A signature whistle is a learned, individually distinctive whistle type in a bottlenose dolphin's (Tursiops truncatus) acoustic repertoire that gives the identity of the whistle owner. [1] The whistles are identified and studied in the wild or in captivity by researchers using hydrophones. [2] Vocal learning strongly influences the development of signature whistles, [3] which can remain stable for up to at least 12 years. [4] More often than not, calves develop signature whistles by the sounds they hear while making sure that they differ from close associates. [1] However, male calves tend to learn signature whistles that resemble their mother's. [1] They are believed to be most frequently used in the communication of captive bottlenose dolphins, [4] along with having specialized functions and properties. [5] Signature whistles are in a higher frequency range than humans can hear. [6] Researchers define a signature whistle as a whistle with a unique frequency curve that dominates in the repertoire of a dolphin. [7] Each dolphin has a distinct signature whistle that other members of its social group use to individually identify the whistler. [4] They are typically used for localisation purposes, however they also provide dolphins with behavioural context. [8] [9] Signature whistles have an important role in facilitating mother–calf contact, [6] group cohesion and social interaction. [3]
Identification of signature whistle calls from wild, unrestrained bottlenose dolphins is essential to understand how they are used in their natural environment. [1] Signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins can be recorded in the wild or in captivity. [7] [3] Hydrophones are used in both cases, but the number of devices may vary based on the researcher's preference and methodology. Using multiple hydrophones allows the researchers to better identify which dolphin emitted which whistle. [9] Although multiple hydrophones are used, signature identification uses a single hydrophone and allows better identification. [2] Variation between signature whistles can be tested by examining the whistle contours frequency modulation pattern. [1] A frequency modulation pattern differs between dolphins, which is usually identified by using standard acoustic parameter measurements, such as beginning, end, maximum or minimum frequency. [1] Combining the measurements of the frequencies will show the modulation pattern of the individuals signature whistle.
Signature whistles are difficult to identify because bottlenose dolphins emit non-signature whistles as well. However, there is a distinct amount of time in between each signature whistle that helps researchers distinguish them from the rest of dolphin noises. [2] Signature whistles are typically emitted within 1 to 10 seconds of each other, while non-signature whistles can occur with longer or shorter intervals between each whistle. [2] This distinction can be used to identify signature whistles in hydrophone recordings.
An early definition of signature whistles defined them as the most common whistle emitted when a dolphin is kept in isolation. [1] This description restricted research designs to only studying dolphins in captivity. The current method for identifying signature whistles is known as signature identification, or SIGID. Developed by Janik and King, this method combines human observations with vocalization analysis. The vocalizations are recorded by a single hydrophone. [2] This method allows researchers to better identify signature whistles when studying wild dolphins because it can be used in situations where many dolphins emit whistles at the same time.
A dolphin's signature whistle development is influenced by auditory experience, [1] and usually fully develops within the first year of life, rarely changing throughout adulthood. [10] Stable whistle call structure for at least a decade is necessary for long term call recognition between bottlenose dolphins. [1] A calf develops its own signature whistle based on the sounds they hear and most calves develop signature whistles that differ from their close associates. [1] Calves tend to model their signature whistles after those of adult dolphins who they do not spend much time with. [10] Due to the fact that signature whistles are unique, a calf's signature whistle never matches a single whistle but instead has similarities to multiple whistles. [10] However, male calves tend to develop signature whistles that resemble the signature whistles of their mother's. [1] The male calf does not copy the whistle, instead it uses it as a model. [10]
Vocal learning is essential in a dolphin calf's signature whistle development. [11] It plays an important role in development because it helps to build an individualized whistle with a unique frequency modulation pattern. [1] It is an animal's ability to imitate vocalizations from other animals of the same species, and eventually produces its own sounds. [12] Social interaction plays a significant role in vocal learning. [10] Although vocal learning is often associated with aggressive behavior in some animals, this is not the case in dolphins. [11] Rather, vocal learning strengthens social bonds, such as those between mother-calf pairs and alliance partners. [11]
Apart from signature whistles being developed socially there are other behavioural, ecological, genetic and modification factors that can also influence the development of a dolphin's whistle. [1] For example, signature whistles may vary from a behavioural factor and the temporal production of whistles can be affected by stress or other psychological influences. [1] Determining the contribution of different influences on signature whistle variation, aside from social influence, is difficult to achieve due to minimal populations. It is best examined in multiple populations using recordings collected from various behavioural contexts. [1]
Signature whistles are primarily used to locate group members, however they can also provide dolphins with the behavioural situations their group members are in. [8] Dolphins primarily emit signature whistles when one individual is separated from the rest of the group. [10] Non-signature whistles are the main vocalization when all of the group members are together. [10] Although signature whistles are used to locate other dolphins, captive dolphins, who can use their sight to locate other animals, also emit these whistles. [7] It is believed that signature whistles contain information other than location or identity, and can explain why dolphins in captivity use them. [3] Signature whistle emissions also increase during socialization and predation. [8]
Similar to vocal learning, bottlenose dolphins demonstrate vocal copying. [11] Bottlenose dolphins repeat another dolphin's signature whistle back in order to address that particular dolphin individually. [11] Unlike other animals, dolphins do not display this behavior in aggressive situations. [11] Humans and dolphins are the only known species to use vocal copying in cooperative contexts. [11] Although this behavior is rare, dolphins with close relationships are more likely to demonstrate vocal copying. [11] Also, bottlenose dolphins are capable of producing almost perfect copies of another dolphin's whistle. Any differences in the repeated whistle could be deliberate, reinforcing the idea that the dolphins individually address close relatives. [11]
In the Sarasota Dolphin Research Program's library of recordings were 19 female common bottlenose dolphins producing signature whistles both with and without the presence of their dependent calf. [6] In all 19 cases, the mother dolphin inflected the signature whistle when their calf was present, by reaching a higher frequency, or using a wider frequency range. [13] Similarly, humans use higher fundamental frequencies and a wider pitch range to inflect child–directed speech (CDS). [13] [6] [14] This has rarely been discovered in other species. [13] The researchers stated that CDS benefits for humans are cueing the child to pay attention, long-term bonding, and promoting the development of lifelong vocal learning, with parallels in these bottlenose dolphins in an example of convergent evolution. [13]
Dolphins also use signature whistles to provide identity information when meeting new groups of dolphins. [9] [15] When groups of dolphins first join whistle exchanges are a necessary component of the social interaction. [15] Signature whistles between groups are the primary source in providing identity information due to the fact that only 10 percent of joins occurred without whistles. [15] Signature whistle emission rates are nine times higher when two groups encounter each other than in peak whistle rates during general socialization within pods; and they repeat them multiple times during the exchange. [15] The repetition of signature whistle's increases the chances of correct identification. [15]
Whistle matching may occur when two or more groups of dolphins encounter each other, as well. [9] Bottlenose dolphins have low rates of aggression towards close associates and other members of the species. [11] It is observed when a dolphin copies another individual's whistle and repeats it back. [9] Matching occurs in order for one dolphin to acknowledge another individual's presence, which can occur when the animals are up to 600 meters apart. [9] Signature whistles may play an important role in interactions between groups.
A dolphin is an aquatic mammal in the clade Odontoceti. Dolphins belong to the families Delphinidae, Platanistidae, Iniidae, Pontoporiidae, and possibly extinct Lipotidae. There are 40 extant species named as dolphins.
The bottlenose dolphin is a toothed whale in the genus Tursiops. They are common, cosmopolitan members of the family Delphinidae, the family of oceanic dolphins. Molecular studies show the genus contains three species: the common bottlenose dolphin, the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin, and Tamanend's bottlenose dolphin. Others, like the Burrunan dolphin, may be alternately considered their own species or be subspecies of T. aduncus. Bottlenose dolphins inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide, being found everywhere except for the Arctic and Antarctic Circle regions. Their name derives from the Latin tursio (dolphin) and truncatus for the truncated teeth.
Oceanic dolphins or Delphinidae are a widely distributed family of dolphins that live in the sea. Close to forty extant species are recognised. They include several big species whose common names contain "whale" rather than "dolphin", such as the Globicephalinae. Delphinidae is a family within the superfamily Delphinoidea, which also includes the porpoises (Phocoenidae) and the Monodontidae. River dolphins are relatives of the Delphinoidea.
The toothed whales are a clade of cetaceans that includes dolphins, porpoises, and all other whales with teeth, such as beaked whales and the sperm whales. 73 species of toothed whales are described. They are one of two living groups of cetaceans, the other being the baleen whales (Mysticeti), which have baleen instead of teeth. The two groups are thought to have diverged around 34 million years ago (mya).
Cetacean intelligence is the overall intelligence and derived cognitive ability of aquatic mammals belonging in the infraorder Cetacea (cetaceans), including baleen whales, porpoises, and dolphins. In 2014, a study found for first time that the long-finned pilot whale has more neocortical neurons than any other mammal, including humans, examined to date.
The common dolphin is the most abundant cetacean in the world, with a global population of about six million. Despite this fact and its vernacular name, the common dolphin is not thought of as the archetypal dolphin, with that distinction belonging to the bottlenose dolphin due to its popular appearances in aquaria and the media. However, the common dolphin is often depicted in Ancient Greek and Roman art and culture, most notably in a mural painted by the Greek Minoan civilization.
The Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin is a species of bottlenose dolphin. This dolphin grows to 2.6 m (8.5 ft) long, and weighs up to 230 kg (510 lb). It lives in the waters around India, northern Australia, South China, the Red Sea, and the eastern coast of Africa. Its back is dark grey and its belly is lighter grey or nearly white with grey spots.
The Atlantic spotted dolphin is a dolphin found in warm temperate and tropical waters of the Atlantic Ocean. Older members of the species have a very distinctive spotted coloration all over their bodies.
The common bottlenose dolphin or Atlantic bottlenose dolphin is one of three species of bottlenose dolphin in the genus Tursiops. The common bottlenose dolphin is a very familiar dolphin due to the wide exposure it receives in human care in marine parks and dolphinariums, and in movies and television programs. Common bottlenose dolphins inhabit temperate and tropical oceans throughout the world, absent only from polar waters. While formerly known simply as the bottlenose dolphin, this term is now applied to the genus Tursiops as a whole. As considerable genetic variation has been described within this species, even between neighboring populations, many experts think additional species may be recognized.
Animal languages are forms of communication between animals that show similarities to human language. Animals communicate through a variety of signs, such as sounds and movements. Signing among animals may be considered a form of language if the inventory of signs is large enough. The signs are relatively arbitrary, and the animals seem to produce them with a degree of volition.
Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from the predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology, and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning, and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.
Whales use a variety of sounds for communication and sensation. The mechanisms used to produce sound vary from one family of cetaceans to another. Marine mammals, including whales, dolphins, and porpoises, are much more dependent on sound than land mammals due to the limited effectiveness of other senses in water. Sight is less effective for marine mammals because of the way particulates in the ocean scatter light. Smell is also limited, as molecules diffuse more slowly in water than in air, which makes smelling less effective. However, the speed of sound is roughly four times greater in water than in the atmosphere at sea level. As sea mammals are so dependent on hearing to communicate and feed, environmentalists and cetologists are concerned that they are being harmed by the increased ambient noise in the world's oceans caused by ships, sonar and marine seismic surveys.
Human–animal communication is the communication observed between humans and other animals, ranging from non-verbal cues and vocalizations to the use of language.
Louis Herman was an American marine biologist. He was a researcher of dolphin sensory abilities, dolphin cognition, and humpback whales. He was professor in the Department of Psychology and a cooperating faculty member of the Department of Oceanography at the University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa. He founded the Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory (KBMML) in Honolulu, Hawaii in 1970 to study bottlenose dolphin perception, cognition, and communication. In 1975, he pioneered the scientific study of the annual winter migration of humpback whales into Hawaiian waters. Together with Adam Pack, he founded The Dolphin Institute in 1993, a non-profit corporation dedicated to dolphins and whales through education, research, and conservation.
Akeakamai was a female Atlantic bottlenose dolphin, who, along with a companion female dolphin named Phoenix, and later tankmates Elele and Hiapo, were the subjects of Louis Herman's animal language studies at the Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory in Honolulu, Hawaii. The most well-known paper is the original work described in Herman, Richards, & Wolz (1984). Akeakamai was also the subject of many other scientific studies of dolphin cognition, language acquisition, and sensory abilities.
The Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute (BDRI) is a research and educational centre dedicated to the understanding and conservation of cetaceans and the marine environment in which they live. The Institute's BDRI center was founded by the biologist Bruno Díaz López in Sardinia, Italy in 2005. In 2014, the BDRI opened a new facility in Galicia, Spain.
Vocal learning is the ability to modify acoustic and syntactic sounds, acquire new sounds via imitation, and produce vocalizations. "Vocalizations" in this case refers only to sounds generated by the vocal organ as opposed to by the lips, teeth, and tongue, which require substantially less motor control. A rare trait, vocal learning is a critical substrate for spoken language and has only been detected in eight animal groups despite the wide array of vocalizing species; these include humans, bats, cetaceans, pinnipeds, elephants, and three distantly related bird groups including songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. Vocal learning is distinct from auditory learning, or the ability to form memories of sounds heard, a relatively common trait which is present in all vertebrates tested. For example, dogs can be trained to understand the word "sit" even though the human word is not in its innate auditory repertoire. However, the dog cannot imitate and produce the word "sit" itself as vocal learners can.
Brenda McCowan is a research behaviorist interested in evolutionary, biological, and ecological aspects of animal behavior and communication. Her work focuses on improving the health and welfare of domesticated production animals, captive species, and wildlife using applied animal behavior and bioacoustics. She received her BS in Animal Physiology from Cornell University in 1985, and her PhD in Biological Anthropology from Harvard University in 1994. Dr. McCowan is the Program Head of Primate Behavioral Management at California National Primate Research Center, a position she has held since 2004. Dr. McCowan has been on the faculty at the University of California – Davis since 1999, and is currently an Associate Adjunct Professor in the Department of Population Health and Reproduction at the UC Davis School of Veterinary Medicine. She also heads the McCowan Lab of Behavioral Management at UC Davis School of Veterinary Medicine, and is affiliated with the SETI Institute, Hubbs-Sea World Institute, and Santa Fe Institute.
The Black Sea bottlenose dolphin is a subspecies of the common bottlenose dolphin. Recent findings suggest that they differ from the common bottlenose dolphin based on slight differences in cranial activity and genetic composition. This species occupies the Black Sea, and is less frequently studied than the more common Atlantic bottlenose dolphin.
{{cite book}}
: |journal=
ignored (help)