Personality changes

Last updated

Introduction

Personality change refers to the different forms of change in various aspects of personality. These changes include how we experience things, how our perception of experiences changes, and how we react in situations. [1] An individual's personality may stay somewhat consistent throughout their life. Still, more often than not, everyone undergoes some form of change to their personality in their lifetime. [2] [3]

Contents

Personality refers to individual differences in characteristic thinking, feeling, and behavior patterns. [4] Our personality is like a puzzle; each piece can come from internal or external factors. The many pieces can come from events, circumstances, genetics, or life experiences. Each piece creates our personality as a whole.

Every person has their own "individual differences in particular personality characteristics" [4] that separate them from others. The overall study of personality focuses on two broad areas; The first one is understanding individual differences in personality characteristics.The second is understanding how the various parts of a person come together as a whole. [4]

This article will explain why an individual's personality may change. These changes include how social interactions shape us, how our personality evolves with age, how our experiences can influence us, and how significant events (especially traumatic events) may alter our perceptions.

Each person has their own unique personality, and as a result, the many differences and changes that occur, may be confusing. Even psychologists are still studying and researching to fully understand what personality means and why personality changes. The development of personality is often dependent on the stage of life a person is in. [5] Most development occurs in the earlier stages of life and becomes more stable as one grows into adulthood. [5]

While still uncertain, research suggests that genetics play a role in the change and stability of certain traits in a personality. [6] They have also discovered that environmental sources affect personality too. [7] The debate over nature versus nuture have pervaded the field of psychology since its beginning. Cultural is also a large factor in personality trait differences as well. [5]

Definition of personality

Personality, one's characteristic way of feeling, behaving and thinking, is often conceptualized as a person's standing on each Big Five personality trait (extraversion, neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness). A person's personality profile is thus gauged from their standing on five broad concepts which predict, among other life outcomes, behavior and the quality of interpersonal relationships. Initially, it was believed that one's Big Five profile was static and dichotomous in that one was either at one extreme of each trait or another [8] For example, people are typically categorized as introverted or extraverted. Personality was therefore assessed in terms of generalities or averages. In noticing the strong inconsistencies in how people behaved across situations, some psychologists dismissed personality as nonexistent.[ citation needed ]

This school of thought attributes human behavior to environmental factors, relegating individual differences to situational artifacts and contesting the existence of individual predispositions. It was led by situationists like Walter Mischel (1968). Their contention held that personality was a fictitious concept. For them, the discrepancies observed across one's behaviors were evidence that inter-individual differences did not exist [9] Some aspects of the situationist perspective even suggest that all human beings are the same and that the differences we observe are simply illusory byproducts of the environment. [10]

However, personality experts (sometimes referred to as personologists) [11] soon integrated these inconsistencies into their conceptualization of personality. They modified the old, more monolithic construct by measuring how people differ across situations. Their new methods of personality assessment describe fluctuations in personality characteristics that are consistent and predictable for each person, based on his predispositions and the environment they are in. Some work suggests that people can adopt different levels of a personality dimension as the social situations and time of day change. [12]

Therefore, someone is not conscientious all of the time, but can be conscientious at work and a lot less so when they are home. This work also suggests that intrapersonal variations on a trait can be even larger than interpersonal variations. Extraversion varies more within a person than across individuals, for example. This work was based on individual self-ratings during the day across a long period of time. This allowed for researchers to assess moment-to-moment and day to day variations on personality attributes. [12]

The impact of social roles

In addition, social roles (e.g. employee) have been identified as potential sources of personality change. Researchers have found strong correspondences between the demands of a social role and one's personality profile. [13] If the role requires that the person enacting it be conscientious, her standing on this trait is more likely to be high. Conversely, once he leaves that role or takes on another which entails less conscientiousness, he will manifest a lower level standing on that trait. Longitudinal research demonstrates that people's personality trajectories can often be explained by the social roles they adopted and relinquished throughout their life stages. Thus social roles are often studied as fundamental predictors of personality. [14] The goals associated with them elicit the appropriation of certain personality profiles by the people enacting them. For example, employees judged effective by their peers and superiors are often described as conscientious.

Personality also changes through life stages. This may be due to physiological changes associated with development but also experiences that impact behavior. Adolescence and young adulthood have been found to be prime periods of personality changes, especially in the domains of extraversion and agreeableness. [15] It has long been believed that personality development is shaped by life experiences that intensify the propensities that led individuals to those experiences in the first place, [16] which is known as the Correspondence Principle. [17]

Subsequent research endeavors have integrated these findings in their methods of investigation. Researchers distinguish between mean level and rank order changes in trait standing during old age. [18] Their study of personality trajectories is thus contingent on time and on age considerations. Mottus, Johnson and Geary (2012) found that instability engendered by aging does not necessarily affect one's standing within an age cohort. Hence, fluctuations and stability coexist so that one changes relative to one's former self but not relative to one's peers. Similarly, other psychologists found that Neuroticism, Extraversion (only in men), and Openness decreased with age after 70, but Conscientiousness and Agreeableness increased with age (the latter only in men). Moreover, they suggest that there is a decline on each trait after the age of 81. [19]

Inconsistency as a trait

Personality inconsistency has become such a prevalent consideration for personologists that some even conceptualize it as a predisposition in itself. Fleisher and Woehr (2008) suggest that consistency across the Big Five is a construct that is fairly stable and contributes to the predictive validity of personality measures. Hence, inconsistency is quantifiable much like a trait, and constitutes an index of - and enhances - the fit of psychological models. [20]

To accommodate the inconsistency demonstrated on personality tests, researchers developed the Frame Of Reference principle (FOR). Frame of Reference (FOR) refers to the set of conjectures an individual or group of individuals uses to judge ideas, actions, and experiences to create meaning. FOR's include beliefs, values, schemas, preferences and culture. This can lead to prejudice, biases, and stereotypes due to the limited view an individual has. [21] According to this theory, people tend to think of their personality in terms of a specific social context when they are asked to rate them. Whichever environment is cognitively salient at the time of the personality measurement will influence the respondent's ratings on a trait measure. [22] If, for example, the person is thinking in terms of their student identity, then the personality ratings he reports will most likely reflect the profile he espouses in the context of student life. Accounting for the FOR principle aims at increasing the validity of personality measures. This demonstrates that the predictive validity of personality measures which specify a social context is a lot higher than those measures which take a more generic approach.

This point is substantiated by yet another body of work suggesting that FOR instructions moderated the link between extraversion and openness scores on manager ratings of employee performance [23] This research thus recognizes that the importance of intrapersonal fluctuations contingent on personality is context specific and is not necessarily generalizable across social domains and time.

There are several different FOR's: [24]

Process of change

If "Personality... is one of the strongest and most consistent predictors of subjective well-being," [28] then does personality not change? In fact, "personality does change". [28] But what makes that happen?

Most people, in their lifetime, will experience an event that opens their eyes to a new understanding of the world. For example, someone who is carefree and happy might become more serious and stern after experiencing abuse in a relationship. Another who is serious and stern might become more happy and interested in life after finding a religion that provides them with closure and answered questions. Each day of life is met with events and situations that result in a response from those who experience them - and sometimes, these events can change who we are and how we think at the core.

Research has found a correlation between being multilingual and personality, specifically how one may change personality based on the language currently being spoken. One who is raised bilingual or lived a number of years in a foreign country and learned the language of the land not only experience personality change but often adopt different personalities based on the language they are speaking. These changes are often based on cultural norms of the language's origin. [29]

A study published in 2012 found that "personality does change and that the extent to which personality changes is comparable to other characteristics, such as income, unemployment and marital status". [28] Some of the biggest concerns faced in life are the previously listed factors - how much money does one make (income)? Does one have a job or not (unemployment)? Does one have a lifelong companion (marital status)? These situations can lead to bigger, more complex situations. If one seeks to be married but is not, they may become cold. If one has no job but then gets hired somewhere, they may become grateful and filled with hope. When positive changes happen, "personality... meaningfully predicts changes to life satisfaction". [28] Simply, when one experiences a personality change, it can strongly determine how that person will then feel about life.

Change over a lifetime

There are two very specific types of change that researchers tend to focus on: rank-order change and mean-level change. A rank-order change refers to a change in an individual's personality trait relative to other individuals; such changes do not occur very often. [30] A mean-level change refers to an absolute change in the individual's level of a certain trait over time. Longitudinal research shows that mean-level change does occur. [30] However, some traits tend to change while some traits tend to stay stable.

During adolescence there are many increases or rapid changes in hormones, societal pressures, and environment factors, among other things. These things theoretically factor into significant personality changes as one progresses through adolescence. As a person progresses through adulthood, their personality becomes more stable and predictable because they establish patterns of thinking, behaving, and feeling. [31]

Personality does not stop changing at a specific age. [32] [33] Biological and social transitions in life may also be a factor for change. Biological transitions are stages like puberty or giving birth for the first time. Social transitions might be changes in social roles like becoming a parent or working at a first job. These life transitions do not necessarily cause change, but they may be reasons for change. As humans we do not adapt just in our body. Our mind also makes changes to itself in order to thrive in our environment. One theory says that whether or not these life transitions cause personality change is based on whether the transition was expected based on age or was unforeseen. [34] The events that are expected will cause personality change because those events have common scripts. However, events that are unexpected will give prominence to the traits that already exist for the individual. [34] Historical context also affects personality change. Major life events can lead to changes in personality that can persist for more than a decade. [16] A longitudinal study followed women over 30 years and found that they showed increases in individualism. This may have been due to the changes that were occurring in their country at the time. [35]

Stressful life events and trauma

Negative life events, [36] long-term difficulties, [16] and deteriorated life quality, [16] all predict small but persistent increases in neuroticism, [16] [36] while positive life events, [36] and improved life quality, [16] predict small but persistent decreases in neuroticism. [16] [36] There appears to be no point during the lifespan that neuroticism is immutable, [16] which is known as the Plasticity Principle. [17]

While extreme, traumatic brain injury can impact a person's personality, even having an effect throughout the rest of their life. [37] [38]

Mechanisms of change

There are multiple ways for an individual's personality to change. Individuals will change their behavior based on the ideas in their environment that emit rewards and punishments. Some of these ideas might be implicit, like social roles. The individual changes his or her personality to fit into a social role if it is favorable. Other ideas might be more explicit like a parent trying to change a child's behavior. [39] An individual may decide to actively try to change his or her own behavior/ personality after thinking about his or her own actions. Therapy involves the same type of introspection. The individual, along with the therapist, identifies the behaviors that are inappropriate, and then self-monitors to change them. Eventually, the individual internalizes the behavior they want to attain, and that trait will generalize to other areas of the individual's life. Personality change also occurs when individuals observe the actions of others. Individuals may mimic the behaviors of others and then internalize those behaviors. [40] Once the individual internalizes those behaviors, they are said to be a part of that person's personality. [39] Individuals also receive feedback from other individuals or groups about their own personality. This is a driving force of change because the individual has social motivations to change his or her personality; people often act a certain way based on the popular/majority vote of the people they are around. For example, a girl who likes country music may say she hates country music when she learns that all her peers don't like country music. [41] It has also been shown that major positive and negative life events can predict changes in personality. [16] [36] Some of the largest changes are observed in individuals with psychiatric or neurodegenerative disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease and related dementia. A meta-analysis found consistent evidence that large increases in neuroticism and large declines in the other major personality traits are observed in individuals with dementia. [42] Similar changes were also found in a prospective study (N = 22,611) of self-rated personality. [43]

Studies have shown that mindfulness-meditation therapies have a positive effect of personality maturity. [44]

Cognitive Behavioral Therapy has been tested and proved to be effective in the treatment of adults with anxiety disorders. [45]

Following psilocybin therapy one study communicates that Neuroticism scores lowered substantially while Extraversion increased. [46]

The Big Five personality traits

The Big Five personality traits are often used to measure change in personality. There is a mean-level change in the Big Five traits from age 10 to 65. [47] The trends seen in adulthood are different from trends seen in childhood and adolescence. Some research suggests that during adolescence rank-order change does occur and therefore personality is relatively unstable. [48] Gender differences are also shown before adulthood. [47] [49] Conscientiousness drops from late childhood to adolescence, but then picks back up from adolescence into adulthood. As well, a meta-analysis done by Melissa C. O'Connor and Sampo V. Paunonen, "Big Five Personality Predictors of Post-Secondary Academic Performance", 2006, showed that "... conscientiousness, in particular, [is] most strongly and consistently associated with academic success". [50] [51] Agreeableness also drops from late childhood to adolescence, but then picks back up from adolescence into adulthood. Neuroticism shows a different trend for males and females in childhood and adolescence. For females, Neuroticism increases from childhood to adolescence. Then Neuroticism levels from adolescence into adulthood and continues the adult trend of decreasing. Males however, tend to gradually decrease in Neuroticism from childhood to adolescence into adulthood. Extraversion drops from childhood to adolescence and then does not significantly change. Openness to experience also shows a different trend for different genders. Females tend to decrease in Openness to experience from childhood to early adulthood and then gradually increases all throughout adulthood. Males tend to decrease in Openness to experience from childhood to adolescence, then it tends to increase through adulthood. In the same study done by O'Connor and Paunonen, "Openness to Experience was sometimes positively associated with scholastic achievement..." [50] In adulthood, Neuroticism tends to decrease, while Conscientiousness and Agreeableness tend to increase. Extraversion and Openness to experience do not seem to change much during adulthood. These trends seen in adulthood are different from trends seen in childhood and adolescence. [47] Cross-cultural research shows that German, British, Czech, and Turkish people show similar trends of these personality traits. [52] Similar trends seem to exist in other countries. [53]

In a study done by Deborah A. Cobb-Clark and Stefanie Schurer, "The Stability of Big-Five Personality Traits," done in 2011, showed that "On average, individuals report slightly higher levels of agreeableness, emotional stability, and conscientiousness than extraversion and openness to experience. [On top of that], women report higher scores on each trait except for openness to experience". [54] For clarification, openness to experience can be referred to simply as openness. It is often seen as one's willingness to embrace new things, new ideas, and new activities.

The Big Five personality traits can also be broken down into facets. Different facets of each personality trait are often correlated with different behavioral outcomes. Breaking down the personality traits into facets is difficult and not yet at a consensus. However, it is important to look at change in facets over a lifetime separate from just the change in traits because different facets of the same trait show different trends. [55] [47] For example, openness with values decreases substantially with age, while openness with aesthetics is more stable. [55] Neuroticism can be broken into the two facets of anxiety and depression. [56] Anxiety has the same trend as Neuroticism for both males and females. For females, anxiety increases from childhood to adolescence, at emerging adulthood it levels out, and then starts to decrease into and throughout middle age. Anxiety in males tends to decrease from late childhood through adulthood. Depression (not clinical depression, but rather susceptibility to negative affect) shows two peaks in females. Females tend to have higher levels of this kind of depression in adolescence and then again in early adulthood. Depression does, however, have a negative trend through adulthood. For males, depression tends to show an increase from childhood to early adulthood and then shows a slight decrease through middle age. [47] There are four facets that accompany Extraversion. They are social self-esteem, liveliness, social boldness, and sociability. Social Self-esteem, liveliness, and social boldness starts to increase during our mid-teens and continually increases throughout early adulthood and into late adulthood. Sociability seems to follow a different trend that is pretty high during our early teens but tends to decrease in early-adulthood and then stabilize around the age of 39. [57] [58]

Late life changes

Although there is debate surrounding whether or not personality can change in the late stages of life, more evidence is being discovered about how the environmental factors affect people of all ages. Changes in health are regarded as an influential source of personality stability and change. [59] Across multiple facets of health which include cognitive, physical, and sensory functioning, older adults' ability to maintain their everyday routine and lifestyle is being challenged. [7] There are noticeable finds on reverse trends in maturity-related traits, such as increases in neuroticism and declines in conscientiousness. [60] [61] Mainly the debate in this area revolves around whether the health consequences of old age can be linked to changes in traits and whether these changes can, in turn, impair health and functioning.

Related Research Articles

Personality is any person's collection of interrelated behavioral, cognitive and emotional patterns that comprise a person’s unique adjustment to life. These interrelated patterns are relatively stable, but can change over long time periods.

In psychology, trait theory is an approach to the study of human personality. Trait theorists are primarily interested in the measurement of traits, which can be defined as habitual patterns of behavior, thought, and emotion. According to this perspective, traits are aspects of personality that are relatively stable over time, differ across individuals, are relatively consistent over situations, and influence behaviour. Traits are in contrast to states, which are more transitory dispositions.

Conscientiousness is the personality trait of being careful or diligent. Conscientiousness implies a desire to do a task well, and to take obligations to others seriously. Conscientious people tend to be efficient and organized as opposed to easy-going and disorderly. They tend to show self-discipline, act dutifully, and aim for achievement; they display planned rather than spontaneous behavior; and they are generally dependable. Conscientiousness manifests in characteristic behaviors such as being neat, systematic, careful, thorough, and deliberate.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Big Five personality traits</span> Personality model consisting of five broad dimensions

The Big Five personality traits, sometimes known as "the five-factor model of personality" or "OCEAN model", is a grouping of five unique characteristics used to study personality. It has been developed from the 1980s onward in psychological trait theory.

Personality development encompasses the dynamic construction and deconstruction of integrative characteristics that distinguish an individual in terms of interpersonal behavioral traits. Personality development is ever-changing and subject to contextual factors and life-altering experiences. Personality development is also dimensional in description and subjective in nature. That is, personality development can be seen as a continuum varying in degrees of intensity and change. It is subjective in nature because its conceptualization is rooted in social norms of expected behavior, self-expression, and personal growth. The dominant viewpoint in personality psychology indicates that personality emerges early and continues to develop across one's lifespan. Adult personality traits are believed to have a basis in infant temperament, meaning that individual differences in disposition and behavior appear early in life, potentially before language of conscious self-representation develop. The Five Factor Model of personality maps onto the dimensions of childhood temperament. This suggests that individual differences in levels of the corresponding personality traits are present from young ages.

Neuroticism is a personality trait associated with negative emotions. It is one of the Big Five traits. Individuals with high scores on neuroticism are more likely than average to experience such feelings as anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, envy, jealousy, pessimism, guilt, depressed mood, and loneliness. Such people are thought to respond worse to stressors and are more likely to interpret ordinary situations, such as minor frustrations, as appearing hopelessly difficult. Their behavioral responses may include procrastination, substance use, and other maladaptive behaviors, which may aid in relieving negative emotions and generating positive ones.

Agreeableness is a personality trait referring to individuals that are perceived as kind, sympathetic, cooperative, warm, honest, and considerate. In personality psychology, agreeableness is one of the five major dimensions of personality structure, reflecting individual differences in cooperation and social harmony.

Openness to experience is one of the domains which are used to describe human personality in the Five Factor Model. Openness involves six facets, or dimensions: active imagination (fantasy), aesthetic sensitivity, attentiveness to inner feelings, preference for variety (adventurousness), intellectual curiosity, and challenging authority. A great deal of psychometric research has demonstrated that these facets or qualities are significantly correlated. Thus, openness can be viewed as a global personality trait consisting of a set of specific traits, habits, and tendencies that cluster together.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Affective events theory</span> Psychological model

Affective events theory (AET) is an industrial and organizational psychology model developed by organizational psychologists Howard M. Weiss and Russell Cropanzano to explain how emotions and moods influence job performance and job satisfaction. The model explains the linkages between employees' internal influences and their reactions to incidents that occur in their work environment that affect their performance, organizational commitment, and job satisfaction. The theory proposes that affective work behaviors are explained by employee mood and emotions, while cognitive-based behaviors are the best predictors of job satisfaction. The theory proposes that positive-inducing as well as negative-inducing emotional incidents at work are distinguishable and have a significant psychological impact upon workers' job satisfaction. This results in lasting internal and external affective reactions exhibited through job performance, job satisfaction, and organizational commitment.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Extraversion and introversion</span> Personality trait

Extraversion and introversion are a central trait dimension in human personality theory. The terms were introduced into psychology by Carl Jung, though both the popular understanding and current psychological usage are not the same as Jung's original concept. Extraversion tends to be manifested in outgoing, talkative, energetic behavior, whereas introversion is manifested in more reflective and reserved behavior. Jung defined introversion as an "attitude-type characterised by orientation in life through subjective psychic contents", and extraversion as "an attitude-type characterised by concentration of interest on the external object".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">HEXACO model of personality structure</span> Six-dimensional model of human personality

The HEXACO model of personality structure is a six-dimensional model of human personality that was created by Ashton and Lee and explained in their book, The H Factor of Personality, based on findings from a series of lexical studies involving several European and Asian languages. The six factors, or dimensions, include honesty-humility (H), emotionality (E), extraversion (X), agreeableness (A), conscientiousness (C), and openness to experience (O). Each factor is composed of traits with characteristics indicating high and low levels of the factor. The HEXACO model was developed through similar methods as other trait taxonomies and builds on the work of Costa and McCrae and Goldberg. The model, therefore, shares several common elements with other trait models. However, the HEXACO model is unique mainly due to the addition of the honesty-humility dimension.

The Revised NEO Personality Inventory is a personality inventory that assesses an individual on five dimensions of personality. These are the same dimensions found in the Big Five personality traits. These traits are openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion(-introversion), agreeableness, and neuroticism. In addition, the NEO PI-R also reports on six subcategories of each Big Five personality trait.

The Big Five personality traits are Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. The Big Five Personality is a test that people can take to learn more about their personality in relation to the five personality traits. Cross-cultural psychology as a discipline examines the way that human behavior is different and/or similar across different cultures. One important and widely studied area in this subfield of psychology is personality, particularly the study of Big Five. The Big Five model of personality has become the most extensively studied model of personality and has broad support, starting in the United States and later in many different cultures. The Big Five model of personality started in the United States, and through the years has been translated into many different languages and has been used in many countries. Some researchers were attempting to determine the differences in how other cultures perceive this model. Some research shows that the Big Five holds up across cultures even with its origin in the English language. However, there is also some evidence which suggests that the Big Five traits may not be sufficient to completely explain personality in other cultures. In countries such as South America and East Asia, the results weren't as accurate because they weren't as open as some people in other countries are.

In psychology, a facet is a specific and unique aspect of a broader personality trait. Both the concept and the term "facet" were introduced by Paul Costa and Robert McCrae in the first edition of the NEO-Personality Inventory (NEO-PI) Manual. Facets were originally elaborated only for the neuroticism, openness to experience, and extraversion traits; Costa and McCrae introduced facet scales for the agreeableness and conscientiousness traits in the Revised NEO-PI. Each of the Big Five personality traits in the five factor model contains six facets, each of which is measured with a separate scale. The use of facets and facet scales has since expanded beyond the NEO PI-R, with alternative facet and domain structures derived from other models of personality. Examples include the HEXACO model of personality structure, psycholexical studies, circumplex models, the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ), and the California Psychological Inventory.

Most scientists agree that religiosity is not an independent personality trait, despite there being some commonality between their characteristics. Religiosity and personality traits both relate to one's feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. However, unlike for personality, one's level of religiosity is often measured by the presence or lack of belief in and relationship with a higher power, certain lifestyles or behaviors adopted for a higher power, and a sense of belonging with other followers of one's religion. Additionally, personality traits tend to follow a normal distribution, such that the majority of individuals' scores for a personality trait will be concentrated towards the middle, rather than being extremely high or low. Distributions for religiosity, however, follow a non-normal distribution, such that there are more individuals who score particularly high or low on religiosity scales.

The psychology of music preference is the study of the psychological factors behind peoples' different music preferences. Music is heard by people daily in many parts of the world, and affects people in various ways from emotion regulation to cognitive development, along with providing a means for self-expression. Music training has been shown to help improve intellectual development and ability, though no connection has been found as to how it affects emotion regulation. Numerous studies have been conducted to show that individual personality can have an effect on music preference, mostly using personality, though a recent meta-analysis has shown that personality in itself explains little variance in music preferences. These studies are not limited to American culture, as they have been conducted with significant results in countries all over the world, including Japan, Germany, Spain, and Brazil.

The biological basis of personality it is the collection of brain systems and mechanisms that underlie human personality. Human neurobiology, especially as it relates to complex traits and behaviors, is not well understood, but research into the neuroanatomical and functional underpinnings of personality are an active field of research. Animal models of behavior, molecular biology, and brain imaging techniques have provided some insight into human personality, especially trait theories.

Intelligence and personality have traditionally been studied as separate entities in psychology, but more recent work has increasingly challenged this view. An increasing number of studies have recently explored the relationship between intelligence and personality, in particular the Big Five personality traits.

A zero-acquaintance situation requires a perceiver to make a judgment about a target with whom the perceiver has had no prior social interaction. These judgments can be made using a variety of cues, including brief interactions with the target, video recordings of the target, photographs of the target, and observations of the target's personal environments, among others. In zero-acquaintance studies, the target's actual personality is determined through the target's self-rating and/or ratings from close acquaintance(s) of that target. Consensus in ratings is determined by how consistently perceivers rate the target's personality when compared to other raters. Accuracy in ratings is determined by how well perceivers' ratings of a target compare to that target's self-ratings on the same scale, or to that target's close acquaintances' ratings of the target. Zero-acquaintance judgments are regularly made in day-to-day life. Given that these judgments tend to remain stable, even as the length of interaction increases, they can influence important interpersonal outcomes.

Personality neuroscience uses neuroscientific methods to study the neurobiological mechanisms underlying individual differences in stable psychological attributes. Specifically, personality neuroscience aims to investigate the relationships between inter-individual variation in brain structures as well as functions and behavioral measures of persistent psychological traits, broadly defined as "predispositions and average tendencies to be in particular states", including but are not limited to personality traits, sociobehavioral tendencies, and psychopathological risk factors. Personality neuroscience is considered as an interdisciplinary field integrating research questions and methodologies from social psychology, personality psychology, and neuroscience. It is closely related to other interdisciplinary fields, such as social, cognitive, and affective neuroscience.

References

  1. "Personality Change - an overview | ScienceDirect Topics". www.sciencedirect.com. Retrieved 2023-06-01.
  2. Helson, Ravenna; Kwan, Virginia S.Y; John, Oliver P; Jones, Constance (August 2002). "The growing evidence for personality change in adulthood: Findings from research with personality inventories". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (4): 287–306. doi:10.1016/s0092-6566(02)00010-7. ISSN   0092-6566.
  3. Pusch, Sebastian; Mund, Marcus; Hagemeyer, Birk; Finn, Christine (May 2019). "Personality Development in Emerging and Young Adulthood: A Study of Age Differences". European Journal of Personality. 33 (3): 245–263. doi: 10.1002/per.2181 . ISSN   0890-2070. S2CID   149938319.
  4. 1 2 3 "Personality". www.apa.org. Retrieved 2021-09-29.
  5. 1 2 3 Ashton, Michael C. (2017-05-30). Individual Differences and Personality. Academic Press. ISBN   978-0-12-809846-2.
  6. Penke, Lars; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Miller, Geoffrey F. (August 2007). "The evolutionary genetics of personality". European Journal of Personality. 21 (5): 549–587. doi: 10.1002/per.629 . ISSN   0890-2070. S2CID   13403823.
  7. 1 2 Wagner, Jenny; Orth, Ulrich; Bleidorn, Wiebke; Hopwood, Christopher James; Kandler, Christian (2020-03-26). Towards an Integrative Model of Sources of Personality Stability and Change. doi:10.31234/osf.io/qzef8. S2CID   240846892.
  8. Funder, D. C. (2010). The Personality Puzzle (5th Ed.). NY: Norton
  9. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. Hoboken, NJ US: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  10. Kamtekar, Rachana (April 2004). "Situationism and Virtue Ethics on the Content of Our Character" . Ethics. 114 (3): 458–491. doi:10.1086/381696. ISSN   0014-1704. S2CID   44335958.
  11. "personology". APA Dictionary of Psychology.
  12. 1 2 Fleeson, William (2001). "Towards a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 1011–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011. PMID   11414368.
  13. Heller, D.; Perunovic, W. Q. E.; Reichman, D. (2009). "The future of person-situation integration in the interface between traits and goals: A bottom-up framework". Journal of Personality. 43 (2): 171–178. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.011.
  14. Ozer, D. J.; Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). "Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes". Annual Review of Psychology. 57: 401–421. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127. PMID   16318601.
  15. Soto, C. J.; John, O. P.; Gosling, S. D.; Potter, J. (2011). "Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 330–348. doi:10.1037/a0021717. PMID   21171787.
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Jeronimus, B.F.; et al. (2014). "Mutual Reinforcement Between Neuroticism and Life Experiences: A Five-Wave, 16-Year Study to Test Reciprocal Causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID   25111305.
  17. 1 2 Caspi, A.; Shiner, R. (2011). Temperament and Personality, in Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (5th ed). Malden: Blackwell Publishing Limited. p.  182. doi:10.1002/9781444300895. ISBN   9781444300895.
  18. Mõttus, R.; Johnson, W.; Deary, I. J. (2012). "Personality traits in old age: Measurement and rank-order stability and some mean-level change" (PDF). Psychology and Aging. 27 (1): 243–249. doi:10.1037/a0023690. hdl: 20.500.11820/39c061a9-27f6-49a2-9b91-3c1293210310 . PMID   21604884. S2CID   13165488.
  19. Lucas, R. E.; Donnellan, M. (2009). "Age differences in personality: Evidence from a nationally representative Australian sample". Developmental Psychology. 45 (5): 1353–1363. doi:10.1037/a0013914. PMID   19702397.
  20. Fleisher, Matthew S.; Woehr, David J.; Edwards, Bryan D.; Cullen, Kristin L. (2011). "Assessing within-person personality variability via frequency estimation: More evidence for a new measurement approach". Journal of Research in Personality. 45 (6): 535–548. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.06.009.
  21. "Frame of Reference". changingminds.org. Retrieved 2023-03-29.
  22. Reddock, C. M.; Biderman, M. D.; Nguyen, N. T. (2011). "The relationship of reliability and validity of personality tests to frame-of-reference instructions and within-person inconsistency". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 19 (2): 119–131. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2011.00540.x. S2CID   37446065.
  23. Hunthausen, J. M.; Truxillo, D. M.; Bauer, T. N.; Hammer, L. B. (2003). "A field study of frame-of-reference effects on personality test validity". Journal of Applied Psychology. 88 (3): 545–551. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.545. PMID   12814302.
  24. 1 2 "Psychodynamic Frame of Reference | OT Theory". ottheory.com. Retrieved 2023-03-29.
  25. "Psychodynamic Frame of Reference". OccupationalTherapyOT.com. 2017-03-08. Retrieved 2023-03-29.
  26. Kang, Chris (2017-06-01). "A psychospiritual integration frame of reference for occupational therapy. Part 2: Transformative occupations and the change process". Hong Kong Journal of Occupational Therapy. 29 (1): 55–64. doi:10.1016/j.hkjot.2017.05.003. ISSN   1569-1861. PMC   6092005 . PMID   30186073. S2CID   52158286.
  27. "Occupational Therapy Insights: Psychospiritual Integration Frame of Reference: An OT Framework Review". occupationaltherapyinsights.libsyn.com. Retrieved 2023-03-29.
  28. 1 2 3 4 Boyce, Christopher J.; Wood, Alex M.; Powdthavee, Nattavudh (2013-03-01). "Is Personality Fixed? Personality Changes as Much as "Variable" Economic Factors and More Strongly Predicts Changes to Life Satisfaction". Social Indicators Research. 111 (1): 287–305. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0006-z. hdl: 11343/282950 . ISSN   1573-0921. S2CID   13771232.
  29. Chen, Sylvia Xiaohua; Bond, Michael Harris (2010-10-13). "Two Languages, Two Personalities? Examining Language Effects on the Expression of Personality in a Bilingual Context". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (11): 1514–1528. doi:10.1177/0146167210385360. ISSN   0146-1672. PMID   20944020. S2CID   25898650.
  30. 1 2 Funder, David C. (2010). The Personality Puzzle (5th ed.). W.W. Norton & Company. p. 258. ISBN   978-0-393-93348-2.
  31. Borghuis, Jeroen; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Oberski, Daniel; Sijtsma, Klaas; Meeus, Wim H. J.; Branje, Susan; Koot, Hans M.; Bleidorn, Wiebke (October 2017). "Big Five personality stability, change, and codevelopment across adolescence and early adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 113 (4): 641–657. doi:10.1037/pspp0000138. hdl: 1874/358188 . ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   28253001. S2CID   4650504.
  32. Roberts, Brent W.; DelVecchio, Wendy F. (2000). "The Rank-Order Consistency of Personality Traits From Childhood to Old Age: A Quantitative Review of Longitudinal Studies". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID   10668348.
  33. Atherton, Olivia E.; Willroth, Emily C.; Graham, Eileen K.; Luo, Jing; Mroczek, Daniel K.; Lewis‐Thames, Marquita W. (2023-02-16). "Rural–urban differences in personality traits and well‐being in adulthood". Journal of Personality: jopy.12818. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12818 . ISSN   0022-3506. PMC  10390645. PMID   36725776. S2CID   256499919.
  34. 1 2 Caspi, Avshalom; Moffitt, Terrie E. (1993). "When Do Individual Differences Matter? A Paradoxical Theory of Personality Coherence". Psychological Inquiry. 4 (4): 247–271. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0404_1.
  35. Roberts, Brent W.; Ravenna Helson (1997). "Changes in Culture, Changes in Personality: The Influence of Individualism in a Longitudinal Study of Women". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (3): 641–651. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.641. PMID   9120788.
  36. 1 2 3 4 5 Jeronimus, B.F., Ormel, J., Aleman, A., Penninx, B.W.J.H., Riese, H. (2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID   23410535. S2CID   43717734.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. MAX, JEFFREY E.; KOELE, SHARON L.; CASTILLO, CARLOS C.; LINDGREN, SCOTT D.; ARNDT, STEPHAN; BOKURA, HIROKAZU; ROBIN, DONALD A.; SMITH, WILBUR L.; SATO, YUTAKA (March 2000). "Personality change disorder in children and adolescents following traumatic brain injury". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 6 (3): 279–289. doi:10.1017/s1355617700633039. ISSN   1355-6177. PMID   10824500. S2CID   23160666.
  38. Yeates, Giles Noel; Gracey, Fergus; Mcgrath, Joanna Collicutt (November 2008). "A biopsychosocial deconstruction of "personality change" following acquired brain injury". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 18 (5–6): 566–589. doi:10.1080/09602010802151532. ISSN   0960-2011. PMID   18609012. S2CID   25007282.
  39. 1 2 Caspi, Avshalom; Brent W. Roberts (2001). "Personality Development Across the Life Course: The Argument for Change and Continuity". Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 49–66. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1202_01. S2CID   144947217.
  40. Roberts, Brent W.; Wood, Dustin; L. Smith, Jennifer (2005). "Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development". Journal of Research in Personality. 39: 166–184. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.08.002.
  41. "Social Influence Revision Notes | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2021-10-04.
  42. Islam, Maheen; Mazumder, Mridul; Schwabe-Warf, Derek; Stephan, Yannick; Sutin, Angelina R.; Terracciano, Antonio (February 2019). "Personality Changes With Dementia From the Informant Perspective: New Data and Meta-Analysis". Journal of the American Medical Directors Association. 20 (2): 131–137. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2018.11.004. PMC   6432780 . PMID   30630729.
  43. Terracciano, A; Luchetti, M; Stephan, Y; Löckenhoff, CE; Ledermann, T; Sutin, AR (October 2023). "Changes in Personality Before and During Cognitive Impairment". Journal of the American Medical Directors Association. 24 (10): 1465–1470.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2023.05.011 . PMID   37330217.
  44. Crescentini, Cristiano; Matiz, Alessio; Cimenti, Matteo; Pascoli, Eric; Eleopra, Roberto; Fabbro, Franco (2018). "Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Personality and Psychological Well-being in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis". International Journal of MS Care. 20 (3): 101–108. doi:10.7224/1537-2073.2016-093. ISSN   1537-2073. PMC   5991502 . PMID   29896046.
  45. Hofmann, Stefan G.; Smits, Jasper A. J. (April 2008). "Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 69 (4): 621–632. doi:10.4088/jcp.v69n0415. ISSN   0160-6689. PMC   2409267 . PMID   18363421.
  46. Erritzoe, D.; Roseman, L.; Nour, M. M.; MacLean, K.; Kaelen, M.; Nutt, D. J.; Carhart-Harris, R. L. (November 2018). "Effects of psilocybin therapy on personality structure". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 138 (5): 368–378. doi:10.1111/acps.12904. ISSN   1600-0447. PMC   6220878 . PMID   29923178.
  47. 1 2 3 4 5 Soto, Christopher J.; John, OP; Gosling, SD; Potter, J (2011). "Age Differences in Personality Traits From 10 to 65: Big Five Domains and Facets in a Large Cross-Sectional Sample". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 330–348. doi:10.1037/a0021717. PMID   21171787.
  48. McCrae, Robert R.; Paul T. Costa Jr; Antonio Terracciano; Wayne D. Parker; Carol J. Mills; Filip De Fruyt; Ivan Mervielde (2002). "Personality Trait Development From Age 12 to 18: Longitudinal, Cross-Sectional, and Cross-Cultural Analyses". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (6): 1456–1468. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1456. PMID   12500824.
  49. Weisberg, Yanna J.; DeYoung, Colin G.; Hirsh, Jacob B. (2011). "Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big Five". Frontiers in Psychology. 2: 178. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00178 . PMC   3149680 . PMID   21866227.
  50. 1 2 O’Connor, Melissa C.; Paunonen, Sampo V. (2007-10-01). "Big Five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performance". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (5): 971–990. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017. ISSN   0191-8869.
  51. Andersen, Simon Calmar; Gensowski, Miriam; Ludeke, Steven G.; John, Oliver P. (2020). "A stable relationship between personality and academic performance from childhood through adolescence. An original study and replication in hundred-thousand-person samples" (PDF). Journal of Personality. 88 (5): 925–939. doi:10.1111/jopy.12538. PMID   31895473. S2CID   209543944.
  52. McCrae, Robert R.; Paul T. Costa Jr; Margarida Pedrosa de Lima; Antonio Simoes; Fritz Ostendorf; Alois Angleitner; Iris Marusic; Denis Bratko; Gian Vittorio Caprara; Claudio Barbaranelli; Joon-Ho Chae (1999). "Age Differences in Personality Across the Adult Life Span: Parallels in Five Cultures". Developmental Psychology. 35 (2): 466–477. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.466. PMID   10082017.
  53. McCrae, Robert R.; Terracciano, Antonio (March 2005). "Universal Features of Personality Traits From the Observer's Perspective: Data From 50 Cultures" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 88 (3): 547–561. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547. PMID   15740445. S2CID   12498947.
  54. Cobb-Clark, Deborah A.; Schurer, Stefanie (2012-04-01). "The stability of big-five personality traits". Economics Letters. 115 (1): 11–15. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2011.11.015. hdl: 10419/55111 . ISSN   0165-1765. S2CID   12086995.
  55. 1 2 Terracciano, Antonio; McCrae, Robert R.; Brant, Larry J.; Costa, Paul T. (2005). "Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R Scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging". Psychology and Aging. 20 (3): 493–506. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. PMC   2808690 . PMID   16248708.
  56. Muris, Peter; Roelofs, Jeffrey; Rassin, Eric; Franken, Ingmar; Mayer, Birgit (2005). "Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (6): 1105–1111. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.005.
  57. Ashton, Michael (13 June 2017). INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND PERSONALITY. pp. 90–91. ISBN   978-0-12-809845-5.
  58. Lee, Richard M.; Dean, Brooke L.; Jung, Kyoung-Rae (2008). "Social connectedness, extraversion, and subjective well-being: Testing a mediation model". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (5): 414–419. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.017.
  59. Mueller, Swantje; Wagner, Jenny; Gerstorf, Denis (2017), "On the role of personality in late life", Personality Development Across the Lifespan, Elsevier, pp. 69–84, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-804674-6.00006-5, ISBN   978-0-12-804674-6
  60. Wortman, Jessica; Lucas, Richard E.; Donnellan, M. Brent (2012). "Stability and change in the Big Five personality domains: Evidence from a longitudinal study of Australians". Psychology and Aging. 27 (4): 867–874. doi:10.1037/a0029322. PMID   22775362.
  61. Terracciano, Antonio; McCrae, Robert R.; Brant, Larry J.; Costa, Paul T. (2005). "Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging". Psychology and Aging. 20 (3): 493–506. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. PMC   2808690 . PMID   16248708.

Further reading