Neural binding

Last updated
Neural binding involves the complex coordination of diverse neural circuits Forest of synthetic pyramidal dendrites grown using Cajal's laws of neuronal branching.png
Neural binding involves the complex coordination of diverse neural circuits

Neural binding is the neuroscientific aspect of what is commonly known as the binding problem: the interdisciplinary difficulty of creating a comprehensive and verifiable model for the unity of consciousness. "Binding" refers to the integration of highly diverse neural information in the forming of one's cohesive experience. The neural binding hypothesis states that neural signals are paired through synchronized oscillations of neuronal activity that combine and recombine to allow for a wide variety of responses to context-dependent stimuli. These dynamic neural networks are thought to account for the flexibility and nuanced response of the brain to various situations. [1] The coupling of these networks is transient, on the order of milliseconds, and allows for rapid activity. [2]

Contents

A viable mechanism for this phenomenon must address (1) the difficulties of reconciling the global nature of the participating (exogenous) signals and their relevant (endogenous) associations, (2) the interface between lower perceptual processes and higher cognitive processes, (3) the identification of signals (sometimes referred to as “tagging”) as they are processed and routed throughout the brain, and (4) the emergence of a unity of consciousness.

Proposed adaptive functions of neural binding have included the avoidance of hallucinatory phenomena generated by endogenous patterns alone as well as the avoidance of behavior driven by involuntary action alone. [3]

There are several difficulties that must be addressed in this model. First, it must provide a mechanism for the integration of signals across different brain regions (both cortical and subcortical). It must also be able to explain the simultaneous processing of unrelated signals that are held separate from one another and integrated signals that must be viewed as a whole. [1]

Interdisciplinary correlates

The study of the binding problem in neuroscience stems from the much older psychological study of the binding phenomenon, which has its roots in the ancient philosophical study of the same problems. Today, there is a close interplay between neuroscience and psychology, which is especially relevant to neural binding.

Gestalt psychology and correlating critiques

Representation of the multiple ways to perceive the same object Invariance.jpg
Representation of the multiple ways to perceive the same object

Gestalt psychology is a field that has been intertwined with neural binding due to its advances in conceptualizing how the brain views objects, reacts to stimuli, and then reacts on a global level. Since Gestalt Psychology has become so involved, “grouping” has become an important part of understanding neural binding. The scientists that supported this theory were solely focused on the idea that mechanisms in the brain operated preattentively.

"Grouping" has become especially important for neural binding due to studies in gamma wave activity in the brain. From single-unit recording scientists have been able to measure the oscillatory neuronal activity and have been able to better their understanding of the grouping or pairing of neurons. Thus, these pairings connect with Gestalt psychology and its theory that objects are seen independently of their separate pieces, in a more global manner. A study done by Gray, Konig, Engel and Singer in 1989 reported that when studying visual cortex cells, light moving in opposite directions had low relation to one another. [4] Yet when the light rays were then pointed in the same direction, the correlative relationship was stronger. A paper addressing this idea states, "Thus, correlated activity was only obtained when cells responded to different aspects of the same 'Gestalt'". Other studies using EEGs from the human scalp also examined gamma wave activity, showing how stimuli seemed to be in accordance with preattentive binding. [5]

By contrast with this, another group of scientists (Treisman & Gelade) believed that the mechanisms did not work preattentively. They postulated that in order to make correct bindings there had to be attention. Hence, they called their theory "attentional binding." The idea of attentional binding is that, as someone places their attention on an object with multiple features, they develop over time a coherent representation of that object and the characteristics that it holds. These characteristics, after being processed through attention, can be stored in short-term memory or as episodic memories. [5]

One more contrast to Gestalt psychological emphasis in neural binding was brought up by L. Chen. Chen asserts that the local-global idea of Gestalt does not suffice in explaining neural processing. Instead, Chen uses the idea that perception depends on "topological invariants that describe the geometrical potentiality of the entire stimulus configuration". The idea here is that looking at a certain object creates a stimulus which in turn fires a constellation of that object, and then the object becomes recognized. [5]

The rebuttal to Chen's idea is that since each object has its own constellation, as well as its own particular set of neurons, there would not be enough space in the brain for all the appropriate neuronal pathways. This ratio of 1:1 simply could not support the many complex mechanisms of the human brain.

Later, more writings were produced regarding preattentive binding. Although they did not focus as much on Gestalt psychology, they were important to the idea of the preattentiveness of the brain. One such addition onto this theory was put out by Duncan and Humphreys. They said that some types of features, when trying to form the image, compete against one another for "the limited capacity object recognition stage". This means that certain parts of an object have multiple ways of being featured by the brain. Each different type of feature then competes with the others, trying to become the feature that is displayed to the individual. This can then account for the understanding of how each person views a whole object differently than another person. They also then see its global function on a very different scale. [5]

This idea of differential grouping is also brought up by Aksentijevic, Elliott, and Barber. Their theory is based on different perceptions of geometrical space that can then supply a "starting point for a systematic exploration of the subjective properties of certain classes of visual and auditory grouping phenomena, such as apparent motion, grouping within static two-dimensional displays and auditory streaming." This idea shows how different people perceive geometrical space, which can lead to different auditory and visual streams. [5]

Proposed models

Temporal

Some trajectories of a harmonic oscillator. The Temporal Binding Hypothesis and other areas of neural binding use this idea to describe the pairing of two oscillating waves, forming an image. QuantumHarmonicOscillatorAnimation.gif
Some trajectories of a harmonic oscillator. The Temporal Binding Hypothesis and other areas of neural binding use this idea to describe the pairing of two oscillating waves, forming an image.

The Temporal Binding Hypothesis has a history in neuroscience which spans back to at least the early 1980s. Christoph Von der Malsburg proposed that neurons carry two distinct signals, one of conventional rate code in relation to the ‘effectiveness’ of the feature it is encoding and another which is conveyed in terms of spike coordination between assemblies of neurons. [6] A decade later Francis Crick & Christof Koch developed the model further by explaining the synchronization of distant neurons by transient gamma wave oscillations being guided by attention. [7] By 1995, Francisco Varela had established the importance of the role of the oscillatory phase in the pairing of neuron assemblies. [8]

The Temporal Binding Hypothesis was first proposed as a way of addressing the combinatorial problem of neural binding, which emphasizes the impossibly large number of connections that would have to be made between neurons of overlapping function and association in order to achieve the degree of observable complexity in neural binding. Modern models that are currently being developed often retain the structure of temporal synchrony due to this significant advantage as well as the strong experimental support for the existence of global brain oscillations.

Gamma band activity

Gamma waves recorded from an EEG Eeg gamma.svg
Gamma waves recorded from an EEG

It has been widely observed that distributed collections of neurons oscillate within the gamma band of frequencies (typically between 30–80 Hz)..

Phasic pairing mechanisms

Varela’s model describes the selection of cellular assemblies, sometimes referred to as resonant or reverberating (Lorente de Nó) circuits, as being integrated into a larger ensemble by transient spike phase locking. This addresses the explanatory difficulty of the simultaneous nature of binding because it allows many signals to be in transit in parallel within an overlapping frequency range without interference.

Criticism

The temporal binding models have been criticized by some for being unwarranted in their far-reaching speculative nature. [9] This claim is usually made in favor of a more classical model of neural function.

Quantum models

The NMDA receptor acts as a coincidence detector that requires the binding of multiple ligands (e.g., glutamate and glycine, as depicted here) for activation. Activated NMDAR.svg
The NMDA receptor acts as a coincidence detector that requires the binding of multiple ligands (e.g., glutamate and glycine, as depicted here) for activation.

Some more recent models have incorporated quantum theory as a component of the underlying neural dynamics behind binding due to the strong explanatory power of quantum mechanics and their claim of the insufficiency of current models for explaining the complexity of the binding mechanism.

In 2001, da Rocha developed a model which utilized the basic structure of the Temporal Binding Hypothesis, with global phase synchronization being a primary mechanism of distributing information, but emphasized the importance of a quantum computation performed on a local level. [3] His model depicts the NMDA receptor as the coincidence detector proposed in other theories. This is a physiological mechanism which has been criticized for being absent from a full theory of temporal binding previously. [9]

Catecholaminergic Neuron Electron Transport (CNET)

A neural signaling mechanism in certain catecholaminergic neurons has been hypothesized that uses quantum mechanical electron transport (Catecholaminergic Neuron Electron Transport, or CNET). [10] [11] [12] The hypothesis is based in part on the observation by many independent researchers that electron tunneling occurs in ferritin (an iron storage protein that is prevalent in those neurons) at room temperature and ambient conditions. [13] [14] [15] [16] The function of this mechanism would be to assist in action selection, but the mechanism itself would be capable of integrating millions of cognitive and sensory neural signals, using a physical mechanism associated with strong electron-electron interactions.  The hypothesis predicted a number of physical properties of these neurons that have been subsequently observed experimentally, such as electron tunneling in substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) tissue and the presence of disordered arrays of ferritin in SNc tissue. [17] [18] [19] The hypothesis also predicted that disordered ferritin arrays like those found in SNc tissue should be capable of supporting long-range electron transport and providing a switching or routing function, both of which have been subsequently observed. [20] [21] [22]

CNET also predicted that the largest SNc neurons should mediate action selection. This prediction was contrary to earlier proposals about the function of those neurons that were based on predictive reward dopamine signaling. [23] [24] However, a team led by Dr. Pascal Kaiser of Harvard Medical School subsequently demonstrated that those neurons code movement, consistent with the earlier predictions of CNET. [25] While the CNET mechanism has not yet been directly observed, it may be possible to do so using quantum dot fluorophores tagged to ferritin or other analytical techniques for detecting electron tunneling. [26]

Non-synchronous models

Many non-temporal theories attempt to retain a more classical conception of neuron signaling based on hierarchical structures. This relies on the convergence of signals upon specific neural circuits as well as the dynamic routing of signaling pathways through specialized computational sub-assemblies.

These models can be vulnerable to the combinatorial and connectivity problems, which come as a natural consequence of depending solely on one-to-one communication of conventional information via spike rate coding. These models also require the seamless directing and combining of signals, which either assumes a controller (proposed to be the pulvinar nuclei [27] ) or a mechanism for the spontaneous self-assembly of neural ensembles.

Test cases

Sensory

Much of the experimental evidence for neural binding has traditionally revolved around sensory awareness. Sensory awareness is accomplished by integrating things together by cognitively perceiving them and then segmenting them so that, in total, there is an image created. Since there can be an infinite number of possibilities in the perception of an object, this has been a unique area of study. The way the brain then collectively pieces certain things together via networking is important not only in the global way of perceiving but also in segmentation. Much of sensory awareness has to do with the taking of a single piece of an object's makeup and then binding its total characteristics so that the brain perceives the object in its final form.

Much of the research for the understanding of segmentation and how the brain perceives an object has been done by studying cats. A major finding of this research has to do with the understanding of gamma waves oscillating at 40 Hz. The information was extracted from a study using the cat visual cortex. It was shown that the cortical neurons responded differently to spatially different objects. These firings of neurons ranged from 40–60 Hz in measure and when observed showed that they fired synchronously when observing different parts of the object. Such coherent responses point to the fact that the brain is doing a kind of coding where it is piecing certain neurons together in the works of making the form of an object. Since the brain is putting these segmented pieces together unsupervised, a significant consonance is found with many philosophers (like Sigmund Freud) who theorize an underlying subconscious that helps to form every aspect of our conscious thought processes. [28]

In order for these multiple firings from multiple areas to be combined into a specific extrinsic event, there must be help from the dorsal thalamus. It is not proven whether the dorsal thalamus is the primary organizer, but it does fit the specific profile for collecting neuronal activity and rapidly coordinating between what is happening in the brain and outside of it. The space in and around the dorsal thalamus, the thalomocortical area, is able to generate fast voltage-dependent membrane potential oscillations which allow it to react quickly to received messages. The types of channels that cover this area are presumed to be GABAergic. Since sensory awareness needs to be quick, the threshold for sodium and potassium in this area is quite low. [28]

Cognitive

Francis Crick and Christof Koch proposed that specific neural activity can stimulate short-term memory, forming a continuous and dynamic consciousness.

Cognitive binding is associated with the different states of human consciousness. Two of the most studied states of consciousness are the wakefulness and REM sleep. There have been multiple studies showing, electrophysiologically, that these two states are quite similar in nature. This has led some neural binding theorists to study the modes of cognitive awareness in each state. Certain observations have even led these scientists to hypothesize that since there is little cognition going on during REM sleep, the increased thalamocortical responses show the action of processing in the waking preconscious. [29]

The thalamus and cortex are important anatomical features in cognitive and sensory awareness. The understanding of how these neurons fire and relate to one other in each of these states (REM and Waking) is paramount to understanding awareness and its relation to neural binding.

In the waking state, neuronal activity in animals is subject to changes based on the current environment. Changes in environment act as a form of stress on the brain so that when sensory neurons are then fired synchronously, they acclimate to the new state. This new state can then be moved to the hippocampus where it can be stored for later use. In the words of James Newman and Anthony A. Grace in their article, "Binding Across Time" this idea is put forth: "The hippocampus is the primary recipient of inferotemporal outputs and is known to be the substrate for the consolidation of working memories to long term, episodic memories." [30]

The logging of "episodes" is then used for "streaming", which can mediate by the selective gating of certain information reentering sensory awareness. Streaming and building of episodic memories would not be possible if neural binding did not unconsciously connect the two synchronous oscillations.

The pairing of these oscillations can then help input the correct sensory material. If these paired oscillations are not new, then cognitively these firings will be easily understood. If there are new firings, the brain will have to acclimate to the new understanding. [30]

In REM sleep, the only extreme difference from the waking state is that the brain does not have the actual waking amount of sensory firings, so cognitively, there is not as much awareness here, although the activity of the "brain’s eye" is still quite significant and very similar to the waking state. Studies have shown that during sleep there are still 40 Hz Oscillation firings. These firings are due to the perceived stimuli happening in dreams. " [30]

Learning/memory

Opitz argues that the binding of different brain areas is mediated by the hippocampus. Relational bindings, or relationships between separate objects, concepts, and memories, are very flexible because the targets can be combined in so many different ways to deal with the present situation, and the hippocampus ensures that these parts are arranged into a coherent whole. [31] Particularly, the hippocampus has been implicated in binding that is involved with episodic memory, working memory, and language acquisition. According to Opitz, this is viable because the hippocampus meets all criteria to be suitable for the regulation relational binding and, based on its patterns of activity, it is likely that it is involved.

Clinical implications

Autism

A young boy with autism who has arranged his toys in a row Autistic-sweetiepie-boy-with-ducksinarow.jpg
A young boy with autism who has arranged his toys in a row

Several researchers have suggested a clinical link between difficulties in neural binding and autism spectrum disorders. [32] It has been postulated that there is a level of underconnectivity between certain areas of the autistic brain, specifically those areas that involve social cognition. They also hypothesize that there may be overconnectivity within certain neuronal cell assemblies. Researchers have suggested that these issues with coherence of neural networks give rise to the hallmark autistic symptoms, namely, impaired social cognition/interactions and repetitive behaviors.

There is one major piece of evidence suggesting that abnormal connectivity occurs in autism. Autistic brains are known to develop very rapidly early in life, and this could cause problems in the development of cortical connections. It has been suggested that this early overdevelopment may be a cause for the impaired connectivity in autism. Diffusion tensor imaging studies of autistic brains are consistent with this idea, as they show reduced myelination (see:Myelin) in the areas of the brain that deal with social cognition and theory of mind, or one’s ability to comprehend what another person may be thinking. This provides further evidence for underconnectivity of the autistic brain.

Schizophrenia

It is also possible that problems with binding give way to the fractured mental state that is characteristic of schizophrenia. Schizophrenic patients experience a “disintegration of consciousness” which involves hallucinations, delusions, and generally disordered thinking. It is hypothesized that this mental disorganization is caused by underlying neural disorganization due to disordered binding, which is then further complicated by the occurrence of complex mental processes in this disordered state. [33] This can essentially be viewed as a pathological rise in the neural complexity of the brain. The dissociative symptoms and disorganized speech associated with schizophrenia are consistent with this idea. It is also possible that disordered binding between the regulatory mechanisms of the brain gives rise to a lack of synchronized activation of its neural networks.

Anesthesia

A deeper understanding of the mechanisms underlying consciousness on the biochemical level has practical implications which extend to the development of more effective and reliable anesthetics. [3] This method is especially useful for the study of consciousness because of anesthetic's well-known ability to elicit unconsciousness. This often happens in a single moment, giving the impression of a collapse of conscious coherence. Stuart Hameroff is a notable anesthesiologist who promotes this method in the development of a science of consciousness.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive science</span> Interdisciplinary scientific study of cognitive processes

Cognitive science is the interdisciplinary, scientific study of the mind and its processes. It examines the nature, the tasks, and the functions of cognition. Mental faculties of concern to cognitive scientists include language, perception, memory, attention, reasoning, and emotion; to understand these faculties, cognitive scientists borrow from fields such as linguistics, psychology, artificial intelligence, philosophy, neuroscience, and anthropology. The typical analysis of cognitive science spans many levels of organization, from learning and decision to logic and planning; from neural circuitry to modular brain organization. One of the fundamental concepts of cognitive science is that "thinking can best be understood in terms of representational structures in the mind and computational procedures that operate on those structures."

Computational neuroscience is a branch of neuroscience which employs mathematics, computer science, theoretical analysis and abstractions of the brain to understand the principles that govern the development, structure, physiology and cognitive abilities of the nervous system.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Orchestrated objective reduction</span> Theory of a quantum origin of consciousness

Orchestrated objective reduction is a highly controversial theory postulating that consciousness originates at the quantum level inside neurons. The mechanism is held to be a quantum process called objective reduction that is orchestrated by cellular structures called microtubules. It is proposed that the theory may answer the hard problem of consciousness and provide a mechanism for free will. The hypothesis was first put forward in the early 1990s by Nobel laureate for physics, Roger Penrose, and anaesthesiologist Stuart Hameroff. The hypothesis combines approaches from molecular biology, neuroscience, pharmacology, philosophy, quantum information theory, and quantum gravity.

The consciousness and binding problem is the problem of how objects, background, and abstract or emotional features are combined into a single experience.

Electromagnetic theories of consciousness propose that consciousness can be understood as an electromagnetic phenomenon.

Multisensory integration, also known as multimodal integration, is the study of how information from the different sensory modalities may be integrated by the nervous system. A coherent representation of objects combining modalities enables animals to have meaningful perceptual experiences. Indeed, multisensory integration is central to adaptive behavior because it allows animals to perceive a world of coherent perceptual entities. Multisensory integration also deals with how different sensory modalities interact with one another and alter each other's processing.

A gamma wave or gamma rhythm is a pattern of neural oscillation in humans with a frequency between 25 and 140 Hz, the 40 Hz point being of particular interest. Gamma rhythms are correlated with large-scale brain network activity and cognitive phenomena such as working memory, attention, and perceptual grouping, and can be increased in amplitude via meditation or neurostimulation. Altered gamma activity has been observed in many mood and cognitive disorders such as Alzheimer's disease, epilepsy, and schizophrenia.

Holonomic brain theory is a branch of neuroscience investigating the idea that human consciousness is formed by quantum effects in or between brain cells. Holonomic refers to representations in a Hilbert phase space defined by both spectral and space-time coordinates. Holonomic brain theory is opposed by traditional neuroscience, which investigates the brain's behavior by looking at patterns of neurons and the surrounding chemistry.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Thalamocortical radiations</span> Neural pathways between the thalamus and cerebral cortex

In neuroanatomy, thalamocortical radiations, also known as thalamocortical fibres, are the efferent fibres that project from the thalamus to distinct areas of the cerebral cortex. They form fibre bundles that emerge from the lateral surface of the thalamus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neural oscillation</span> Brainwaves, repetitive patterns of neural activity in the central nervous system

Neural oscillations, or brainwaves, are rhythmic or repetitive patterns of neural activity in the central nervous system. Neural tissue can generate oscillatory activity in many ways, driven either by mechanisms within individual neurons or by interactions between neurons. In individual neurons, oscillations can appear either as oscillations in membrane potential or as rhythmic patterns of action potentials, which then produce oscillatory activation of post-synaptic neurons. At the level of neural ensembles, synchronized activity of large numbers of neurons can give rise to macroscopic oscillations, which can be observed in an electroencephalogram. Oscillatory activity in groups of neurons generally arises from feedback connections between the neurons that result in the synchronization of their firing patterns. The interaction between neurons can give rise to oscillations at a different frequency than the firing frequency of individual neurons. A well-known example of macroscopic neural oscillations is alpha activity.

Action selection is a way of characterizing the most basic problem of intelligent systems: what to do next. In artificial intelligence and computational cognitive science, "the action selection problem" is typically associated with intelligent agents and animats—artificial systems that exhibit complex behaviour in an agent environment. The term is also sometimes used in ethology or animal behavior.

Neural coding is a neuroscience field concerned with characterising the hypothetical relationship between the stimulus and the neuronal responses, and the relationship among the electrical activities of the neurons in the ensemble. Based on the theory that sensory and other information is represented in the brain by networks of neurons, it is believed that neurons can encode both digital and analog information.

The quantum mind or quantum consciousness is a group of hypotheses proposing that local physical laws and interactions from classical mechanics or connections between neurons alone cannot explain consciousness, positing instead that quantum-mechanical phenomena, such as entanglement and superposition that cause nonlocalized quantum effects, interacting in smaller features of the brain than cells, may play an important part in the brain's function and could explain critical aspects of consciousness. These scientific hypotheses are as yet unvalidated, and they can overlap with quantum mysticism.

Recurrent thalamo-cortical resonance or Thalamocortical oscillation is an observed phenomenon of oscillatory neural activity between the thalamus and various cortical regions of the brain. It is proposed by Rodolfo Llinas and others as a theory for the integration of sensory information into the whole of perception in the brain. Thalamocortical oscillation is proposed to be a mechanism of synchronization between different cortical regions of the brain, a process known as temporal binding. This is possible through the existence of thalamocortical networks, groupings of thalamic and cortical cells that exhibit oscillatory properties.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Earl K. Miller</span>

Earl Keith Miller is a cognitive neuroscientist whose research focuses on neural mechanisms of cognitive, or executive, control. Earl K. Miller is the Picower Professor of Neuroscience with the Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at Massachusetts Institute of Technology. He is the Chief Scientist and co-founder of SplitSage. He is a co-founder of Neuroblox.

Andreas Karl Engel is a German neuroscientist. He is the director of the Department of Neurophysiology and Pathophysiology at the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE).

A binding neuron (BN) is an abstract concept of processing of input impulses in a generic neuron based on their temporal coherence and the level of neuronal inhibition. Mathematically, the concept may be implemented by most neuronal models including the well-known leaky integrate-and-fire model. The BN concept originated in 1996 and 1998 papers by A. K. Vidybida,

In neuroscience, predictive coding is a theory of brain function which postulates that the brain is constantly generating and updating a "mental model" of the environment. According to the theory, such a mental model is used to predict input signals from the senses that are then compared with the actual input signals from those senses. Predictive coding is member of a wider set of theories that follow the Bayesian brain hypothesis.

Social cognitive neuroscience is the scientific study of the biological processes underpinning social cognition. Specifically, it uses the tools of neuroscience to study "the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the social world". Social cognitive neuroscience uses the epistemological foundations of cognitive neuroscience, and is closely related to social neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience employs human neuroimaging, typically using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Human brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation are also used. In nonhuman animals, direct electrophysiological recordings and electrical stimulation of single cells and neuronal populations are utilized for investigating lower-level social cognitive processes.

The term posterior cortical hot zone was coined by Christof Koch and colleagues to describe the part of the neocortex closely associated with the minimal neural substrate essential for conscious perception. The posterior cortical hot zone includes sensory cortical areas in the parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes. It is the “sensory” cortex, much as the frontal cortex is the “action” cortex.

References

  1. 1 2 Senkowski, D.; T.R. Schneider; J.J. Foxe; A.K. Engel (2008). "Crossmodal binding through neural coherence: implications for multisensory processing". Trends in Neurosciences. 31 (8): 401–409. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.05.002. PMID   18602171. S2CID   14535021.
  2. Rippon i, G.; J. Brock; C. Brown; J. Boucher (2007). "Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: Challenges for the 'new psychophysiology'". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 63 (2): 164–172. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.012. PMID   16820239.
  3. 1 2 3 da Rocha, A.F.; A. Pereira Jr.; F.A.B. Coutinho (October 2001). "N-methyl-D-aspartate channel and consciousness: from signal coincidence detection to quantum computing". Progress in Neurobiology. 64 (1): 555–573. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00069-1. PMID   11311462. S2CID   9129440.
  4. Gray, Charles M.; König, Peter; Engel, Andreas K.; Singer, Wolf (1989). "Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties". Nature. 338 (6213): 334–337. doi:10.1038/338334a0. ISSN   0028-0836. PMID   2922061. S2CID   4281744.
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 Muller, Hermann J.; Mark A. Elliot; Christoph S. Muller; Axel Mecklinger (2001). "Neural Binding of Space and Time: An Introduction". Visual Cognition. 8 (3–5): 273–285. doi:10.1080/13506280143000007. S2CID   145472271.
  6. von der Malsburg, C. (1981). "The Correlation Theory of Brain Function". MPI Biophysical Chemistry.
  7. Crick, F.; C. Koch (1990). "A Framework for Consciousness". Nature Neuroscience. 6 (2): 119–26. doi:10.1038/nn0203-119. PMID   12555104. S2CID   13960489.
  8. Varela, FJ (1995). "Resonant cell assemblies: a new approach to cognitive functions and neuronal synchrony". Biological Research. 28 (1): 81–95. PMID   8728823.
  9. 1 2 Shadlen, Michael N.; J. Anthony Movshon (September 1999). "Synchrony Unbound: A Critical Evaluation of the Temporal Binding Hypothesis". Neuron. 24 (1): 67–77. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80822-3 . PMID   10677027.
  10. Rourk, Christopher John (September 2018). "Ferritin and neuromelanin "quantum dot" array structures in dopamine neurons of the substantia nigra pars compacta and norepinephrine neurons of the locus coeruleus". Biosystems. 171: 48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2018.07.008 . ISSN   0303-2647. PMID   30048795. S2CID   51722018.
  11. Rourk, Chris (2022-01-06). "Application of the Catecholaminergic Neuron Electron Transport (CNET) Physical Substrate for Consciousness and Action Selection to Integrated Information Theory". Entropy. 24 (1): 91. Bibcode:2022Entrp..24...91R. doi: 10.3390/e24010091 . ISSN   1099-4300. PMC   8774445 . PMID   35052119.
  12. Rourk, Christopher J. (2020), "Functional neural electron transport", Quantum Boundaries of Life, Advances in Quantum Chemistry, vol. 82, Elsevier, pp. 25–111, doi:10.1016/bs.aiq.2020.08.001, ISBN   9780128226391, S2CID   229230562 , retrieved 2022-10-09
  13. Xu, Degao; Watt, Gerald D.; Harb, John N.; Davis, Robert C. (2005-03-25). "Electrical Conductivity of Ferritin Proteins by Conductive AFM". Nano Letters. 5 (4): 571–577. doi:10.1021/nl048218x. ISSN   1530-6984. PMID   15826089.
  14. Kumar, Karuppannan Senthil; Pasula, Rupali Reddy; Lim, Sierin; Nijhuis, Christian A. (2015-12-28). "Long-Range Tunneling Processes across Ferritin-Based Junctions". Advanced Materials. 28 (9): 1824–1830. doi:10.1002/adma.201504402. ISSN   0935-9648. PMID   26708136. S2CID   2238319.
  15. Kolay, J.; Bera, S.; Rakshit, T.; Mukhopadhyay, R. (2018-02-07). "Negative Differential Resistance Behavior of the Iron Storage Protein Ferritin". Langmuir. 34 (9): 3126–3135. doi:10.1021/acs.langmuir.7b04356. ISSN   0743-7463. PMID   29412680.
  16. Ho, Ru-Hung; Chen, Yu-Hung; Wang, Chong Mou (June 2012). "Surface differentiation of ferritin and apoferritin with atomic force microscopic techniques". Colloids and Surfaces B: Biointerfaces. 94: 231–235. doi:10.1016/j.colsurfb.2012.01.044. ISSN   0927-7765. PMID   22377219.
  17. Rourk, Christopher J. (May 2019). "Indication of quantum mechanical electron transport in human substantia nigra tissue from conductive atomic force microscopy analysis". Biosystems. 179: 30–38. doi:10.1016/j.biosystems.2019.02.003. ISSN   0303-2647. PMID   30826349. S2CID   73509918.
  18. Sulzer, David; Cassidy, Clifford; Horga, Guillermo; Kang, Un Jung; Fahn, Stanley; Casella, Luigi; Pezzoli, Gianni; Langley, Jason; Hu, Xiaoping P.; Zucca, Fabio A.; Isaias, Ioannis U.; Zecca, Luigi (2018-04-10). "Neuromelanin detection by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and its promise as a biomarker for Parkinson's disease". npj Parkinson's Disease. 4 (1): 11. doi:10.1038/s41531-018-0047-3. ISSN   2373-8057. PMC   5893576 . PMID   29644335.
  19. Friedrich, I.; Reimann, K.; Jankuhn, S.; Kirilina, E.; Stieler, J.; Sonntag, M.; Meijer, J.; Weiskopf, N.; Reinert, T.; Arendt, T.; Morawski, M. (2021-03-22). "Cell specific quantitative iron mapping on brain slices by immuno-µPIXE in healthy elderly and Parkinson's disease". Acta Neuropathologica Communications. 9 (1): 47. doi: 10.1186/s40478-021-01145-2 . ISSN   2051-5960. PMC   7986300 . PMID   33752749.
  20. Bera, Sudipta; Kolay, Jayeeta; Pramanik, Pallabi; Bhattacharyya, Anirban; Mukhopadhyay, Rupa (2019). "Long-range solid-state electron transport through ferritin multilayers". Journal of Materials Chemistry C. 7 (29): 9038–9048. doi:10.1039/c9tc01744e. ISSN   2050-7526. S2CID   198849306.
  21. Rourk, Christopher; Huang, Yunbo; Chen, Minjing; Shen, Cai (2021-06-16). "Indication of Highly Correlated Electron Transport in Disordered Multilayer Ferritin Structures". doi:10.31219/osf.io/7gqmt. S2CID   241118606 . Retrieved 2022-10-09.{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  22. Labra-Muñoz, Jacqueline A.; de Reuver, Arie; Koeleman, Friso; Huber, Martina; van der Zant, Herre S. J. (2022-05-15). "Ferritin-Based Single-Electron Devices". Biomolecules. 12 (5): 705. doi: 10.3390/biom12050705 . ISSN   2218-273X. PMC   9138424 . PMID   35625632.
  23. Schultz, Wolfram (1998-07-01). "Predictive Reward Signal of Dopamine Neurons". Journal of Neurophysiology. 80 (1): 1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1 . ISSN   0022-3077. PMID   9658025. S2CID   52857162.
  24. Schultz, Wolfram (2016-02-02). "Reward functions of the basal ganglia". Journal of Neural Transmission. 123 (7): 679–693. doi:10.1007/s00702-016-1510-0. ISSN   0300-9564. PMC   5495848 . PMID   26838982.
  25. Liu, Changliang; Goel, Pragya; Kaeser, Pascal S. (2021-04-09). "Spatial and temporal scales of dopamine transmission". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 22 (6): 345–358. doi:10.1038/s41583-021-00455-7. ISSN   1471-003X. PMC   8220193 . PMID   33837376.
  26. Garg, Mayank; Vishwakarma, Neelam; Sharma, Amit L.; Singh, Suman (2021-07-08). "Amine-Functionalized Graphene Quantum Dots for Fluorescence-Based Immunosensing of Ferritin". ACS Applied Nano Materials. 4 (7): 7416–7425. doi:10.1021/acsanm.1c01398. ISSN   2574-0970. S2CID   237804893.
  27. Olshausen, B.A.; C. H.Anderson (November 1993). "A Neurobiological Model of Visual Attention and Invariant Pattern Recognition Based on Dynamic Routing of Information". The Journal of Neuroscience. 13 (11): 4700–4719. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-11-04700.1993 . PMC   6576339 . PMID   8229193.
  28. 1 2 Engel, Andreas K.; Pascal Fries; Peter Konig; Michael Brecht; Wolf Singer (1999). "Temporal Binding, Binocular Rivalry, and Consciousness". Consciousness and Cognition. 8 (2): 128–151. doi:10.1006/ccog.1999.0389. PMID   10447995. S2CID   15376936.
  29. Pare, Denis; Rodolfo Llinas (November 1994). "Conscious and Pre-conscious Processes as Seen From the Standpoint of Sleep-Waking Cycle Neurophysiology". Neuropsychologia. 33 (9): 1155–1164. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00055-8 . PMID   7501136. S2CID   21781139.
  30. 1 2 3 Newman, Newman; Anthony A. Grace (1999). "Binding Across Time: The Selective Gating of Frontal and Hippocampal Systems Modulating Working Memory and Attentional States". Consciousness and Cognition. 8 (2): 196–212. doi:10.1006/ccog.1999.0392. PMID   10448002. S2CID   40723493.
  31. Opitz, Bertram (2010). "Neural Binding Mechanisms in Learning and Memory" (PDF). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (7): 1036–1046. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.001. PMID   19914286. S2CID   15291863.
  32. Brock, J.; C.C. Brown; J. Boucher; G. Rippon (2002). "The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism" (PDF). Development and Psychopathology. 14 (2): 209–224. doi:10.1017/s0954579402002018. PMID   12030688. S2CID   3462619.
  33. Bob, Petr (2012). "Consciousness, schizophrenia, and complexity". Cognitive Systems Research. 13: 87–94. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2011.05.001. S2CID   45599813.

Further reading