self-disclosure.

In psychology and related fields, future orientation is broadly defined as the extent to which an individual thinks about the future, anticipates future consequences, and plans ahead before acting. Across development, future orientation is particularly important during periods of major changes, for example during the transition from adolescence to adulthood, when youth must make choices about social groups, academic paths, as well as risky behaviors like drug and alcohol use, and sexual activity. Several models have been developed to describe the various factors that combine to impact future orientation.

Race-based traumatic stress is the traumatic response to stress following a racial encounter. Robert T. Carter's (2007) theory of race-based traumatic stress implies that there are individuals of color who experience racial discrimination as traumatic, and often generate responses similar to post-traumatic stress. Race-based traumatic stress combines theories of stress, trauma and race-based discrimination to describe a particular response to negative racial encounters.

Social emotional development represents a specific domain of child development. It is a gradual, integrative process through which children acquire the capacity to understand, experience, express, and manage emotions and to develop meaningful relationships with others. As such, social emotional development encompasses a large range of skills and constructs, including, but not limited to: self-awareness, joint attention, play, theory of mind, self-esteem, emotion regulation, friendships, and identity development.

Moral identity is a concept within moral psychology concerning the importance of morality to a person’s identity, typically construed as either a trait-like individual difference, or set of chronically accessible schemas.

References

  1. 1 2 Leflot, Geertje; Onghena, Patrick; Colpin, Hilde (2010). "Teacher–child interactions: relations with children's self-concept in second grade". Infant and Child Development. 19 (4): 385–405. doi:10.1002/icd.672. ISSN   1522-7219.
  2. Flook, Lisa; Repetti, Rena L; Ullman, Jodie B (March 2005). "Classroom social experiences as predictors of academic performance". Developmental Psychology. 41 (2): 31–327. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.590.5750 . doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.319. ISSN   0012-1649. PMID   15769188.
  3. 1 2 Myers, David G. (2009). Social psychology (10th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. ISBN   978-0073370668.
  4. Ayduk, Ozlem; Gyurak, Anett; Luerssen, Anna (November 2009). "Rejection sensitivity moderates the impact of rejection on self-concept clarity". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 35 (11): 1467–1478. doi:10.1177/0146167209343969. ISSN   1552-7433. PMC   4184908 . PMID   19713567.
  5. Markus, H.; Nurius, P. (1986). "Possible selves". American Psychologist. 41 (9): 954–969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954. S2CID   550525.
  6. Wilson, AE; Ross, M (April 2001). "From chump to champ: people's appraisals of their earlier and present selves". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (4): 572–584. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572. ISSN   0022-3514. PMID   11316222.
  7. Ross, Michael; Wilson, Anne E (May 2002). "It feels like yesterday: self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (5): 792–803. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.792. ISSN   0022-3514. PMID   12003478.
  8. Wilson, Anne E.; Buehler, Roger; Lawford, Heather; Schmidt, Colin; Yong, An Gie (2012). "Basking in projected glory: The role of subjective temporal distance in future self-appraisal". European Journal of Social Psychology. 42 (3): 342–353. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1863. ISSN   1099-0992.
  9. Ahmad, Nik; Ismail, Hisham (2015). "Rediscovering Rogers's Self Theory and Personality". Journal of Educational, Health and Community Psychology. 4 via ResearchGate.
  10. Aronson, E.; Wilson, T.; Akert, R. (2007). Social Psychology. New York: Pearson Prentice Hall. p. 113. ISBN   9780132382458.
  11. 1 2 3 4 Rogers, C. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In (ed.) S. Koch, Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 3: Formulations of the person and the social context. New York: McGraw Hill.
  12. 1 2 3 4 McLeod, S. A. (2008). Self Concept. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/self-concept.html
  13. "Maslow, Abraham H." Psychologists and Their Theories for Students, edited by Kristine Krapp,vol. 2, Gale, 2005, pp. 303–324. Gale eBooks,https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/CX3456300032/GVRL?u=lincclin_pbcc&sid=GVRL&xid=123255e1
  14. Guimond, Serge; Chatard, Armand; Martinot, Delphine; Crisp, Richard J.; Redersdorff, Sandrine (2006). "Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (2): 221–242. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   16536648.
  15. 1 2 3 4 Trautwein, Ulrich; Lüdtke, Oliver; Marsh, Herbert W.; Nagy, Gabriel (2009). "Within-school social comparison: How students perceive the standing of their class predicts academic self-concept". Journal of Educational Psychology. 101 (4): 853–866. doi:10.1037/a0016306. ISSN   1939-2176.
  16. Gest, Scott D; Rulison, Kelly L; Davidson, Alice J; Welsh, Janet A (May 2008). "A reputation for success (or failure): the association of peer academic reputations with academic self-concept, effort, and performance across the upper elementary grades". Developmental Psychology. 44 (3): 625–636. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.625. ISSN   0012-1649. PMID   18473632.
  17. Gerrig, Richard J.; Zimbardo, Philip G. (2002). "Glossary of Psychological Terms". Psychology And Life. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Retrieved 14 March 2011.
  18. 1 2 Gore, Jonathan S.; Cross, Susan E. (2011). "Defining and Measuring Self-Concept Change". Psychological Studies. 56 (1): 135–141. doi:10.1007/s12646-011-0067-0. ISSN   0033-2968. S2CID   144222471.
  19. Turner, John; Oakes, Penny (1986). "The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence". British Journal of Social Psychology. 25 (3): 237–252. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1986.tb00732.x .
  20. Social Psychology in Action: Evidence-Based Interventions from Theory to Practice. Springer Link: Springer Nature. 2020. ISBN   978-3-030-13790-8. OCLC   1182516016. The thoughts and feelings that arise when you think about the group you belong to form your social identity.
  21. 1 2 Tiedemann, Joachim (2000). "Parents' gender stereotypes and teachers' beliefs as predictors of children's concept of their mathematical ability in elementary school". Journal of Educational Psychology. 92 (1): 144–151. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.144. ISSN   1939-2176.
  22. C., Broderick, Patricia (6 January 2014). The life span : human development for helping professionals. Blewitt, Pamela. (Fourth ed.). Boston. ISBN   9780132942881. OCLC   858749675.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. Markus, Hazel; Nurius, Paula (1984). "Self-Understanding And Self-Regulation In Middle Childhood". In Collins, WA (ed.). Development During Middle Childhood: The Years From Six to Twelve. National Research Council. Retrieved 9 July 2021.
  24. Zafar, Human; Nabeel, Tanzila; Khalily, Muhammad (June 2013). "Adolescents' Self-Concept and Their Attitudes Towards Parents, Teachers, and Police Authority". Pakistan Journal of Psychology. 44: 15–35.
  25. Shapka, Jennifer; Keating, Daniel (April 2005). "Structure and Change in Self-Concept during Adolescence". Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 37 (2): 83–96. doi:10.1037/h0087247.
  26. Interpersonal relationships and the self-concept. Brent A. Mattingly, Kevin P. McIntyre, Gary W. Lewandowski. Cham: Springer. 2020. ISBN   978-3-030-43747-3. OCLC   1156101130.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  27. 1 2 Mattingly, Brent A.; Lewandowski, Gary W. (January 2014). "Broadening Horizons: Self-Expansion in Relational and Non-Relational Contexts: Relational and Non-Relational Self-Expansion". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 8 (1): 30–40. doi:10.1111/spc3.12080.
  28. Gupta, Deepty; Thapliyal, Geeta (Summer 2015). "A Study of Prosocial Behaviour and Self Concept of Adolescents". I-Manager's Journal of Educational Psychology. 9: 38–45.
  29. 1 2 Preckel, Franzis; Brüll, Matthias (October 2010). "The benefit of being a big fish in a big pond: Contrast and assimilation effects on academic self-concept". Learning and Individual Differences. 20 (5): 522–531. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.007. ISSN   1041-6080.
  30. Marsh, Herbert W.; Martin, Andrew J. (2011). "Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 81 (1): 59–77. doi:10.1348/000709910X503501. ISSN   2044-8279. PMID   21391964.
  31. Sadhwani, Indu (Spring 2012). "Effect of Self-Concept on Adolescent Depression". Journal of Psychosocial Research. 7: 147–52.
  32. Rubie-Davies, Christine M. (May 2006). "Teacher Expectations and Student Self-Perceptions: Exploring Relationships". Psychology in the Schools. 43 (5): 537–552. doi:10.1002/pits.20169. ISSN   0033-3085.
  33. 1 2 3 Freund, Philipp Alexander; Kasten, Nadine (1 January 2012). "How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (2): 296–321. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.473.5659 . doi:10.1037/a0026556. PMID   22181852.
  34. Craven, Rhonda G.; Marsh, Herbert W. Marsh (1991). "Effects of internally focused feedback and attributional feedback on enhancement of academic self-concept". Journal of Educational Psychology. 83 (1): 17–27. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.1.17. ISSN   0022-0663.
  35. Çaglar, Emine (Summer 2009). "Similarities and Differences in Physical Self-Concept of Males and Females during Late Adolescence and Early Adulthood". Adolescence. 44 (174): 407–19. PMID   19764275.
  36. Klomsten, Anne; Skaalvik, Einar; Espnes, Geir (January 2004). "Physical Self-Concept and Sports: Do Gender Differences Still Exist?". Sex Roles. 50: 119–27. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000011077.10040.9a. S2CID   144257373.
  37. Findlay, Leanne; Bowker, Anne (January 2009). "The Link between Competitive Sport Participation and Self-Concept in Early Adolescence: A Consideration of Gender and Sport Orientation". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 38 (1): 29–40. doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9244-9. PMID   19636789. S2CID   23442548.
  38. Bukatko, Danuta; Daehler, Marvin W. (2004). Child Development: A Thematic Approach . Houghton Mifflin. p.  495. ISBN   978-0-618-33338-7.
  39. 1 2 Hine, F. R.; Carson, R. C.; Maddox, G. L.; Thompson, R. J. Jr; Williams, R. B. (2012). Introduction to Behavioral Science in Medicine. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 106. ISBN   978-1-4612-5452-2.
  40. LEHNE, GREGORY K. "Gender Identity." International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family,edited by James J. Ponzetti Jr., Gale, 2nd edition, 2003. Credo Reference, https://go.openathens.net/redirector/palmbeachstate.edu?url=https%3A%2F%2Fsearch.credoreference.com%2Fcontent%2Fentry%i%2Fgender_identity%2F0%3FinstitutionId%3D6086.
  41. "Lawrence Kohlberg." The Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology, edited by Jacqueline L. Longe, 3rded., vol. 1, Gale, 2016, pp. 631–632. Gale eBooks, https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/CX36310004ndi9/GVRL?u=lincclin_pbcc&sid=GVRL&xid=179efc26.
  42. Meek, William R.; Sullivan, Diane M. (September 2018). "The Influence of Gender, Self-Identity and Organizational Tenure on Environmental Sustainability Orientation". Journal of Developmental Entrepreneurship. 23 (3): 1850018. doi:10.1142/S1084946718500188. ISSN   1084-9467. S2CID   158670007.
  43. 1 2 3 Taher, Naeema S. (1 January 2007). "Self-Concept and Masculinity/Femininity Among Normal Male Individuals and Males with Gender Identity Disorder". Social Behavior and Personality. 35 (4): 469–478. doi:10.2224/sbp.2007.35.4.469.
  44. Butler, Richard J.; Gasson, Sarah L. (November 2005). "Self Esteem/Self Concept Scales for Children and Adolescents: A Review". Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 10 (4): 190–201. doi:10.1111/j.1475-3588.2005.00368.x. ISSN   1475-357X. PMID   32806865.
  45. 1 2 3 Palenzuela-Luis, Natacha; Duarte-Clíments, Gonzalo; Gómez-Salgado, Juan; Rodríguez-Gómez, José Ángel; Sánchez-Gómez, María Begoña (10 January 2022). "Questionnaires Assessing Adolescents' Self-Concept, Self-Perception, Physical Activity and Lifestyle: A Systematic Review". Children. 9 (1): 91. doi: 10.3390/children9010091 . ISSN   2227-9067. PMC   8774997 . PMID   35053716.
  46. 1 2 Potard, Catherine; Amoura, Camille; Kubiszewski, Violaine; Le Samedy, Mathieu; Moltrecht, Brigitte; Courtois, Robert (June 2015). "Psychometric Properties of the French Version of the Short Form of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory Among Adolescents and Young Adults". Evaluation & the Health Professions. 38 (2): 265–279. doi:10.1177/0163278715568990. ISSN   0163-2787. PMID   25655375.
  47. Ryden, Muriel B. (December 1978). "An Adult Version of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory: Test-Retest Reliability and Social Desirability". Psychological Reports. 43 (3_suppl): 1189–1190. doi:10.2466/pr0.1978.43.3f.1189. ISSN   0033-2941.
  48. 1 2 3 Palenzuela-Luis, Natacha; Duarte-Clíments, Gonzalo; Gómez-Salgado, Juan; Rodríguez-Gómez, José Ángel; Sánchez-Gómez, María Begoña (January 2022). "Questionnaires Assessing Adolescents' Self-Concept, Self-Perception, Physical Activity and Lifestyle: A Systematic Review". Children. 9 (1): 91. doi: 10.3390/children9010091 . ISSN   2227-9067. PMC   8774997 . PMID   35053716.
  49. 1 2 Frydenberg, Erica (1 November 1994). "Coping and its correlates: What the adolescent coping scale tells us". The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist. 11 (2): 1–11. doi:10.1017/S0816512200027000. ISSN   0816-5122.
  50. 1 2 Koole, S. (2007). Self-Concept. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 797–798). SAGE Publications.
  51. 1 2 3 Reeve, J. (2018). The Self and Its Strivings. In Understanding Motivation and Emotion (7th ed., pp. 255–282). essay, Wiley.
  52. 1 2 3 4 Swann, William B.; Jetten, Jolanda; Gómez, Ángel; Whitehouse, Harvey; Bastian, Brock (1 January 2012). "When group membership gets personal: A theory of identity fusion" (PDF). Psychological Review. 119 (3): 441–456. doi:10.1037/a0028589. PMID   22642548. Archived from the original (PDF) on 13 April 2015.
  53. Markus, Hazel R.; Kitayama, Shinobu (1991). "Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation". Psychological Review. 98 (2): 224–253. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.320.1159 . doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224. ISSN   1939-1471. S2CID   13606371.
  54. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Usborne, Esther; Taylor, Donald M (2010). "The Role of Cultural Identity Clarity for Self-Concept Clarity, Self-Esteem, and Subjective Well-Being". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (7): 883–897. doi:10.1177/0146167210372215. PMID   20519575. S2CID   45197124.
  55. Shin, Kiseong (2017). The Concept of Self in Hinduism, Buddhism, and Christianity and Its Implication for Interfaith Relations. Eugene, Oregon: Wipf and Stock Publishers. ISBN   9781532600951 . Retrieved 15 October 2018.
  56. 1 2 English, Tammy; Chen, Serena (2011). "Self-Concept Consistency and Culture: The Differential Impact of Two Forms of Consistency". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (6): 838–849. doi:10.1177/0146167211400621. PMID   21398583. S2CID   9708510.
  57. Hoffer, Axel, ed. (2015). Freud and the Buddha: The Couch and the Cushion. Abingdon: Routledge (published 2018). ISBN   9780429913969 . Retrieved 15 October 2018. Impermanence is also the basis for the Buddhist view that the self has no substantial existence. It does not exist except in one's mind and what goes on in the mind, along with everything else, is impermanent. Life itself is in constant motion. Thus, there is no self. This view of the 'not self,' or 'no self,' is the most difficult concept for non-Buddhists to understand, particularly because the self, individuality, and autonomy are so basic to Western sensibilities.
  58. 1 2 3 4 Somech, Anit (1 March 2000). "The independent and the interdependent selves: different meanings in different cultures". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 24 (2): 161–172. doi:10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00030-9. ISSN   0147-1767.
  59. Nishikawa, Saori; Norlander, Torsten (2007). "A Cross-Cultural Validation of Adolescent Self-Concept in Two Cultures: Japan and Sweden". Social Behavior and Personality. 35 (2): 269–286. doi:10.2224/sbp.2007.35.2.269.
  60. Steele, Claude (2010). "Whistling Vivaldi: and Other Clues to How Stereotypes Affect Us". Issues of Our Time: 59–60.
  61. 1 2 Meador, Audrey (3 January 2018). "Examining Recruitment and Retention Factors for Minority STEM Majors Through a Stereotype Threat Lens". School Science and Mathematics. 118 (1–2): 61–69. doi:10.1111/ssm.12260. ISSN   0036-6803.
  62. Sunny, Cijy (April 2017). "Stereotype Threat and Gender Differences in Chemistry". Instructional Science. 45 (2): 157–175. doi:10.1007/s11251-016-9395-8. S2CID   151919352.
  63. Gofton, Wade; Regehr, Glenn (2006). "What We Don't Know We Are Teaching". Clinical Orthopaedics & Related Research. 449: 20–27. doi:10.1097/01.blo.0000224024.96034.b2. ISSN   0009-921X. PMID   16735868.
  64. Delpit, Lisa (2006). "Education in a Multicultural Society: Our Future's Greatest Challenge". Other People's Children: Cultural Conflict in the Classroom: 167–183.
  65. Grasha, Anthony (2010). "A Matter of Style: The Teacher as Expert, Formal Authority, Personal Model, Facilitator, and Delegator, College Teaching". College Teaching. 42 (4): 142–149. doi:10.1080/87567555.1994.9926845.
  66. 1 2 3 Maccoby, EE (April 1990). "Gender and relationships. A developmental account". The American Psychologist. 45 (4): 513–520. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.513. ISSN   0003-066X. PMID   2186679.
  67. Benenson, Joyce F; Heath, Anna (March 2006). "Boys withdraw more in one-on-one interactions, whereas girls withdraw more in groups". Developmental Psychology. 42 (2): 272–282. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.272. ISSN   0012-1649. PMID   16569166. S2CID   15754277.
  68. Klomsten, Anne Torhild; Skaalvik, Einar M.; Espnes, Geir Arild (1 January 2004). "Physical Self-Concept and Sports: Do Gender Differences Still Exist?". Sex Roles. 50 (1–2): 119–127. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000011077.10040.9a. ISSN   0360-0025. S2CID   144257373.
  69. Jacklin, Carol Nagy; Maccoby, Eleanor E. (September 1978). "Social Behavior at Thirty-Three Months in Same-Sex and Mixed-Sex Dyads". Child Development. 49 (3): 557–569. doi:10.2307/1128222. ISSN   0009-3920. JSTOR   1128222.
  70. Skaalvik, Sidsel; Skaalvik, Einar M. (1 February 2004). "Gender Differences in Math and Verbal Self-Concept, Performance Expectations, and Motivation". Sex Roles. 50 (3–4): 241–252. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000015555.40976.e6. ISSN   0360-0025. S2CID   144393272.
  71. Ertl, Bernhard; Luttenberger, Silke; Paechter, Manuela (17 May 2017). "The Impact of Gender Stereotypes on the Self-Concept of Female Students in STEM Subjects with an Under-Representation of Females". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00703 . PMC   5434750 . PMID   28567022.
  72. 1 2 Cheong, P., Hwang, J., Elbirt, B., Chen, H., Evans, C., & Woelfel, J. (2010). Media use as a function of identity: The role of the self concept in media usage. In v. M. Hinner (Ed.), The role of communication in business transactions and relationships, Vol. 6: Freiberger Beiträge zur interkulturellen und Wirtschaftskommunikation: A Forum for General and Intercultural Business Communication (pp. 365 – 381). Berlin: Peter Lang.
  73. Gündüz, Uğur (Fall 2017). "The Effect of Social Media on Identity Construction". Mediterranean Journal of Social Sciences. 8 (5): 85–92. doi: 10.1515/mjss-2017-0026 .
  74. 1 2 Beddington, Sir John (2013). "Future Identities Changing Identities in the UK: the next 10 years". Foresight Government Office for Science.
  75. 1 2 Long, Chen, J, M (2007). "The Impact of Internet Usage on Adolescent Self-Identity Development". China Media Research. 3: 99–109.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)

Further reading

_*:MANGLIGOT.MARVIN. RUIZ. FOUR(04.). JANUARY(01.). NINETEEN HUNDRED(1900+). EIGHTY FIVE(1985.)!_*: