Geoffrey B Saxe

Last updated
Geoffrey B Saxe
Geoffrey Saxe photo.jpg
Saxe in 2019
Born1948 (age 7576)
Awards2015 Eleanor E. Maccoby Book Award American Psychological Association

2014 Stirling Prize for Best Published Book in Psychological Anthropology. American Anthropological Association

Contents

2013 Best Authored Book Award from the Cognitive Development Society

2010, 2022 Presidential Citations from the American Educational Research Association
Academic background
Alma mater
Website https://culturecognition.com

Geoffrey B. Saxe (born June 12, 1948) is an American developmental psychologist. He is a Distinguished Professor of the Graduate School in the School of Education at UC Berkeley. He is a former President of the Jean Piaget Society for the Study of Knowledge and Development, elected member of the National Academy of Education, elected fellow of the American Educational Research Association, and former Editor-in-Chief of the international journal, Human Development.

Saxe's research focuses on the interplay between cultural and cognitive developmental processes in the reproduction and alteration of ideas in human communities. Using mathematics as an illustrative domain, Saxe situates his work in the collective practices of daily life. [1] He has also extended his developmental perspective to a program of design research in mathematics education. [2]

Education and early life

Saxe grew up in Los Angeles, attending public schools along with his sister. His parents, the children of East European immigrants, became psychotherapists.

Saxe graduated from the University of California, Berkeley with a B.A. in psychology in 1970, and again, from the same institution and department, with a PhD in 1975 under the mentorship of Professor Jonas Langer. His training focused on cognitive development.  Influenced by seminal developmental scholars including Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky, and Heinz Werner, Saxe used numerical cognition to explore issues of cognitive development in children's thinking. Subsequent to graduate studies, Saxe spent two years as a postdoctoral fellow at Children's Hospital in Boston at Harvard Medical School under the sponsorship of Peter H. Wolff as well as Harvard's Project Zero and the Boston Veteran Administration hospital with Howard Gardner as his advisor. During this postdoctoral period, he extended his training in cognitive development to atypical development and the breakdown of cognitive functioning following brain injury, again with a focus on numerical cognition as an arena for analysis.

Research and writing career

Saxe's faculty appointments began in 1977, first at the Graduate Center of the City University of New York in the PhD Program in Educational Psychology. Later in 1982, he joined the faculty at UCLA, and still later, in 1997, joined the faculty at UC Berkeley as a professor in the Human Development Program of the Graduate School of Education.

Numerical Thought and its Development in Collective Practices of Everyday Life

Saxe's program of work has contributed to our understanding of the development numerical thought in different cultural communities. In a 1987 monograph, [3] he and colleagues conducted a range of studies on the home environments and adult-child interactions in middle- and working- class communities. In home settings, they found that young children are inadvertently and sometimes purposely engaged with numerical activities by parents. They showed that not only do children adjust their activities involving number to home supports, often unwittingly, parents adjust their interactions with children in relations to their children's developing understandings.  Hence children's early numerical environments constructed are dynamic and interactive, a process that supports children's acquisition of cultural forms for number representation, like counting systems, and increasingly use these forms to serve increasingly complex functions (e.g., counting to compare groups, elementary arithmetical transformations.)

In a related program of work, Saxe investigated the mathematical understandings of unschooled and schooled children who sell candy in the streets in NE Brazil during periods of a rapidly inflating economy. [4] Through a coordination of ethnographic/observational research and quasi-experimental designs, his research revealed that largely unschooled sellers, as they plied their trade, create a distinct mathematics that is unlike school math, one rooted in different kinds of representations and ideas. Further, Saxe showed that sellers outstripped same-age schooled non-sellers in knowledge linked to their practice, like comparing ratios, and that when sellers did attend school, they made use of their out-of-school knowledge to support their understanding of school-linked math instruction.

Saxe's long-term field research project was conducted over more than a 30-year stretch of time with a remote Papua New Guinea group, the Oksapmin, who traditionally use a 27-body part counting system. The work formed the empirical basis for his book, Cultural Development of Mathematical Ideas. [5] Through field visits (1978, 1980, 2001, 2014), he made use of archival, ethnographic, interview methods coupled with quasi-experimental designs in order to trace the interplay between historical change in the everyday numerical problems that emerged in collective practices and shifts in forms of the body system and functions that it served in daily life.

Design Research

In a second arena of research, Saxe's focus, together with colleagues, was to support children's understandings of integers and fractions, important but difficult-to-learn and hard-to-teach ideas. Through a coordination of interview, tutorial, and classroom studies. [2] informed by prior work on collective practices and children's mathematical thinking, Saxe and colleagues developed a curriculum unit that uses the number line as a central representational context. An experimental study revealed effects for children's learning who participated in LMR classrooms, [6] and that the curriculum provided supports for English language learners who participated in the same classrooms as their native-English speaking peers.[9]

Honors and research awards

Saxe's theoretical and research work received awards from diverse behavioral science disciplines. In 2013, the Cognitive Development Society awarded his book, Cultural Development of Mathematical Ideas: Papua New Guinea Studies, [5] the best book award. The American Anthropological Association in 2014 awarded the same book the Stirling Prize for Best Published Book in Psychological Anthropology, [7] and the American Psychological Association in 2015 awarded the same book the Eleanor E. Maccoby Book Award. [8] In addition, for his theoretical and empirical work he received two Presidential Citations from the American Educational Research Association, [9] the first in 2010 from President Carol D. Lee and the second, in 2022, from President Na'ilah Nasir. He has received numerous grants to support his work, including awards from the National Science Foundation, National Institutes of Mental Health, National Institutes of Education, the Fulbright organization, and private foundations, including the Spencer Foundation and Sigma Xi. He was an invited Fellow in residence at both Stanford's Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences (2003–2004), and the Rockefeller Foundation's Bellagio center (2006). He also participated as an invited senior scholar-in-residence at the Max Planck Institute for the History of Science, Berlin, Germany (1999), and an invited fellow-in-residence at the Center for Interdisciplinary Studies (CIRADE), Université du Québec à Montréal (1995).

Reviews

Saxe's 1990 book, Culture and Cognitive Development, and Saxe's 2012 book, Cultural Development of Mathematical Ideas, each received extended published reviews in major journals in fields of developmental psychology, anthropology, and education. His work in mathematics education has also received published journal reviews.

In reviewing Saxe's book, Culture and Cognitive Development, James Hiebert, from the University of Delaware writes in the journal Contemporary Psychology, "The book is pioneering it its conceptualization of the cognition-culture interface and exemplary in its presentation of informative data." [10] In a similar vein, Roy Pea, Stanford University, in a review published in the Educational Researcher, points to the importance of the book, "[Saxe's] main achievement is the construction and exemplification of a constructivist research framework... beyond the works of Piaget or Vygotsky...— a level where culture and cognition are constitutive of one another.... Most importantly, one can imagine from the detailed example he offers how one might apply the theoretical and research framework to culture-cognition relations for other content domains." [11] Finally, Alan Bishop of Monash University published a review in Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, where he points to the import of the book from the point of view of mathematics education when he writes, "this book could well achieve "cult" status. It is a seductive book that will appeal to many readers at different levels...It is a fascinating and rare combination of psychological and ethnographic inquiry styles....As Chevallard (1990) says, "The problem of mathematics education and culture should be everyone's concern", and Saxe's book demonstrates convincingly the truth of that statement."[12] [12]

Saxe's 2012 book, Cultural Development of Mathematical Ideas also received published reviews in journals that cut across disciplines. Professor Joseph Glick, a developmental psychologist from the Graduate Center of the City University of New York writes in the journal Human Development about the book's theoretical contribution, "This book is a tour de force in moving theory toward a fully historical, social and activity-based account of cultural and developmental change. The case presented is particular to an "exotic" (to us) group, but the theoretical apparatus developed has wide-ranging application." [13]   Professor Ellice Forman, from the University of Pittsburgh writes in Mathematical Thinking and Learning, "Cultural anthropologist Clifford Geertz proposed that "small facts speak to large issues" (1973, p. 23). In many ways, Geoffrey Saxe's new book echoes the Geertz quote. ... Saxe has assembled a dynamic documentary report of answers to the question: How are culture and cognition related? ... I would like to add that this important book provides a window into the professional career of a remarkable researcher and scholar." [14] Professor Anna Sfard, a scholar from the University of Haifa, writes in Educational Studies in Mathematics, "The main contribution of the Oksapmin study... is in the opportunity it creates for tackling the question of how it happens that individual "cognitive leaps" propagate in space and time, amounting to cultural changes, visible on the societal-historical scale. This question is of paramount importance... I can see at least three reasons why this book is a must read for every mathematics education researcher. First, as an inexhaustible source of insights about numerical thinking and its development and also about human thinking in general, it can be trusted to provide the interested reader with a hearty serving of food for thought. Second, it is not every day that one sees a longitudinal study capturing a historical change in the making. This turns Saxe's story into something truly unique and precious. Finally, these 330 pages of the well-written text are too enjoyable a read to be missed." [15] Finally, Professor Ashley Maynard, a psychologist from the University of Hawaii praises Saxe's theoretical and research contributions in the Journal of Cognition and Development, "Saxe is a master of theory and method in human development, and he provides a model for how to conduct contextually grounded, theoretically useful, path-breaking research that moves the field forward." [16]

Saxe's work in mathematics education has received published commentaries. For example, Richard Lehrer from Vanderbilt University wrote about Saxe's article analyzing classroom studies of the LMR curriculum, [6] "this research stands as a landmark of productive simplifications of complex forms of interaction in a way that is tractable for both design and analysis. As in any good design, its elegance belies its difficulty." [17]

Selected publications

Related Research Articles

Educational psychology is the branch of psychology concerned with the scientific study of human learning. The study of learning processes, from both cognitive and behavioral perspectives, allows researchers to understand individual differences in intelligence, cognitive development, affect, motivation, self-regulation, and self-concept, as well as their role in learning. The field of educational psychology relies heavily on quantitative methods, including testing and measurement, to enhance educational activities related to instructional design, classroom management, and assessment, which serve to facilitate learning processes in various educational settings across the lifespan.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lev Vygotsky</span> Soviet psychologist (1896–1934)

Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky was a Russian and Soviet psychologist, best known for his work on psychological development in children and creating the framework known as cultural-historical activity theory. After his early death, his books and research were banned in the Soviet Union until Joseph Stalin's death in 1953, with a first collection of major texts published in 1956.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jean Piaget</span> Swiss psychologist, biologist, logician, philosopher and academic (1896–1980)

Jean William Fritz Piaget was a Swiss psychologist known for his work on child development. Piaget's theory of cognitive development and epistemological view are together called genetic epistemology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jerome Bruner</span> American psychologist and scholar

Jerome Seymour Bruner was an American psychologist who made significant contributions to human cognitive psychology and cognitive learning theory in educational psychology. Bruner was a senior research fellow at the New York University School of Law. He received a BA in 1937 from Duke University and a PhD from Harvard University in 1941. He taught and did research at Harvard University, the University of Oxford, and New York University. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Bruner as the 28th most cited psychologist of the 20th century.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Constructivism (philosophy of education)</span> Philosophical viewpoint about the nature of knowledge; theory of knowledge

Constructivism is a theory in education which posits that individuals or learners do not acquire knowledge and understanding by passively perceiving it within a direct process of knowledge transmission, rather they construct new understandings and knowledge through experience and social discourse, integrating new information with what they already know. For children, this includes knowledge gained prior to entering school. It is associated with various philosophical positions, particularly in epistemology as well as ontology, politics, and ethics. The origin of the theory is also linked to Swiss developmental psychologist Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development.

Cognitive development is a field of study in neuroscience and psychology focusing on a child's development in terms of information processing, conceptual resources, perceptual skill, language learning, and other aspects of the developed adult brain and cognitive psychology. Qualitative differences between how a child processes their waking experience and how an adult processes their waking experience are acknowledged. Cognitive development is defined as the emergence of the ability to consciously cognize, understand, and articulate their understanding in adult terms. Cognitive development is how a person perceives, thinks, and gains understanding of their world through the relations of genetic and learning factors. There are four stages to cognitive information development. They are, reasoning, intelligence, language, and memory. These stages start when the baby is about 18 months old, they play with toys, listen to their parents speak, they watch TV, anything that catches their attention helps build their cognitive development.

Group cognition is a social, largely linguistic phenomenon whereby a group of people produce a sequence of utterances that performs a cognitive act. That is, if a similar sequence was uttered or thought by an individual it would be considered an act of cognition or thinking. The group can be a small group, such as 3–5 people talking together or working together online. The group can also be a larger collective, such as a classroom of students or a global community contributing asynchronously to an extended discourse on a problem or topic or to a knowledge repository like Wikipedia. The theory of group cognition is a postcognitivism philosophy, which considers a larger unit of analysis than an individual mind as a producer of cognitive activities such as creative problem solving.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Oksapmin language</span> Trans–New Guinea language spoken in Papua New Guinea

Oksapmin is a Trans–New Guinea language spoken in Oksapmin Rural LLG, Telefomin District, Sandaun, Papua New Guinea. The two principal dialects are distinct enough to cause some problems with mutual intelligibility.

Evolutionary educational psychology is the study of the relation between inherent folk knowledge and abilities and accompanying inferential and attributional biases as these influence academic learning in evolutionarily novel cultural contexts, such as schools and the industrial workplace. The fundamental premises and principles of this discipline are presented below.

Susan E. Carey is an American psychologist who is a Professor of Psychology at Harvard University. She studies language acquisition, children's development of concepts, conceptual changes over time, and the importance of executive functions. She has conducted experiments on infants, toddlers, adults, and non-human primates. Her books include Conceptual Change in Childhood (1985) and The Origin of Concepts (2009).

Some of the research that is conducted in the field of psychology is more "fundamental" than the research conducted in the applied psychological disciplines, and does not necessarily have a direct application. The subdisciplines within psychology that can be thought to reflect a basic-science orientation include biological psychology, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology, and so on. Research in these subdisciplines is characterized by methodological rigor. The concern of psychology as a basic science is in understanding the laws and processes that underlie behavior, cognition, and emotion. Psychology as a basic science provides a foundation for applied psychology. Applied psychology, by contrast, involves the application of psychological principles and theories yielded up by the basic psychological sciences; these applications are aimed at overcoming problems or promoting well-being in areas such as mental and physical health and education.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Andreas Demetriou</span> Greek Cypriot developmental psychologist (born 1950)

Andreas Demetriou is a Greek Cypriot developmental psychologist and former Minister of Education and Culture of Cyprus. He is a founding fellow and president of The Cyprus Academy of Sciences, Letters and Arts.

Educational neuroscience is an emerging scientific field that brings together researchers in cognitive neuroscience, developmental cognitive neuroscience, educational psychology, educational technology, education theory and other related disciplines to explore the interactions between biological processes and education. Researchers in educational neuroscience investigate the neural mechanisms of reading, numerical cognition, attention and their attendant difficulties including dyslexia, dyscalculia and ADHD as they relate to education. Researchers in this area may link basic findings in cognitive neuroscience with educational technology to help in curriculum implementation for mathematics education and reading education. The aim of educational neuroscience is to generate basic and applied research that will provide a new transdisciplinary account of learning and teaching, which is capable of informing education. A major goal of educational neuroscience is to bridge the gap between the two fields through a direct dialogue between researchers and educators, avoiding the "middlemen of the brain-based learning industry". These middlemen have a vested commercial interest in the selling of "neuromyths" and their supposed remedies.

Telefomin District is a district of Sandaun Province of Papua New Guinea. Its capital is Telefomin. Its inhabitants include the Mountain Ok people, a cultural group with numerous sub-groups including the Telefol, the Urapmin, and the Wopkaimin. The Oksapmin also live in this district. Notable ethnographic research by Geoffrey B. Saxe at UC Berkeley has documented the encounter between pre-contact uses of number and its cultural evolution under conditions of monetization and exposure to schooling and the formal economy among the Oksapmin.

Conceptual change is the process whereby concepts and relationships between them change over the course of an individual person's lifetime or over the course of history. Research in four different fields – cognitive psychology, cognitive developmental psychology, science education, and history and philosophy of science - has sought to understand this process. Indeed, the convergence of these four fields, in their effort to understand how concepts change in content and organization, has led to the emergence of an interdisciplinary sub-field in its own right. This sub-field is referred to as "conceptual change" research.

Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC) is a social science laboratory located at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) since 1978. Scholars at LCHC pursue research focused on understanding the complex relationship between cognition and culture in individual and social development. Such research requires collaboration among scholars from a variety of research disciplines, including cognitive science, education, linguistics, psychology, anthropology, and sociology. LCHC also functions as a research and training institution, arranging for pre-doctoral, doctoral, and post-doctoral training, as well as research exchanges with scholars throughout the world. In addition, LCHC sponsors a journal, Mind, Culture and Activity: An International Journal (MCA), and an open internet discussion group, XMCA.

Kelly S. Mix is an American developmental psychologist known for her research on the development of numerical concepts and their origins in infancy and toddlerhood. She is professor and chair of the Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology at the University of Maryland. Mix was awarded the Boyd McCandless Early Career Award in 2002 for her innovative research on the early emergence of numerocity. Her co-authored book Quantitative Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, with Janellen Huttenlocher and Susan Cohen Levine, provides an overview of the early development of quantitative reasoning and mathematical concepts. Her co-edited book The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition, with Linda B. Smith and Michael Gasser, examines the role of space in structuring human cognition.

Some languages of the world have numeral systems that do not make use of an arithmetic base. One such system is the body-part counting system which make use of further body parts to extend the system beyond the ten fingers.

Arthur "Art" J. Baroody is an educational psychologist, academic, and an expert in mathematics education research. He is a Professor Emeritus of Curriculum and Instruction at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, and a Senior Research Fellow in Morgridge College of Education (COE) at the University of Denver.

Vera (Veronka) John-Steiner (1930–2017) was a Hungarian-American educational psychologist and activist. She was known for her work on creative collaboration, and her contributions to psycholinguistics, cultural-historical activity theory, cross-cultural education, bilingualism, psychology of women, and cognitive psychology. Her theoretical framework was heavily influenced by the work of Lev Vygotsky. In 2007, the American Educational Research Association honored her with its Lifetime Achievement Award.

References

  1. Saxe, Geoffrey B. (1994). "Studying cognitive development in sociocultural context: The development of a practice-based approach". Mind, Culture, and Activity. 1 (3): 135–157. doi:10.1080/10749039409524666. ISSN   1074-9039.
  2. 1 2 Saxe, Geoffrey; de Kirby, Kenton; Le, Marie; Sitabkhan, Yasmin; Kang, Bona (2015). "Understanding learning across lessons in classroom communities: A multi-leveled analytic approach". In A. Bikner-Ahsbahs; C. Knipping; NC. Presmeg (eds.). Approaches to Qualitative Research in Mathematics Education. Springer. pp. 253–318. ISBN   978-94-017-9180-9.
  3. Saxe, Geoffrey B.; Guberman, Steven; Gearhart, Maryl (1987). "Social processes in early number development". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 52 (2). doi:10.2307/1166071. JSTOR   1166071.
  4. Saxe, Geoffrey B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ISBN   9781315788968.
  5. 1 2 Saxe, Geoffrey B. (2012). Cultural development of mathematical ideas: Papua New Guinea studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   9780521761666.
  6. 1 2 Saxe, Geoffrey B.; Diakow, Ronli; Gearhart, Maryl (2013). "Towards curricular coherence in integers and fractions: The efficacy of a lesson sequence that uses the number line as the principal representational context". ZDM. 45: 343–364. doi:10.1007/s11858-012-0466-2. S2CID   255427587 via Springer.
  7. https://spa.americananthro.org/prizes/stirling-award/
  8. https://www.apadivisions.org/division-7/awards/book#?tab=4
  9. https://www.aera.net/About-AERA/Awards/Presidential-Citation
  10. Hiebert, James (1992). "Individual and situated cognition: An integrative perspective". Contemporary Psychology. 37 (6): 580–581. doi:10.1037/032241.
  11. Pea, Roy (1990). "Inspecting everyday mathematics: Reexamining culture-cognition relations". Educational Researcher. 19 (4): 28–31 via Sage.
  12. Bishop, Alan J. (1991). "Toward a cultural psychology of mathematics". Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 22 (1): 76–80.
  13. Glick, Joseph (2013). "Cultural-historical analysis done right: Review of Cultural Development of Mathematical Ideas". Human Development. 56: 213–216. doi:10.1159/000348338. S2CID   55228161.
  14. Forman, Ellice A. (2014). "Cultural development of mathematical ideas: Papua New Guinea Studies, by Geoffrey Saxe". Mathematical Thinking and Learning. 16 (2): 175–180. doi:10.1080/10986065.2014.893718. S2CID   145080869.
  15. Sfard, Anna (2015). "Book Review: When Oksapmin number system met Western arithmetic - Answering the question of how cognitive and cultural change induce each other". Educational Studies in Mathematics. 88 (2): 283–290. doi:10.1007/s10649-014-9586-7. S2CID   254471610 via Springer.
  16. Maynard, Ashley E. (2014). "Review: Cultural Development of mathematical ideas: Papua New Guinea Studies". Journal of Cognition and Development. 15 (3): 524–525. doi:10.1080/15248372.2014.933390. S2CID   143007926.
  17. Lehrer, Richard (2015). "Designing for development". Human Development. 58: 45–49. doi:10.1159/000373867.