Autobiographical memory

Last updated

Autobiographical memory (AM) [1] is a memory system consisting of episodes recollected from an individual's life, based on a combination of episodic (personal experiences and specific objects, people and events experienced at particular time and place) [2] and semantic (general knowledge and facts about the world) memory. [3] It is thus a type of explicit memory.

Contents

Formation

Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) proposed that autobiographical memory is constructed within a self-memory system (SMS), a conceptual model composed of an autobiographical knowledge base and the working self. [4] [5]

Autobiographical knowledge base

The autobiographical knowledge base contains knowledge of the self, used to provide information on what the self is, what the self was, and what the self can be. [6] This information is categorized into three broad areas: lifetime periods, general events, and event-specific knowledge. [4] [7]

Lifetime periods are composed of general knowledge about a distinguishable and themed time in an individual's life. For example, the period spent at school (school theme), or entering the workforce (work theme). Lifetime periods have a distinct beginning and ending, but they are often fuzzy and overlap. [4] Lifetime periods contain thematic knowledge about the features of that period, such as the activities, relationships, and locations involved, as well as temporal knowledge about the duration of the period. [4] The thematic information in these periods can be used to group them together under broader themes, which can reflect personal attitudes or goals. [4] As an example, a lifetime period with the theme of "when I lost my job" could fall under the broader category of either "when everything went downhill for me" or "minor setbacks in my life."

General events are more specific than lifetime periods and encompass single representations of repeated events or a sequence of related events. [4] General events group into clusters with a common theme, so that when one memory of a general event is recalled, it cues the recall of other related events in memory. These clusters of memories often form around the theme of either achieving or failing to achieve personal goals. [4] Clusters of general events that fall under the category of "first-time" achievements or occasions seem to have a particular vividness, such as the first time kissing a romantic partner, or the first time going to a ball game. [8] These memories of goal-attainment pass on important information about the self, such as how easily a skill can be acquired, or an individual's success and failure rates for certain tasks. [4]

Event-specific knowledge (ESK) is vividly detailed information about individual events, often in the form of visual images and sensory-perceptual features. [4] The high levels of detail in ESK fade very quickly, though certain memories for specific events tend to endure longer. [9] Originating events (events that mark the beginning of a path towards long-term goals), turning points (events that re-direct plans from original goals), anchoring events (events that affirm an individual's beliefs and goals) and analogous events (past events that direct behaviour in the present) are all event specific memories that will resist memory decay. [9]

The sensory-perceptual details held in ESK, though short-lived, are a key component in distinguishing memory for experienced events from imagined events. [10] In the majority of cases, it is found that the more ESK a memory contains, the more likely the recalled event has actually been experienced. [10] Unlike lifetime periods and general events, ESK are not organized in their grouping or recall. Instead, they tend to simply 'pop' into the mind. [4] ESK is also thought to be a summary of the content of episodic memories, which are contained in a separate memory system from the autobiographical knowledge base. [6] This way of thinking could explain the rapid loss of event-specific detail, as the links between episodic memory and the autobiographical knowledge base are likewise quickly lost. [6]

Hierarchical structure of the autobiographical knowledge base AMB hierarchy.png
Hierarchical structure of the autobiographical knowledge base

These three areas are organized in a hierarchy within the autobiographical knowledge base and together make up the overall life story of an individual. [6] Knowledge stored in lifetime periods contain cues for general events, and knowledge at the level of general events calls upon event-specific knowledge. [4] When a cue evenly activates the autobiographical knowledge base hierarchy, all levels of knowledge become available and an autobiographical memory is formed. [4]

When the pattern of activation encompasses episodic memory, then autonoetic consciousness may result. [6] Autonoetic consciousness or recollective experience is the sense of "mental time travel" that is experienced when recalling autobiographical memories. [6] These recollections consist of a sense of self in the past and some imagery and sensory-perceptual details. [3] Autonoetic consciousness reflects the integration of parts of the autobiographical knowledge base and the working self. [6]

Working self

The working self, often referred to as just the 'self', is a set of active personal goals or self-images organized into goal hierarchies. These personal goals and self-images work together to modify cognition and the resulting behavior so an individual can operate effectively in the world. [3]

The working self is similar to working memory: it acts as a central control process, controlling access to the autobiographical knowledge base. [6] The working self manipulates the cues used to activate the knowledge structure of the autobiographical knowledge base and in this way can control both the encoding and recalling of specific autobiographical memories. [6]

The relationship between the working self and the autobiographical knowledge base is reciprocal. While the working self can control the accessibility of autobiographical knowledge, the autobiographical knowledge base constrains the goals and self-images of the working self within who the individual actually is and what they can do. [6]

Types

There are four main categories for the types of autobiographical memories:

  1. Biographical or Personal: These autobiographical memories often contain biographical information, such as where one was born or the names of one's parents. [3]
  2. Copies vs. Reconstructions: Copies are vivid autobiographical memories of an experience with a considerable amount of visual and sensory-perceptual detail. Such autobiographical memories have different levels of authenticity. Reconstructions are autobiographical memories that are not reflections of raw experiences, but are rebuilt to incorporate new information or interpretations made in hind-sight. [3]
  3. Specific vs. Generic: Specific autobiographical memories contain a detailed memory of a certain event (event-specific knowledge); generic autobiographical memories are vague and hold little detail other than the type of event that occurred. Episodic autobiographical memories can also be categorized into generic memories, where one memory of an event is representative of a series of similar events. [3]
  4. Field vs. Observer: Autobiographical memories can be experienced from different perspectives. Field memories are memories recollected in the original perspective, from a first-person point of view. Observer memories are memories recollected from a perspective outside ourselves, a third-person point of view. [3] Typically, older memories are recollected through an observer perspective, [11] and observer memories are more often reconstructions while field memories are more vivid like copies. [3]

Autobiographical memories can also be differentiated into Remember vs. Know categories. The source of a remembered memory is attributed to personal experience. The source of a known memory is attributed to an external source, not personal memory. This can often lead to source-monitoring error, wherein a person may believe that a memory is theirs when the information actually came from an external source. [12]

Functions

Autobiographical memory serves three broad functions: directive, social, and self-representative. [13] A fourth function, adaptive, was proposed by Williams, Conway and Cohen (2008). [3]

The directive function of autobiographical memory uses past experiences as a reference for solving current problems and a guide for our actions in the present and the future. [3] Memories of personal experiences and the rewards and losses associated with them can be used to create successful models, or schemas, of behavior. which can be applied over many scenarios. [14] In instances where a problem cannot be solved by a generic schema, a more specific memory of an event can be accessed in autobiographical memory to give some idea of how to confront the new challenge. [3]

The social function of autobiographical memory develops and maintains social bonds by providing material for people to converse about. [13] Sharing personal memories with others is a way to facilitate social interaction. [3] Disclosing personal experiences can increase the intimacy level between people and reminiscing of shared past events strengthens pre-existing bonds. [3] The importance of this function can easily be seen in individuals with impaired episodic or autobiographical memory, where their social relationships suffer greatly as a result. [15]

Autobiographical memory performs a self-representative function by using personal memories to create and maintain a coherent self-identity over time. [3] This self-continuity is the most commonly referred to self-representative function of autobiographical memory. [13] A stable self-identity allows for evaluation of past experiences, known as life reflection, which leads to self-insight and often self-growth. [13]

Finally, autobiographical memory serves an adaptive function. Recalling positive personal experiences can be used to maintain desirable moods or alter undesirable moods. [15] This internal regulation of mood through autobiographical memory recall can be used to cope with negative situations and impart an emotional resilience. [3] The effects of mood on memory are explained in better detail under the Emotion section.

Memory disorder

There are many sorts of amnesia, and in studying their different forms, apparent defects in individual subsystems of the brain's memory systems may be observed, and their function in the normally-working brain clarified. Other neurological disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease can also affect memory and cognition. [16]

Individual differences

Autobiographical memory may differ greatly between individuals. Hyperthymesia, also known as hyperthymestic syndrome or highly superior autobiographical memory (HSAM), is a condition that affects an individual's autobiographical memory, essentially meaning that they cannot forget small details which otherwise would not be stored. [17] It forms one extreme, in which a person might recall vividly almost every day of their life (usually from around the age of 10). On the other extreme is severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM), [18] where a person cannot relive memories from their lives, although this does not affect other memory capabilities or general cognition. [19] [20] SDAM is a severe autobiographical memory deficiency, but without amnesia. [21] [22]

Memory perspectives

People often re-experience visual images when remembering events; one specific aspect of these images is their perspective. [23] Basically, there are two types of perspective:

The field and observer perspectives have also been described as "pre-reflective" and "reflective," respectively. [25] Different brain regions are activated by the pre-reflective and reflective perspectives. [25]

Moderators of perspective

Studies tested the prevalence of field and observer memories to determine which kind of memories occur at which times. Some of the moderators that change individuals' recalled perspectives are memory age, emotionality, and self-awareness. [24] Additionally, emotion and affect are associated with the field perspective's brain region, while complex cognitive processing is associated with the observer perspective's brain region. [25] The many factors that contribute to determining memory perspective are not affected by whether the recall of the memory was voluntary or involuntary. [26]

Cultural effects

Studies have shown that culture can affect the point of view autobiographical memory is recalled in. People living in Eastern cultures are more likely to recall memories through an observer point of view than those living in Western cultures. [29] Also, in Eastern cultures, situation plays a larger role in determining the perspective of memory recall than in Western cultures. For example, Easterners are more likely than Westerners to use observer perspective when remembering events where they are at the center of attention (like giving a presentation, having a birthday party, etc.). [30]

There are many reasons for these differences in autobiographical perspectives across cultures. Each culture has its own unique set of factors that affect the way people perceive the world around them, such as uncertainty avoidance, masculinity, and power distance. [30] While these various cultural factors contribute to shaping one's memory perspective, the biggest factor in shaping memory perspective is individualism. [30] One's sense of self is important in influencing whether autobiographical memories are recalled in the observer or field point of view. Western society has been found to be more individualistic, with people being more independent and stressing less importance on familial ties or the approval of others. [29] On the other hand, Eastern cultures are thought of as less individualistic, focusing more on acceptance and maintaining family relationships while focusing less on the individual self. [29]

The way people in different cultures perceive the emotions of the people around them also play a role in shaping the recall perspective of memories. Westerners are said to have a more "inside-out view" of the world, and unknowingly project their current emotions onto the world around them. This practice is called egocentric projection. For example, when a person is feeling guilty about something they did earlier, they will perceive the people around them as also feeling guilty. [29] On the other hand, Easterners have a more "outside-in view" of the world, perceiving the people around them as having complementary emotions to their own. [29] With an outside-in view, someone who was feeling guilt would imagine the people around them looking upon them with scorn or disgust. These different perceptions across cultures of how one is viewed by others lead to different amounts of field or observer recall. [29]

Effects of gender

Women on average report more memories in the observer perspective than men. [31] A theory for this phenomenon is that women are more conscious about their personal appearance than men. [31] According to objectification theory, social and cultural expectations have created a society where women are far more objectified than men. [31]

In situations where one's physical appearance and actions are important (for example, giving a speech in front of an audience), the memory of that situation will likely be remembered in the observer perspective. [31] This is due to the general trend that when the focus of attention in a person's memory is on themselves, they will likely see themselves from someone else's point of view. This is because, in "center-of-attention" memories, the person is conscious about the way they are presenting themselves and instinctively try to envision how others were perceiving them. [31]

According to the theory - since women feel more objectified than men, they tend to be put in center-of-attention situations more often, which results in recalling more memories from the observer perspective. Studies also show that events with greater social interaction and significance produce more observer memories in women than events with low or no social interaction or significance. [31] Observer perspective in men was generally unaffected by the type of event. [31] Counterarguments to the theory are that people with social anxiety don't have a particularly strong autobiographical memory, which is what you would expect according to this theory. Females also show differences in male memory that isn't accounted for by some sociological reason.

Effects of personal identity

Another theory of the visual perspective deals with the continuity or discontinuity of the self. [28] Continuity is seen as a way to connect and strengthen the past self to the current self and discontinuity is distanced from the self. [28] This theory breaks down the observer method (i.e. when an individual recalls memories as an observer) into two possibilities: the "dispassionate observer" and the "salient self". [28]

People who picture their past self as different or conflicting with their current self often recall memories of their old self using the observer perspective. [32] People who have undergone some kind of change often look upon their past self (before the change) as if they were a completely different person. [32] These drastic personal changes include things like graduating, getting over an addiction, entering or leaving prison, getting diagnosed with cancer, losing weight, and any other major life events. [32] There is a split between the present self that is remembering and the past self that is remembered. [32]

Thus, the visual perspective employed for continuous and discontinuous memories is the opposite for each view. [28]

People who use observer perspective to remember their old self tend to believe that they are less likely to revert to their old self. [33] When a person recalls memories from the observer perspective, it helps preserve their self-image and self-esteem. [33] Remembering a traumatic or embarrassing event from an observer perspective helps detach that person from that negative event, as if they were not the one experiencing it, but rather someone else. [33] Given the distancing nature of observer perspective, it also results in a worse sense of self-continuity. [33]

Effects of trauma

Events high in emotional content, such as stressful situations (ex: fighting in the Vietnam War), are likely to be recalled using observer perspective, while memories low in emotional content (ex: driving to work) are likely to be recalled using field perspective. [27]

The main reason for this is probably that the observer perspective distances the person from the traumatic event, allowing them to recall the specifics and details of the event without having to relive the feelings and emotions. [34] The observer perspective tends to focus more on one's physical appearance, along with the spatial relations and peripheral details of the scene, which allows people to remember the specifics and important facts of their traumatic experience, without reliving most of the pain. [32] The field perspective, on the other hand, focuses on the physical and psychological feelings experienced at the time of the event. For many people, it can be too difficult to use this perspective to recall the event. [34]

Clinical psychologists have found that the observer perspective acts like a psychological "buffer" to decrease the stress an individual feels when recalling a difficult memory. [28] This is especially seen in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). [28] When patients with PTSD were asked to recall their traumatic experience, 89 percent of those who used observer perspective to recall the traumatic event said they did so because it was emotionally easier and spared them from reliving the horror of their traumatic event. [34] Although this is a useful coping mechanism, some argue that effective treatment of PTSD requires the patient to re-experience the emotions and fear from that traumatic event so that it can be processed into something less distressing. [34] Peter Lang and other researchers have hypothesized that the short-term relief the observer perspective provides may actually impede long-term recovery from PTSD. [34]

Methods of study

Diaries

Writing in a diary 2004-02-29 Ball point pen writing.jpg
Writing in a diary

Memory can be inaccurate and critical details of a raw experience can be forgotten or re-imagined. [35] The diary method of study circumvents these issues by having groups of participants keep a diary over a period of weeks or months, during which they record the details of everyday events that they judge to be memorable. In this way a record of true autobiographical memories can be collected. [35]

These true autobiographical memories can then be presented to the participants at a later date in a recognition test, often in comparison to falsified diary entries or 'foils'. [35] The results from these studies can give us information about the level of detail retained in autobiographical memory over time, and if certain features of an event are more salient and memorable in autobiographical memory. [35] [36]

A study performed by Barclay and Wellman (1986) included two types of foils in their recognition task: ones that were entirely false and ones that were the original diary entry with a few details altered. [36] Against the false foils, participants were found to be highly accurate at recognizing their true entries (at an average rate of 95%) and false foils were only judged as true 25% of the time. [36] However, when judging between true diary entries and the altered foils, the altered foils were incorrectly judged as true 50% of the time. [36] Barclay and Wellman theorized this was due to the tendency to group similar or repeated autobiographical memories into generic memories or schemas, and thus diary entries that seemed familiar enough to fit into these schemas would be judged as true. [36]

Memory probe

Originally devised by Galton (1879), the memory probe method uses a list of words as cues to bring to mind autobiographical memories, which the participant then tries to describe in as much detail as possible. [37] [38] The answers can then be analyzed in order to gain a better understanding as to how recall of autobiographical memory works, especially in cases dealing with brain damage or amnesia. [39]

Recent studies have used non-verbal cues for memory, such as visual images, music or odours. [40] Emerging evidence suggests that music is a strong cue for autobiographical memories. Compared to face-evoked, food-evoked, and television-evoked cues, music-evoked autobiographical memory cues were found to be more salient through measures including episodic richness, personal significance, and recall. [41] [42] [43] Chu and Downes (2002) found ample evidence that odour cues are particularly good at cueing autobiographical memories. [44] Odour-cued memories for specific events were more detailed and more emotionally loaded than those for verbal, visual, or non-related odour cues. [44]

Emotion

Happy emotions will strengthen a memory of an Olympic goal Celebrating gold.jpg
Happy emotions will strengthen a memory of an Olympic goal

Emotion affects the way autobiographical memories are encoded and retrieved. Emotional memories are reactivated more, they are remembered better and have more attention devoted to them. [45] Through remembering our past achievements and failures, autobiographical memories affect how we perceive and feel about ourselves. [46]

Positive

Positive autobiographical memories contain more sensory and contextual details than negative and neutral memories. [45] People high in self-esteem recall more details for memories where the individual displayed positive personality traits than memories dealing with negative personality traits. [46] People with high self-esteem also devoted more resources to encoding these positive memories over negative memories. [46] In addition, it was found that people high in self-esteem reactivate positive memories more often than people with low self-esteem, and reactivate memories about other people's negative personality traits more often to maintain their positive self-image. [46]

Positive memories appear to be more resistant to forgetting. All memories fade, and the emotions linked with them become less intense over time. [47] However, this fading effect is seen less with positive memories than with negative memories, leading to a better remembrance of positive memories. [47]

As well, recall of autobiographical memories that are important in defining ourselves differs depending on the associated emotion. Past failures seem farther away than past achievements, regardless if the actual length of time is the same. [46]

Negative

Negative memories generally fade faster than positive memories of similar emotional importance and encoding period. [47] This difference in retention period and vividness for positive memories is known as the fading affect bias. [48] In addition, coping mechanisms in the mind are activated in response to a negative event, which minimizes the stress and negative events experienced. [48]

While it seems adaptive to have negative memories fade faster, sometimes it may not be the case. Remembering negative events can prevent us from acting overconfident or repeating the same mistake, and we can learn from them in order to make better decisions in the future. [46]

However, increased remembering of negative memories can lead to the development of maladaptive conditions. The effect of mood-congruent memory, wherein the mood of an individual can influence the mood of the memories they recall, is a key factor in the development of depressive symptoms for conditions such as dysphoria or major depressive disorder. [49]

Dysphoria: Individuals with mild to moderate Dysphoria show an abnormal trend of the fading effect bias. The negative memories of dysphoric individuals did not fade as quickly relative to control groups, and positive memories faded slightly faster. [48] In severely dysphoric individuals the fading affect bias was exacerbated; negative memories faded more slowly and positive memories faded more quickly than non-dysphoria individuals. [48]

Unfortunately, this effect is not well understood. One possible explanation suggests that, in relation to mood-congruent memory theory, the mood of the individual at the time of recall rather than the time of encoding has a stronger effect on the longevity of negative memories. [48] If this is the case, further studies should hopefully show that changes in mood state will produce changes in the strength of the fading affect bias. [48]

Depression: Depression impacts the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Adolescents with depression tend to rate their memories as more accurate and vivid than never-depressed adolescents, and the content of recollection is different. [50]

Individuals with depression encounter trouble remembering specific personal past events, and instead recall more general events (repeated or recurring events). [51] Specific memory recall can further be inhibited by significant psychological trauma occurring in comorbidity. [50] When a specific episodic memory is recalled by an individual with depression, details for the event are almost non-existent and instead purely semantic knowledge is reported. [52]

The lack of remembered detail especially affects positive memories; generally people remember positive events with more detail than negative events, but the reverse is seen in those with depression. [51] Negative memories will seem more complex and the time of occurrence will be more easily remembered than positive and neutral events. [45] This may be explained by mood congruence theory, as depressed individuals remember negatively charged memories during frequent negative moods. [52] Depressed adults also tend to actively rehearse negative memories, which increases their retention period and vividness. [52]

Another explanation may be the tendency for individuals suffering from depression to separate themselves from their positive memories and focus more on evidence that supports their current negative self-image to keep it intact. [51] Depressed adults also recall positive memories from an observer perspective rather than a field perspective, where they appear as a spectator rather than a participant in their own memory. [52]

Finally, the autobiographical memory differences may be attributed to a smaller posterior hippocampal volume in any individuals going through cumulative stress. [53]

Effects of age

Temporal components

Memory changes with age; the temporal distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan, as modelled by Rubin, Wetzler, and Nebes (1986), [54] is separated into three components:

  1. Childhood or infantile amnesia
  2. The retention function (recency effect)
  3. The reminiscence bump

Infantile amnesia concerns memories from very early childhood, before age 6; very few memories before age 3 are available. The retention function is the recollection of events in the first 20 to 30 most recent years of an individual's life. This results in more memories for events closest to the present, a recency effect. Finally, there is the reminiscence bump occurring after around age 40, marked by an increase in the retrieval of memories from ages 10 to 30. For adolescents and young adults the reminiscence bump and the recency effect coincide. [54] [55]

Age effects

Autobiographical memory demonstrates only minor age differences, but distinctions between semantic versus episodic memories in older adults compared with younger people have been found.

Episodic to semantic shift

Piolino, Desgranges, Benali, and Eustache (2002) investigated age effects on autobiographical memory using an autobiographical questionnaire that distinguished between the recall of semantic and episodic memory. They proposed a transition from episodic to semantic memory in autobiographical memory recollection with increased age. Using four groups of adults aged 40–79, Piolino and colleagues found evidence for a greater decline in episodic memories with longer retention intervals and a more substantial age-related decline in recall of episodic memory than semantic memory. They also found support for the three components of autobiographical memory, as modeled by David Rubin and colleagues. [55]

Semanticizing memories, generalizing episodic memories by removing the specific temporal and spatial contexts, makes memories more persistent than age-sensitive episodic memories. Recent memories (retention interval) are episodic. Older memories are semanticized, becoming more resilient (reminiscence bump). [55] Semantic memories are less sensitive to age effects. Over time, autobiographical memories may consist more of general information than specific details of a particular event or time. In one study where participants recalled events from five life periods, older adults concentrated more on semantic details which were not tied to a distinct temporal or spatial context. Younger participants reported more episodic details such as activities, locations, perceptions, and thoughts. Even when probed for contextual details, older adults still reported more semantic details compared with younger adults. [56]

Voluntary versus involuntary memories

Research on autobiographical memory has focused on voluntary memories, memories that are deliberately recalled; nevertheless, research has evidenced differential effects of age on involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memory. One study found that fewer involuntary and voluntary memories were reported by older adults compared with younger adults. The voluntary memories of older adults were not as specific and were not recalled as quickly as those of younger adults. There was no consistent distinction between involuntary memories for younger and older adults. [57]

Positivity effect

Several studies have shown a positivity effect for autobiographical memories in older adults. One study found a positivity bias for involuntary memories, where younger adults did not rate their involuntary memories as positively as did older adults. Voluntary memories did not show this difference. [57] Another study found a reminiscence bump for adults in their 20s for happy involuntary memories but not for unhappy involuntary memories. Happy involuntary memories were also more than twice as frequent as unhappy involuntary memories. In older participants, a bump for memories reported as most important and happy was found. The saddest and most traumatic memories showed a declining retention function. [58] The positivity bias could reflect an emphasis on emotional-regulation goals in older adults. [59]

Accuracy

Judging the veracity of autobiographical memories can be a source of difficulty. However, it is important to be able to verify the accurateness of autobiographical memories in order to study them.

Vividness

The vividness of the memory can increase one's belief in the veracity of the memory but not as strongly as spatial context. [60] Some memories are extremely vivid. For the person recalling vivid memories of personal significance, these memories appear to be more accurate than everyday memories. These memories have been termed flashbulb memories. However, flashbulb memories may not be any more accurate than everyday memories when evaluated objectively. In one study, both flash bulb memories of 9/11 and everyday memories deteriorated over time; however, reported vividness, recollection and belief in accuracy of flashbulb memories remained high. [61]

False memories

False memories often do not have as much visual imagery as true memories. [60] In one study comparing the characteristics of true and false autobiographical memories, true memories were reported to be wealthier in "recollective experience" or providing many details of the originally encoded event, by participants and observers. The participants engaging in recall reported true memories as being more important, emotionally intense, less typical, and having clearer imagery. True memories were generally reported to have a field perspective versus an observer perspective. An observer perspective was more prominent in false memories. True memories provided more information, including details about the consequences following the recalled event. However, with repeated recollection, false memories may become more like true memories and acquire greater detail. [62]

False memory syndrome is a controversial condition in which people demonstrate conviction for vivid but false personal memories. [63] False memories and confabulation, reporting events that did not occur, may reflect errors in source-monitoring. Confabulation can be a result of brain damage, but it can also be provoked by methods employed in memory exploration.

Professionals such as therapists, police and lawyers must be aware of the malleability of memory and be wary of techniques that might promote false memory generation. [64]

Neuroanatomy

Neural networks

Diagram of the different lobes of the brain Cerebrum lobes.svg
Diagram of the different lobes of the brain

The autobiographical memory knowledge base is distributed through neural networks in the frontal, temporal and occipital lobes. The most abstract or conceptual knowledge is represented in frontal and anterior temporal networks, possibly bilaterally. Sensory and perceptual details of specific events are represented in posterior temporal and occipital networks, predominantly in the right cortex. [65]

A "core" neural network composed of the left medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices, medial and lateral temporal cortices, temporoparietal junction, posterior cingulate cortex, and cerebellum [66] are consistently identified as activated regions in at least half of the current imaging studies on autobiographical memory. A "secondary" neural network composed of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, superior medial cortex, superior lateral cortex, anterior cingulate, medial orbitofrontal, temporopolar and occipital cortices, thalamus and amygdala [66] can be identified as active regions in a quarter to a third of imaging studies on autobiographical memory. Regions of the brain that are reported infrequently, in less than a quarter of autobiographical memory imaging studies, include the frontal eye fields, motor cortex, medial and lateral parietal cortices, fusiform gyrus, superior and inferior lateral temporal cortices, insula, basal ganglia and brain stem. [66]

These widespread activation patterns suggest that a number of varying domain-specific processes unique to re-experiencing phenomena, such as emotional and perceptual processes, and domain-general processes, such as attention and memory, are necessary for successful autobiographical memory retrieval.[ citation needed ]

Construction and retrieval

Autobiographical memories are initially constructed in left prefrontal neural networks. As a memory forms over time, activation then transitions to right posterior networks where it remains at a high level while the memory is held in the mind. [65]

Networks in the left frontal lobe in the dorsolateral cortex and bilaterally in the prefrontal cortex become active during autobiographical memory retrieval. These regions are involved with reconstructive mnemonic processes and self-referential processes, both integral to autobiographical memory retrieval. There is a complex pattern of activation over time of retrieval of detailed autobiographical memories that stimulates brain regions used not only in autobiographical memory, but feature in other memory tasks and other forms of cognition as well.[ clarification needed ] It is the specific pattern in its totality that distinguishes autobiographical cognition from other forms of cognition. [65]

Maintenance of a detailed memory

Autobiographical memory maintenance is predominantly observed as changing patterns of activity within posterior sensory regions; more specifically, occipitotemporal regions of the right hemisphere. [65]

Related Research Articles

Recall in memory refers to the mental process of retrieval of information from the past. Along with encoding and storage, it is one of the three core processes of memory. There are three main types of recall: free recall, cued recall and serial recall. Psychologists test these forms of recall as a way to study the memory processes of humans and animals. Two main theories of the process of recall are the two-stage theory and the theory of encoding specificity.

A flashbulb memory is a vivid, long-lasting memory about a surprising or shocking event.

Episodic memory is the memory of everyday events that can be explicitly stated or conjured. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at particular times and places; for example, the party on one's 7th birthday. Along with semantic memory, it comprises the category of explicit memory, one of the two major divisions of long-term memory.

In psychology and cognitive science, a schema describes a pattern of thought or behavior that organizes categories of information and the relationships among them. It can also be described as a mental structure of preconceived ideas, a framework representing some aspect of the world, or a system of organizing and perceiving new information, such as a mental schema or conceptual model. Schemata influence attention and the absorption of new knowledge: people are more likely to notice things that fit into their schema, while re-interpreting contradictions to the schema as exceptions or distorting them to fit. Schemata have a tendency to remain unchanged, even in the face of contradictory information. Schemata can help in understanding the world and the rapidly changing environment. People can organize new perceptions into schemata quickly as most situations do not require complex thought when using schema, since automatic thought is all that is required.

Explicit memory is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts. This type of memory is dependent upon three processes: acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval.

A flashback, or involuntary recurrent memory, is a psychological phenomenon in which an individual has a sudden, usually powerful, re-experiencing of a past experience or elements of a past experience. These experiences can be frightful, happy, sad, exciting, or any number of other emotions. The term is used particularly when the memory is recalled involuntarily, especially when it is so intense that the person "relives" the experience, and is unable to fully recognize it as memory of a past experience and not something that is happening in "real time".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Involuntary memory</span> Memory triggered by an environmental cue

Involuntary memory, also known as involuntary explicit memory, involuntary conscious memory, involuntary aware memory, madeleine moment, mind pops and most commonly, involuntary autobiographical memory, is a sub-component of memory that occurs when cues encountered in everyday life evoke recollections of the past without conscious effort. Voluntary memory, its opposite, is characterized by a deliberate effort to recall the past.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reminiscence bump</span> Memory phenomenon related to human aging

The reminiscence bump is the tendency for adults over forty to have increased or enhanced recollection for events that occurred during their adolescence and early adulthood. It was identified through the study of autobiographical memory and the subsequent plotting of the age of encoding of memories to form the lifespan retrieval curve.

There is evidence suggesting that different processes are involved in remembering something versus knowing whether it is familiar. It appears that "remembering" and "knowing" represent relatively different characteristics of memory as well as reflect different ways of using memory.

Emotion can have a powerful effect on humans and animals. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.

In psychology, mental time travel is the capacity to mentally reconstruct personal events from the past as well as to imagine possible scenarios in the future. The term was coined by Thomas Suddendorf and Michael Corballis, building on Endel Tulving's work on episodic memory.

Autonoetic consciousness is the human ability to mentally place oneself in the past and future or in counterfactual situations, and to thus be able to examine one's own thoughts.

The development of memory is a lifelong process that continues through adulthood. Development etymologically refers to a progressive unfolding. Memory development tends to focus on periods of infancy, toddlers, children, and adolescents, yet the developmental progression of memory in adults and older adults is also circumscribed under the umbrella of memory development.

Childhood amnesia, also called infantile amnesia, is the inability of adults to retrieve episodic memories before the age of three to four years. It may also refer to the scarcity or fragmentation of memories recollected from early childhood, particularly occurring between the ages of 3 and 6. On average, this fragmented period wanes off at around 4.7 years. Around 5–6 years of age in particular is thought to be when autobiographical memory seems to stabilize and be on par with adults. The development of a cognitive self is also thought by some to have an effect on encoding and storing early memories.

The encoding specificity principle is the general principle that matching the encoding contexts of information at recall assists in the retrieval of episodic memories. It provides a framework for understanding how the conditions present while encoding information relate to memory and recall of that information.

A personal-event memory is an individual's memory of an event from a certain moment of time. Its defining characteristics are that it is for a specific event; includes vivid multi-sensory elements ; is usually recalled in detail; and is usually believed by the individual to be an accurate representation of the event.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reconstructive memory</span> A theory of memory recall

Reconstructive memory is a theory of memory recall, in which the act of remembering is influenced by various other cognitive processes including perception, imagination, motivation, semantic memory and beliefs, amongst others. People view their memories as being a coherent and truthful account of episodic memory and believe that their perspective is free from an error during recall. However, the reconstructive process of memory recall is subject to distortion by other intervening cognitive functions and operations such as individual perceptions, social influences, and world knowledge, all of which can lead to errors during reconstruction.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bilingual memory</span>

Bilingualism is the regular use of two fluent languages, and bilinguals are those individuals who need and use two languages in their everyday lives. A person's bilingual memories are heavily dependent on the person's fluency, the age the second language was acquired, and high language proficiency to both languages. High proficiency provides mental flexibility across all domains of thought and forces them to adopt strategies that accelerate cognitive development. People who are bilingual integrate and organize the information of two languages, which creates advantages in terms of many cognitive abilities, such as intelligence, creativity, analogical reasoning, classification skills, problem solving, learning strategies, and thinking flexibility.

The fading affect bias, more commonly known as FAB, is a psychological phenomenon in which memories associated with negative emotions tend to be forgotten more quickly than those associated with positive emotions. FAB only refers to the feelings one has associated with the memories and not the content of the memories themselves. Early research studied FAB retrospectively, or through personal reflection, which brought about some criticism because retrospective analysis can be affected by subjective retrospective biases. However, new research using non-retrospective recall studies have found evidence for FAB, and the phenomenon has become largely accepted.

Music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) refer to the recollection of personal experiences or past events that are triggered when hearing music or some musical stimulus. While there is a degree of inter-individual variation in music listening patterns and evoked responses, MEAMs are generally triggered in response to a wide variety of music, often popular or classical genres, and are estimated to occur in the range from one to a few times per day, regardless of formal instrumental practice or music lessons. Consistent with the hallmarks of general autobiographical memories, everyday MEAMs similarly exhibit a recency effect, a reminiscence bump, and childhood amnesia, encoding autobiographical knowledge at several levels of specificity and across several common social and situational contexts. The phenomenon of MEAMs has been widely studied in the fields of psychology, neuroscience, and musicology. In recent years, the subject has garnered significant interest from researchers and the general public alike due to music's capacity to evoke vivid, emotional, and episodically rich autobiographical memories.

References

  1. Vranić, Andrea; Jelić, Margareta; Tonković, Mirjana (2018). "Functions of Autobiographical Memory in Younger and Older Adults". Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 219. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00219 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   5863506 . PMID   29599732.
  2. "Autobiographical Memory". ScienceDirect . Retrieved 2023-01-07.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Cohen, Gillian; Conway, Martin A., eds. (2008). "Williams, Helen L., et al. 2 Autobiographical memory". Memory in the Real World (3rd ed.). Hove: Psychology Press. ISBN   978-0203934852. (Master e-book) ISBN   978-1841696409 (hbk) ISBN   978-1841696416 (pbk) ISBN   978-1135419875 (Google Books).
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Conway, M. A.; Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). "The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system". Psychological Review. 107 (2): 261–288. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.621.9717 . doi:10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261. PMID   10789197.
  5. Jenkins, Laura (2020-11-17). "Remembering Our Past: What Are the Factors That Can Affect Autobiographical Memory?". Psychreg.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Conway, Martin A. (2005-10-01). "Memory and the self". Journal of Memory and Language. 53 (4): 594–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005 . ISSN   0749-596X.
  7. Gregory, Lauren (2011), "Autobiographical Memory", in Goldstein, Sam; Naglieri, Jack A. (eds.), Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development, Boston: Springer, p. 187, doi:10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_252, ISBN   978-0-387-79061-9, S2CID   246947866 , retrieved 2023-01-07
  8. 1 2 3 Conway, Martin A.; Rubin, David C.; Spinnler, Hans; Wagenaar, Willem A., eds. (1992). "Robinson, John A. First Experience Memories: Contexts and Functions in Personal Histories". Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory. Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-015-7967-4. ISBN   978-94-015-7967-4. (eBook).
  9. 1 2 Pillemer, David B. (2001-06-01). "Momentous events and the life story". Review of General Psychology. 5 (2): 123–134. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.123. S2CID   197650204. APA PsycNet.
  10. 1 2 Johnson, M. K.; Foley, M. A.; Suengas, A. G.; Raye, C. L. (1988). "Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 117 (4): 371–376. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.4.371. PMID   2974863.
  11. Piolino, P.; Desgranges, B.; Clarys, D.; Guillery-Girard, B.; Taconnat, L.; Isingrini, M.; Eustache, F. (2006). "Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging". Psychology and Aging. 21 (3): 510–525. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.510. eISSN   1939-1498. ISSN   0882-7974. PMID   16953713.
  12. Hyman, Ira E. Jr.; Gilstrap, Livia L.; Decker, Kevin; Wilkinson, Carol (1999-01-06). "Manipulating remember and know judgements of autobiographical memories: An investigation of false memory creation". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 12 (4): 371–386. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199808)12:4<371::aid-acp572>3.0.co;2-u.
  13. 1 2 3 4 Bluck, Susan; Alea, Nicole; Haberman, Tilmann; Rubin, David C. (2005-02-01). "A tale of three functions: The self-reported uses of autobiographical memory". Social Cognition. 23 (1): 91–117. doi:10.1521/soco.23.1.91.59198. hdl: 10161/10106 . S2CID   145337468.
  14. Pillemer, David B. (2003-03-01). "Directive functions of autobiographical memory: The guiding power of the specific episode". Memory. 11 (2). Hove: 193–202. doi:10.1080/741938208. ISSN   0965-8211. PMID   12820831. S2CID   19179261.
  15. 1 2 Robinson, John A.; Swanson, Karen L. (Summer 1990). "Autobiographical memory: The next phase". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 4 (4): 321–335. doi:10.1002/acp.2350040407.
  16. Acosta, Sandra Antonieta (2011). Multivariate Anti-inflammatory Approaches to Rescue Neurogenesis and Cognitive function in Aged Animals (PhD thesis). University of South Florida. OCLC   781835583. S2CID   141004870
  17. Marshall, Jessia (2008-02-13). "Forgetfulness is key to a healthy mind". New Scientist .
  18. "What's Your Memory Style?". Neuroscience News . 2015-12-10. Their responses fell between the extremes seen in people with Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM) or Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (SDAM) recently described by memory researchers.
  19. Richárd, Reichardt (2018-06-17). "Highly superior and severely deficient autobiographical memory". Agykutatás egyszerűen [Brain research made simple (Neuroscience News)] (in Hungarian). Retrieved 2023-01-07 via Wordpress site.
  20. Palombo, Daniela J. (2018-06-18). "Individual Differences in Autobiographical Memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 22 (7): 583–597. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.007. PMID   29807853. S2CID   44062714 . Retrieved 16 July 2022. The topic of individual differences in autobiographical memory has been brought to the forefront by the dramatic descriptions of HSAM and SDAM, which may represent the extremes along a distribution of individual differences in autobiographical memory.
  21. "What is SDAM ?". Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (SDAM). Brian Levine: The Levine Lab (Baycrest; University of Toronto) via Weebly site.
  22. Palombo, D. J.; Alain, C.; Söderlund, H.; Khuu, W.; Levine, B. (2015). "Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: A new mnemonic syndrome". Neuropsychologia. 72: 105–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.012 . PMID   25892594. S2CID   8255136.
  23. 1 2 Rice, Heather J.; Rubin, David C. (September 2011). "Remembering from any angle: The flexibility of visual perspective during retrieval". Consciousness and Cognition. 20 (3): 568–577. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.013. PMC   3085051 . PMID   21109466.
  24. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Nigro, Georgia; Neisser, Ulric (October 1983). "Point of view in personal memories". Cognitive Psychology. 15 (4): 467–482. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(83)90016-6. ISSN   0010-0285. S2CID   54382888.
  25. 1 2 3 Tagini, Angela; Raffone, Antonino (2009-09-11). "The 'I' and the 'Me' in self-referential awareness: a neurocognitive hypothesis". Cognitive Processing. 11 (1): 9–20. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0336-1. PMID   19763648. S2CID   12194649.
  26. Mace, John H.; Atkinson, Elizabeth; Moeckel, Christopher H.; Torres, Varinia (2011-01-10). "Accuracy and perspective in involuntary autobiographical memory". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 25 (1): 20–28. doi:10.1002/acp.1634.
  27. 1 2 Rubin, David (1995). Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 89–90. ISBN   9780521657235.
  28. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Sutin, Angelina R. (2008). "Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations" (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (4): 1060–1066. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.10.002.
  29. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Cohen, Dov; Gunz, Alex (2002-01-01). "As seen by the other...: Perspectives on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Westerners". Psychological Science. 13 (1): 55–59. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00409. PMID   11892778. S2CID   44783196.
  30. 1 2 3 Martin, Maryanne; Jones, Gregory V. (September 2012). "Individualism and the field viewpoint: Cultural influences on memory perspective". Consciousness and Cognition. 21 (3): 1498–1503. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.009. PMID   22673375. S2CID   22479760.
  31. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Huebner, David M.; Fredrickson, Barbara L. (September 1999). "Gender Differences in Memory Perspectives: Evidence for Self-Objectification in Women". Sex Roles. 41 (5/6): 459–467. doi:10.1023/A:1018831001880. S2CID   141065483.
  32. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Libby, Lisa K.; Eibach, Richard P. (February 2002). "Looking back in time: Self-concept change affects visual perspective in autobiographical memory". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (2): 167–179. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.167. PMID   11831407.
  33. 1 2 3 4 Libby, Lisa K.; Eibach, Richard P.; Gilovich, Thomas (January 2005). "Here's Looking at Me: The Effect of Memory Perspective on Assessments of Personal Change". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 88 (1): 50–62. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.323.9004 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.50. PMID   15631574.
  34. 1 2 3 4 5 McIsaac, Heather K.; Eich, Eric (April 2004). "Vantage Point in Traumatic Memory". Psychological Science. 15 (4): 248–253. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00660.x. PMID   15043642. S2CID   9134361.
  35. 1 2 3 4 Conway, Martin A.; Collins, Alan F.; Gathercole, Susan E.; Anderson, Stephen J. (1996). "Recollections of true and false autobiographical memories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 125 (1): 69–95. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.125.1.69. ISSN   1939-2222. S2CID   198671490. Lancaster University Library id:18990 ScienceOpen:f25fcb61-1ac7-464c-9db9-12cacb7ea590 CONROT-8 at PhilPapers.
  36. 1 2 3 4 5 Barclay, Craig R.; Wellman, Henry M. (February 1986). "Accuracies and inaccuracies in autobiographical memories" (PDF). Journal of Memory and Language. 25 (1): 93–103. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(86)90023-9. hdl: 2027.42/26271 .
  37. Galton, Francis (July 1879). "Psychometric experiments". Brain. 2 (2): 149–162. doi:10.1093/brain/2.2.149. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-002B-2CD1-7 .
  38. Rubin, David C., ed. (1986). Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511558313. ISBN   9780521368506.
  39. Zola-Morgan, Stuart; Cohen, Neal J.; Squire, Larry R. (1983). "Recall of remote episodic memory in amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 21 (5): 487–500. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.421.6932 . doi:10.1016/0028-3932(83)90005-2. PMID   6646401. S2CID   17209127.
  40. Platz, Friedrich; Kopiez, Reinhard; Hasselhorn, Johannes; Wolf, Anna (2015). "The impact of song-specific age and affective qualities of popular songs on music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs)". Musicae Scientiae. 19 (4): 327–349. doi:10.1177/1029864915597567. S2CID   148255269.
  41. Belfi, Amy M.; Bai, Elena; Stroud, Ava; Twohy, Raelynn; Beadle, Janelle N. (April 2022). "Investigating the role of involuntary retrieval in music-evoked autobiographical memories". Consciousness and Cognition. 100: 103305. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2022.103305. PMC   9059816 . PMID   35278896.
  42. Jakubowski, Kelly; Belfi, Amy M.; Eerola, Tuomas (2021-06-01). "Phenomenological Differences in Music- and Television-Evoked Autobiographical Memories". Music Perception. 38 (5): 435–455. doi: 10.1525/mp.2021.38.5.435 . ISSN   0730-7829. S2CID   236371564.
  43. Jakubowski, Kelly; Belfi, Amy M.; Kvavilashvili, Lia; Ely, Abbigail; Gill, Mark; Herbert, Gemma (2023-02-13). "Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study". British Journal of Psychology. 114 (3): 580–604. doi:10.1111/bjop.12639. ISSN   0007-1269. PMC   10363233 . PMID   36779290. S2CID   256826532.
  44. 1 2 Chu, Simon; Downes, John J. (June 2002). "Proust nose best: Odors are better cues of autobiographical memory". Memory and Cognition. 30 (4): 511–518. doi: 10.3758/bf03194952 . PMID   12184552.
  45. 1 2 3 D'Argembeau, Arnaud; Comblain, Christine; Van der Linden, Martial (2002-11-21). "Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for positive, negative, and neutral events". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 17 (3): 281–294. doi:10.1002/acp.856. hdl: 2268/1394 .
  46. 1 2 3 4 5 6 D'Argembeau, A.; Van der Linden, M. (2008). "Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory". Memory. 16 (5): 538–547. doi:10.1080/09658210802010463. hdl:2268/3574. PMID   18569682. S2CID   7123671.
  47. 1 2 3 Walker, W. Richard; Vogl, Rodney J.; Thompson, Charles P. (1999-01-06). "Autobiographical memory: Unpleasantness fades faster than pleasantness over time". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 11 (5): 399–413. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199710)11:5<399::aid-acp462>3.3.co;2-5.
  48. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Walker, W. Richard; Skowronski, John; Gibbons, Jeffrey; Vogl, Rodney; Thompson, Charles (2003). "On the emotions that accompany autobiographical memories: Dysphoria disrupts the fading affect bias". Cognition & Emotion. 17 (5): 703–723. doi:10.1080/02699930302287. S2CID   145630088.
  49. Watkins, P.C.; Vache, K.; Vernay, S.P.; Muller, S. (February 1996). "Unconscious mood-congruent memory bias in depression". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 105 (1): 34–41. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.105.1.34. PMID   8666709.
  50. 1 2 Kuyken, William; Howell, Rachael (2006-04-01). "Facets of autobiographical memory in adolescents with major depressive disorder and never-depressed controls". Cognition & Emotion. 20 (3): 466–487. doi:10.1080/02699930500342639. PMID   26529216. S2CID   25421196.
  51. 1 2 3 Bergouignan, Loretxu; Lemogne, Cédric; Foucher, Aurélie; Longin, Estelle; Vistoli, Damien; Allilaire, Jean-François; Fossati, Philippe (2008). "Field perspective deficit for positive memories characterizes autobiographical memory in euthymic depressed patients". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 46 (3): 322–333. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.12.007. PMID   18243159.
  52. 1 2 3 4 Lemogne, Cédric; Piolino, Pascale; Friszer, Stéphanie; Claret, Astrid; Girault, Nathalie; Jouvent, Roland; Allilaire, Jean-François; Fossati, Philippe (June 2006). "Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: Specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self perspective". Consciousness and Cognition. 15 (2): 258–268. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.07.005. PMID   16154765. S2CID   39929699.
  53. Bergouignan, Loretxu; Lefranc, Jean Pierre; Chupin, Marie; Morel, Nastassja; Spano, Jean Philippe; Fossati, Philippe (2011-10-10). Mattson, Mark P. (ed.). "Breast Cancer Affects Both the Hippocampus Volume and the Episodic Autobiographical Memory Retrieval". PLOS ONE. 6 (10): e25349. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...625349B. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025349 . PMC   3189914 . PMID   22016764.
  54. 1 2 Rubin, David C.; Schulkind, Mathew D. (November 1997). "The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan". Memory & Cognition. 25 (6): 859–866. doi: 10.3758/bf03211330 . hdl: 10161/10156 . PMID   9421572.
  55. 1 2 3 Piolino, Pascale; Desgranges, Béatrice; Benali, Karim; Eustache, Francis (July 2002). "Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing". Memory. 10 (4): 239–257. doi:10.1080/09658210143000353. PMID   12097209. S2CID   33870697.
  56. Levine, Brian; Svoboda, Eva; Hay, Janine F.; Winocur, Gordon; Moscovitch, Morris (December 2002). "Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval". Psychology and Aging. 17 (4): 677–689. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677. PMID   12507363.
  57. 1 2 Schlagman, S.; Kliegel, M.; Schulz, J.; Kvavilashvili, L. (2009). "Differential effects of age on involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memory". Psychology and Aging. 24 (2): 397–411. doi:10.1037/a0015785. hdl: 2299/9705 . PMID   19485657.
  58. Berntsen, Dorthe; Rubin, David C. (2002). "Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories". Psychology and Aging. 17 (4): 636–652. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.636. PMID   12507360.
  59. Mather, Mara; Carstensen, Laura L. (2005). "Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (10): 496–502. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005. PMID   16154382. S2CID   17433910.
  60. 1 2 Rubin, David C.; Schrauf, Robert W.; Greenberg, Daniel L. (September 2003). "Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories". Memory and Cognition. 31 (6): 887–901. doi: 10.3758/bf03196443 . hdl: 10161/10117 . PMID   14651297.
  61. Talarico, Jennifer M.; Rubin, David C. (September 2003). "Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories". Psychological Science. 14 (5): 455–461. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02453. hdl: 10161/10118 . PMID   12930476. S2CID   14643427.
  62. Heaps, C. M.; Nash, M. (July 2001). "Comparing recollective experience in true and false autobiographical memories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (4): 920–930. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.4.920. PMID   11486925.
  63. Boakes, Janet (1995-10-21). "False memory syndrome". The Lancet. 346 (8982). London: 1048–1049. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(95)91736-5. PMID   7564781. S2CID   38462342.
  64. Johnson, M. K.; Raye, C. L. (1998). "False memories and confabulation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (4): 137–145. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01152-8. PMID   21227110. S2CID   10033853.
  65. 1 2 3 4 Conway, Martin A.; Pleydell-Pearce, Christopher W.; Whitecross, Sharron E. (October 2001). "The neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a slow cortical potential study of autobiographical memory retrieval". Journal of Memory and Language. 45 (3): 493–524. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2781.
  66. 1 2 3 Svoboda, Eva; McKinnon, Margaret C.; Levine, Brian (2006). "The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis". Neuropsychologia. 44 (12): 2189–2208. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023. PMC   1995661 . PMID   16806314.