Cat communication

Last updated
A primary form of cat communication is body language, such as the position of the ears. Cats Jip and Noah communicating 28022008(004).jpg
A primary form of cat communication is body language, such as the position of the ears.

Cats communicate for a variety of reasons, including to show happiness, express anger, solicit attention, and observe potential prey. Additionally, they collaborate, play, and share resources. When cats communicate with humans, they do so to get what they need or want, such as food, water, attention, or play. As such, cat communication methods have been significantly altered by domestication. [1] Studies have shown that domestic cats tend to meow much more than feral cats. [2] They rarely meow to communicate with fellow cats or other animals. Cats can socialize with each other and are known to form "social ladders," where a dominant cat is leading a few lesser cats. This is common in multi-cat households.

Contents

Cats can use a range of communication methods, including vocal, visual, tactile and olfactory communication. Up to 21 different cat vocalizations have been observed. [3] They use visual signals, or body language, to express emotions like relaxation, fear, and aggression. Cats use several types of tactile behaviors to communicate, such as grooming or biting each other. They also use olfactory communication, such as marking their territory via urine.

Vocal communication

Visual communication

GAto.jpg
A cat hissing and arching its back to make itself appear larger to ward off a threat
Surprised cat.jpg
Surprised cats have enlarged pupils and erect ears held slightly backward
Arched Cyprus and Tabby.jpg
A Tabby cat hissing at a Cyprus cat; the Cyprus cat is arched with erect hair to appear larger and threatening
Abby playing in basket.jpg
This tortoiseshell cat has erect hairs on her tail and back. This indicates excitement or curiosity
Cat Cuddle Grey Brown.jpg
Two cats lay with each other to conserve body heat, notice how they lay on their stomach and side indicating relaxation

Cats use body language and movement to communicate a wide range of feelings and information. There are various responses such as when cats arch their backs, erect their hairs and adopt a sideward posture to communicate fear or aggression. Other visual communication can be a single behavioral change (as perceived by humans) such as slowly blinking to signal relaxation and comfort in their environment. Domestic cats frequently use visual communication with their eyes, ears, mouths, tails, coats and body postures. The change in a cat's facial features can be a strong indicator of their communication. [16]

Body language

A cat's posture can be friendly or aggressive, depending on the situation. Some of the most basic and familiar cat postures include: [27] [28]

Ears

Cats can change the position of their ears very quickly, and continuously. They are erect when the cat is alert and focused, slightly relaxed when the cat is calm, and flattened against the head when extremely defensive or aggressive. [33] In cats, flattened ears generally indicate that it feels threatened and may attack used as a defense or attack posture. [34]

Eyes

A direct stare by a cat usually communicates a challenge or threat and is more likely to be seen in high-ranking cats; lower-ranking cats usually withdraw in response. [16] The direct stare is often used during predation or for territorial reasons. In contrast to a direct stare, cats will lower their eyelids or slowly blink them to show trust and affection to their owners. [35]

Tail

Cats often use their tails to communicate. For example, a cat holding its tail vertically generally indicates positive emotions such as happiness or confidence; the vertical tail is often used as a friendly gesture toward people or other cats. Additionally, a cat may twitch its tail when playing. [36] A half-raised tail can indicate less pleasure, and discontent is indicated with a tail held low. A cat's tail may swing from side to side, and if this motion is slow and "lazy", it generally indicates that the cat is in a relaxed state. Cats will twitch the tip of their tails when hunting, alert, or playful. A stalking domestic cat will typically hold its tail low to the ground while in a crouch, and twitch it quickly from side to side. This tail behavior is also observed when a cat becomes "irritated" and is about to lash out and attack typically done by biting or scratching with the claws extended.

When playing, a cat, usually a kitten, may raise the base of its tail high and stiffen all but the tip into a shape like an upside-down "U". This signals great excitement, to the point of hyperactivity. It may also be seen when younger cats chase each other, or when they run around by themselves. When greeting their owners, cats often hold their tails straight up with a quivering motion that indicates extreme happiness. [37] A scared or surprised cat may erect the hairs on its tail and back. It may stand more upright and turn its body sideways to increase its apparent size as a threat. Tailless cats, such as the Manx, which possess only a small stub of a tail, move the stub around as if they have a full tail.

Panting

A cat panting Una gata jadeando.JPG
A cat panting

Unlike dogs, panting is a rare occurrence in cats, except in warm weather environments, or after delivery. Some cats may pant in response to anxiety, fear or excitement. It can also be caused by play, exercise, or stress from stimuli, such as car rides. Panting in cat-moms after delivery is normal and not related to temperature, female cats may pant for several days up to weeks postpartum. [38]

However, if panting is excessive or the cat appears in distress, it may be a symptom of a more serious condition, such as a nasal blockage, heartworm disease, head trauma, or drug poisoning. [39] In many cases, feline panting, especially if accompanied by other symptoms, such as coughing or shallow breathing (dyspnea), is considered to be abnormal and is treated as a medical emergency. [40]

Tactile communication

Grooming

Cat grooming itself

Cats often lick other cats as social grooming or to bond (this grooming is usually done between familiar cats). They also sometimes lick humans, which may indicate affection. Oral grooming for domestic and feral cats is a common behavior. Domestic cats spend about 8% of waking time grooming themselves. [41]

Grooming is extremely important not only to clean themselves but also to control ectoparasites. Fleas tend to be the most common ectoparasite in cats and some studies show indirect evidence that grooming in cats is effective in dislodging fleas from the head and neck. Cats may also use grooming to scratch itchy areas of the body. [42]

Kneading

Cats sometimes repeatedly tread their front paws on humans or soft objects with a kneading action. This is instinctive to kittens and adults and is presumably derived from the action used to stimulate milk let-down from the mother during nursing. Kittens "knead" the breast while suckling, using the forelimbs one at a time in an alternating pattern to push against the mammary glands to stimulate lactation. Cats have scent glands on the underside of their paws and when they knead or scratch objects or people, it is likely these pheromones are transferred to the person or object being kneaded or scratched.

Cats carry these infantile behaviors beyond nursing and into adulthood. Some cats "nurse", that is, suck on clothing or bedding during kneading. The cat exerts firm downward pressure with its paw, spreading its toes to expose its claws, then curls its toes as it lifts its paw. The process takes place with alternate paws at intervals of one to two seconds. They may knead while sitting on their owner's lap, which may be painful if the cat has sharp claws.

Since most of the preferred "domestic traits" are neotenous or juvenile traits that persist in the adults, kneading may be a relic juvenile behavior retained in adult domestic cats. [43] It may also stimulate the cat and make it feel good, similar to a human stretching. Kneading is often a precursor to sleeping, and many cats purr while kneading, usually taken to indicate contentment and affection. They also purr mostly when newborn, when feeding, or when trying to feed on their mother's teat. The common association between the two behaviors may confirm the evidence in favor of the origin of kneading as a remnant instinct. [44]

Bunting

Cats sometimes "head-bump" humans or other cats with the front part of the head; this action is referred to as "bunting". [45] This communication might have an olfactory component as there are scent glands in this area of the body, and is possibly for seeking attention when the cat turns its head down or to the side. [46] Some cats also rub their faces on humans as a friendly greeting or indicating affection. This tactile action is combined with olfactory communication as the contact leaves scent around the mouth and cheeks.

Head-bumping and cheek rubbing may be displays of social dominance as they are often exhibited by a dominant cat toward a subordinate. [16]

Touching noses, sometimes referred to as "sniffing noses", as a sign of affection and to mark territory. [47]

Biting

Gentle biting (often accompanied by purring and kneading) can communicate affection or playfulness, directed at the human owner or another cat. Stronger bites that are often accompanied by hissing or growling usually communicate aggression. [48] When cats mate, the tom bites the scruff of the female's neck as she assumes a lordosis position which communicates that she is receptive to mating.

Olfactory communication

Scent rubbing Rubbing herself on a lilac bush (5625025069).jpg
Scent rubbing

Cats communicate through scent using urine, feces, and chemicals or pheromones from glands located around the mouth, chin, forehead, cheeks, lower back, tail and paws. [49] Their rubbing and head-bumping behaviors are methods of depositing these scents on substrates, including humans. The cat rubs its cheeks on prominent objects in the preferred territory, depositing a chemical pheromone produced in glands in the cheeks. This is known as a contentment pheromone. Synthetic versions of the feline facial pheromone are available commercially. [50]

Cats have nine different scent glands in their body. These are the pinna (outer ear flaps), temporal (on their temples), cheek (on the sides of their face), perioral (on the mouth corners), submandibular (under the jaw), interdigital (between toes), anal (on the sides of the anus), caudal (all along tail), and supra-caudal (at the base of tail). [51]

Urine spraying is also a territorial marking. [52] Cats urinate by squatting onto a horizontal surface, while standing up. Unlike a dog's penis, a cat's penis points backward. Although cats may mark with both sprayed and non-sprayed urine, the spray is usually more thick and oily than normally deposited urine, and may contain additional secretions from anal sacs that help the cat make a stronger communication. While cats mark their territory both by rubbing the scent glands, by urine and fecal deposits, spraying seems to be the "loudest" feline olfactory communication. It is most frequently observed in intact male cats in competition with other males. Males neutered in adulthood may still spray after neutering. Female cats also sometimes spray. [16]

A cat that urinates outside the litter box may indicate dissatisfaction with the box, due to a variety of factors such as substrate texture, cleanliness, and privacy. It can also be a sign of urinary tract problems. Male cats on poor diets are susceptible to crystal formation in the urine which can block the urethra and lead to health problems.

The urine of mature male cats in particular contains the amino acid known as felinine which is a precursor to 3-mercapto-3-methylbutan-1-ol (MMB), the sulfur-containing compound that gives cat urine its characteristically strong odor. Felinine is produced in the urine from 3-methylbutanol-cysteinylglycine (3-MBCG) that is excreted peptidase cauxin. It then slowly degrades via bacterial lyase into the more-volatile chemical MMB. [53] Felinine is a possible cat pheromone.

Felinine chemical structure.svg 3-mercapto-3-methylbutan-1-ol.svg
FelinineMMB

Socialization

Cats, domestic or wild, participate in social behaviors, even though it is thought that most cat species (besides lions) are solitary, antisocial animals. [54] These include socialization between humans and other cats, social learning, and conflicts.

There are 52 measured cat personality traits in cats, with one study showing that five reliable personality factors were found using principal-axis factor-analysis: neuroticism, extroversion, dominance, impulsiveness and agreeableness. [55]

Humans

Cats between the age of three and nine weeks are sensitive to human socialization. [56] After this period, socialization can be less effective. [57] Studies have shown that the earlier the kitten is handled by people, the less fearful the kitten will be toward people. [57] Other factors that can enhance socialization are having many people handle the kitten frequently, the presence of the mother, and feeding. [56] [57] The presence of the mother is important because cats are observational learners. For example, a mother that is comfortable around humans can reduce anxiety in the kitten and promote the kitten-human relationship. [56]

There are a number of problematic behaviors that affect the human-cat relationship. One behavior is when cats attack people by scratching and biting. [58] This often occurs spontaneously or could be triggered by sudden movements. [58] Another problematic behavior is the "petting and biting syndrome", which involves the cat being petted and then suddenly attacking and running away. [58] Other problems are house soiling, scratching furniture, and bringing dead prey into the house. [59] It is these kinds of behaviors that put a strain on the relationship between cats and people.

Feral kittens around two to seven weeks old can be socialized usually within a month of capture. [60] Some species of cats cannot be socialized because of factors such as genetic influence and in some cases specific learning experiences. [60] The best way to get a kitten to socialize is to handle the kitten for many hours a week. [60] The process is made easier if there is another socialized cat present but not necessarily in the same space as the one being socialized. If the handler can get a cat to urinate in the litter tray, then the others in a litter will usually follow. Initial contact with thick gloves is highly recommended until trust is established, usually within the first week. On the other hand, it is a challenge to socialize an adult cat. This is because socialized adult feral cats tend to trust only those who they trusted in their socialization period, and therefore can be very fearful around strangers. [60]

Social learning

Cats are observational learners. [61] [58] This type of learning emerges early in a cat's life, [62] and has been shown in many laboratory studies. Young kittens learn to hunt from their mothers by observing their techniques when catching prey. [61] The mother ensures their kittens learn hunting techniques by first bringing dead prey to the litter, followed by live prey. With the live prey, she demonstrates the techniques required for successful capture. [61] Prey-catching behavior of kittens improves at higher levels over time when their mothers are present. [63]

Observational learning for cats can be described in terms of the drive to complete the behavior, the cue that initiates the behavior, the response to the cue, and the reward for completing the behavior. [62] This is shown when cats learn predatory behavior from their mothers. The drive is hunger, the cue is the prey, the response is to catch the prey, and the reward is to relieve the hunger sensation.

Kittens also show observational learning when they are socializing with humans. They are more likely to initiate socialization with humans when their mothers are exhibiting non-aggressive and non-defensive behaviors. [61] Even though mothers spend more time with their kittens, male cats play an important role by breaking up fights among littermates. [61]

Observational learning is not limited to kitten-hood – it can also be observed during adulthood. Studies have shown that adult cats that see others performing a task, such as pressing a lever after a visual cue, learn to perform the same task faster than those who did not witness another cat at that task. [58]

Dominance

Two cats fighting Fighting cat (crop).jpg
Two cats fighting
A conflict situation of one tomcat meeting another, making loud calls
Domestic cats fighting shortly after being introduced to each other

Dominance can be seen among domestic cats in multi-cat households. "Subordinate" cats submit to the "dominant" cat. Dominance includes such behaviors as the submissive cats walking around the dominant cat, waiting for the dominant cat to walk past them, avoiding eye contact, crouching, lying on their side (defensive posture), and retreating when the dominant cat approaches. [61] Dominant cats present a specific body posture as well. The cat displays ears straight up, the base of its tail will be arched, and it looks directly at subordinate cats. [61] These dominant cats are usually not aggressive, but if a subordinate cat blocks the food source they may become aggressive. [64] When this aggressive behavior occurs, it could also lead to the dominant cat preventing subordinate cats from eating and using the litter box. [61] This can cause the subordinate cat to defecate somewhere else and create problems with human interaction. [61]

Usually, when strange cats meet, one of them makes a sudden move that puts the other cat into a defensive mode. The cat will then draw in on itself and prepare to attack if needed. [65] The submissive cat will usually run away before a physical altercation ensues. This is not always the case, and what is known as a "tomcat duel" may follow. [65] Dominance is also seen as an underlying factor that depict how conspecifics interact with each other.

Conflict

Social conflict among cats depends solely on the behavior of the cats. Some research has shown that cats rarely pick fights, but when they do, it is usually for protecting food and/or litters, and defending their territory. [64] Fights can happen between two females or between a male and a female. Cats may need to be reintroduced or separated to avoid fights in a closed household.

The first sign of an imminent tomcat duel is when both cats draw themselves up high on their legs, all hair along the middle of their backs is raised straight up, and they mew and howl loudly as they approach one another. [65] The steps the cats make become slower and shorter, as they get closer to one another. Once they are close enough to attack, they pause slightly, and then one cat leaps and tries to bite the nape of the other cat. [65] The other cat has no choice but to retaliate and both cats roll aggressively on the ground. During such confrontations both cats produce loud intense screams. [65] After some time, the cats separate and stand face to face to begin the attack all over again. This can go on for some time until one remains seated, showing defeat. [65] The defeated cat does not move until the victor completes sniffing the area and moves outside the fighting area. Once this happens, the defeated cat leaves the area, ending the fight. [65]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat</span> Small domesticated carnivorous mammal

The cat, commonly referred to as the domestic cat or house cat, is a small domesticated carnivorous mammal. It is the only domesticated species of the family Felidae. Recent advances in archaeology and genetics have shown that the domestication of the cat occurred in the Near East around 7500 BC. It is commonly kept as a house pet and farm cat, but also ranges freely as a feral cat avoiding human contact. It is valued by humans for companionship and its ability to kill vermin. Its retractable claws are adapted to killing small prey like mice and rats. It has a strong, flexible body, quick reflexes, sharp teeth, and its night vision and sense of smell are well developed. It is a social species, but a solitary hunter and a crepuscular predator. Cat communication includes vocalizations like meowing, purring, trilling, hissing, growling, and grunting as well as cat body language. It can hear sounds too faint or too high in frequency for human ears, such as those made by small mammals. It also secretes and perceives pheromones.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

A purr or whirr is a tonal fluttering sound made by some species of felids, including both larger, outdoor cats and the domestic cat, as well as two species of genets. It varies in loudness and tone among species and in the same animal. In smaller and domestic cats it is known as a purr, while in larger felids, such as the panther, it is called a whirr.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog communication</span> Communication of dogs with other dogs and as well as humans

Dog communication is the transfer of information between dogs, as well as between dogs and humans. Behaviors associated with dog communication are categorized into visual and vocal. Visual communication includes mouth shape and head position, licking and sniffing, ear and tail positioning, eye gaze, facial expression, and body posture. Dog vocalizations, or auditory communication, can include barks, growls, howls, whines and whimpers, screams, pants and sighs. Dogs also communicate via gustatory communication, utilizing scent and pheromones.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bark (sound)</span> Sound mainly produced by Dogs

A bark is a sound most often produced by dogs. Other animals that make this noise include, but are not limited to, wolves, coyotes, foxes, seals, frogs, and barking owls. "Bark" is also a verb that describes the sound of many canids.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Kneading (cats)</span> Feline behavior of alternately pushing out and pulling in front paws

Kneading is a behavior frequently observed in domestic cats where, when a cat feels at ease, it may push out and pull in its front paws against a surface such as furniture or carpet, often alternating between right and left limbs. Each stroke is accompanied by a grasping motion of the claws as if the cat were kneading dough.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Emotion in animals</span> Research into similarities between non-human and human emotions

Emotion is defined as any mental experience with high intensity and high hedonic content. The existence and nature of emotions in non-human animals are believed to be correlated with those of humans and to have evolved from the same mechanisms. Charles Darwin was one of the first scientists to write about the subject, and his observational approach has since developed into a more robust, hypothesis-driven, scientific approach. Cognitive bias tests and learned helplessness models have shown feelings of optimism and pessimism in a wide range of species, including rats, dogs, cats, rhesus macaques, sheep, chicks, starlings, pigs, and honeybees. Jaak Panksepp played a large role in the study of animal emotion, basing his research on the neurological aspect. Mentioning seven core emotional feelings reflected through a variety of neuro-dynamic limbic emotional action systems, including seeking, fear, rage, lust, care, panic and play. Through brain stimulation and pharmacological challenges, such emotional responses can be effectively monitored.

Growling is a low, guttural vocalization produced by animals as an aggressive warning but can also be found in other contexts such as playful behaviors or mating. Different animals will use growling in specific contexts as a form of communication. In humans, low or dull rumbling noises may also be emitted when they are discontent with something or they are angry, although this human sound is often termed "groaning".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog behavior</span> Internally coordinated responses of dogs to internal and external stimuli

Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli. It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles. As a result of this physical and social evolution, dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans. Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social-cognitive abilities in domestic dogs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat behavior</span> Behavior of cats

Cat behavior encompasses the actions and reactions displayed by a cat in response to various stimuli and events. Cat behavior includes body language, elimination habits, aggression, play, communication, hunting, grooming, urine marking, and face rubbing. It varies among individuals, colonies, and breeds.

A cat pheromone is a chemical molecule, or compound, that is used by cats and other felids for communication. These pheromones are produced and detected specifically by the body systems of cats and evoke certain behavioural responses.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat play and toys</span> Entertainment of domestic cats

Cat play and toys incorporates predatory games of "play aggression". Cats' behaviors when playing are similar to hunting behaviors. These activities allow kittens and younger cats to grow and acquire cognitive and motor skills, and to socialize with other cats. Cat play behavior can be either solitary or social. They can play with a multitude of toys ranging from strings, to small furry toys resembling prey, to plastic bags.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Meow</span> Vocalization by cats

A meow or miaow is a cat vocalization. Meows may have diverse tones in terms of their sound, and what is heard can vary from being chattered to calls, murmurs, and whispers. Adult cats rarely meow to each other. Thus, an adult cat meowing to human beings is generally considered a post-domestication extension of meowing by kittens: a call for attention. Felines usually communicate with each other via their shared sense of smell, yet with people they often make verbal cues around behavior, such as having a specific sound indicate a desire to go outside.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat training</span> Modifying a cats behavior

Cat training is the process of modifying a domestic cat's behavior for entertainment or companionship purposes. Training is commonly used to reduce unwanted or problematic behaviors in domestic cats, to enhance interactions between humans and pet cats, and to allow them to coexist comfortably. There are various methods for training cats which employ different balances between reward and punishment.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Abyssinian cat</span> Breed of cat

The Abyssinian is a breed of domestic short-haired cat with a distinctive "ticked" tabby coat, in which individual hairs are banded with different colors. They are also known simply as Abys.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog-like cat</span> Behavioral pattern found in domestic cats

Dog-like cat is a term in popular culture that refers to specific breeds of domestic cats that have unusual behavioral tendencies that are reminiscent of young domestic dogs. These are within the scope of feline behavior, and may be enhanced through selective breeding. These behaviors, not specific to any breed, include following people around from room to room, the desire to receive frequent moments of physical affection such as being held and petted, a lack of aggression toward some fellow animals, and a placid nature. Certain owners may successfully attempt to train their cats to perform on command, such as via clicker training, akin to canines and other domesticated animals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bunting (animal behavior)</span>

Bunting is a form of animal behavior, often found in felids, in which the animal butts or rubs its head against other things, including people. Bunting as a behaviour can be viewed as a variation of scent rubbing. This is when an animal, typically a carnivore, will rub its back on a scent, such as that of prey, or on the urine of an animal of the same species. Evolutionarily speaking, scent rubbing is the oldest form of scent communication and bunting is a derivative of this behaviour. Rolling in the scent of another animal was an adaptation to camouflage the scent of a predator or outside male, in order to get closer to mates.

The body language of dogs is one form of non-verbal communication whereby dogs can express emotions and intentions through bodily movements. It refers to the interpretation of posture and behaviour of species in the genus Canis. This form of visual communication is generally used for identifying emotions and intentions of domestic dogs, though it can also be applied to wild canines such as wolves. Understanding the body language of dogs is particularly important in preventing dog bites, especially of children. This communication can occur between dogs, or during a dog-human interaction. Such movements primarily involve the tail, the ears, and the head/body. Tail-wagging is a common tail movement used by dogs to communicate. Additionally, ear flattening or heightening are typical movements made using the ears. In terms of the head/body, it is of interest to study turning of the head, as well as the overall posture of the dog.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Scent rubbing</span> Behaviour where a mammal rubs its body against an object in their environment

Scent rubbing is a behavior where a mammal rubs its body against an object in their environment, sometimes in ones covered with strongly odored substances. It is typically shown in carnivores, although many mammals exhibit this behavior. Lowering shoulders, collapsing the forelegs, pushing forward and rubbing the chin, temples, neck, or back is how this act is performed. A variety of different odors can elicit this behavior including feces, vomit, fresh or decaying meat, insecticide, urine, repellent, ashes, human food and so on. Scent rubbing can be produced by an animal smelling novel odors, which include manufactured smells such as perfume or motor oil and carnivore smells including feces and food smells.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wolf communication</span> Overview about the communication of wolves

Wolves communicate using vocalizations, body postures, scent, touch, and taste. The lunar phases have no effect on wolf vocalisation. Despite popular belief, wolves do not howl at the Moon. Gray wolves howl to assemble the pack, usually before and after hunts, to pass on an alarm particularly at a den site, to locate each other during a storm or while crossing unfamiliar territory, and to communicate across great distances. Other vocalisations include growls, barks and whines. Wolves do not bark as loudly or continuously as dogs do but they bark a few times and then retreat from a perceived danger. Aggressive or self-assertive wolves are characterized by their slow and deliberate movements, high body posture and raised hackles, while submissive ones carry their bodies low, sleeken their fur, and lower their ears and tail. Raised leg urination is considered to be one of the most important forms of scent communication in the wolf, making up 60–80% of all scent marks observed.

References

  1. Turner, Dennis C.; Bateson, Paul Patrick Gordon (2000-06-08). The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour. Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-521-63648-3.
  2. "How do cats communicate with each other?". Library of Congress, Washington, D.C. 20540 USA. Retrieved 2024-03-05.
  3. Tavernier, Chloé; Ahmed, Sohail; Houpt, Katherine Albro; Yeon, Seong Chan (2020). "Feline vocal communication". Journal of Veterinary Science. 21 (1): e18. doi:10.4142/jvs.2020.21.e18. ISSN   1229-845X. PMC   7000907 . PMID   32017479.
  4. Moelk, Mildred (1944). "Vocalizing in the House-Cat; A Phonetic and Functional Study". The American Journal of Psychology. 57 (2): 184–205. doi:10.2307/1416947. ISSN   0002-9556. JSTOR   1416947.
  5. Brown, K. A.; Buchwald, J. S.; Johnson, J. R.; Mikolich, D. J. (November 1978). "Vocalization in the cat and kitten". Developmental Psychobiology. 11 (6): 559–570. doi:10.1002/dev.420110605. ISSN   0012-1630. PMID   720761.
  6. Owens, Jessica L.; Olsen, Mariana; Fontaine, Amy; Kloth, Christopher; Kershenbaum, Arik; Waller, Sara (June 2017). "Visual classification of feral cat Felis silvestris catus vocalizations". Current Zoology. 63 (3): 331–339. doi:10.1093/cz/zox013. ISSN   1674-5507. PMC   5804184 . PMID   29491992.
  7. "Whisker Whispers | Association of Animal Behavior Professionals". 2013-11-05. Archived from the original on 2013-11-05. Retrieved 2024-03-05.
  8. Herbst, Christian T.; Svec, Jan G.; Fitch, Tecumseh (2023). "About the voice production mechanism of cat purring – a critical appraisal of Remmers & Gautier, 1972". doi:10.25365/phaidra.420.{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  9. "What Does It Mean When A Cat Purrs Loudly? - Top Cat Breeds". topcatbreeds.com. 27 September 2020.
  10. "Why Do Cats Purr". 31 August 2018. Archived from the original on 1 March 2019. Retrieved 4 November 2023.
  11. Turner, Dennis C.; Bateson, Paul Patrick Gordon (2000-06-08). The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour. Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-521-63648-3.
  12. Herbst, Christian T.; Prigge, Tamara; Garcia, Maxime; Hampala, Vit; Hofer, Riccardo; Weissengruber, Gerald E.; Svec, Jan G.; Fitch, W. Tecumseh (November 2023). "Domestic cat larynges can produce purring frequencies without neural input". Current Biology. 33 (21): 4727–4732.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.09.014 . PMID   37794583.
  13. "Textbook of Veterinary Anatomy by Dyce, K.M., Sack, W.O. and Wensing, C.J.G." Equine Veterinary Journal. 20 (5): 387. September 1988. doi:10.1111/j.2042-3306.1988.tb01554.x. ISSN   0425-1644.
  14. 1 2 Bradshaw, John W.S. (January 2016). "Sociality in cats: A comparative review". Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 11: 113–124. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2015.09.004.
  15. 1 2 Schötz, Susanne; van de Weijer, Joost; Eklund, Robert (25 August 2017). Phonetic Characteristics of Domestic Cat Vocalisations (PDF). 1st Intl. Workshop on Vocal interactivity in-and-between Humans, Animals and Robots (PDF). pp. 5–6. ISBN   9782956202905.
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Miller, P. (2000). "Whisker whispers". Association of Animal Behavior Professionals. Archived from the original on 5 November 2013. Retrieved 5 November 2013.
  17. Brown, Kenneth A.; Buchwald, Jennifer S.; Johnson, Judith R.; Mikolich, Dennis J. (November 1978). "Vocalization in the cat and kitten". Developmental Psychobiology. 11 (6): 559–570. doi:10.1002/dev.420110605. PMID   720761.
  18. 1 2 Rennex, Michelle (12 February 2020). "People Are Seriously Convinced That Their Cats Are Speaking English". Junkee. Retrieved 5 November 2022.
  19. Laurent, Anaëlle (December 8, 2020). "Can Cats Speak English? - Cats That Mimic The Human Language". animalwised.com. Retrieved 5 November 2022.
  20. Szenczi, P.; Bánszegi, O.; Urrutia, A.; Faragó, T.; Hudson, R. (2016). "Mother–offspring recognition in the domestic cat: Kittens recognize their own mother's call". Developmental Psychobiology. 58 (5): 568–577. doi:10.1002/dev.21402. PMID   26935009.
  21. Schötz, S. (2013). "A phonetic pilot study of chirp, chatter, tweet and tweedle in three domestic cats". Fonetik: 65–68.
  22. "Why Do Cats Chatter at Birds?". The Spruce Pets. Retrieved 2024-03-18.
  23. "Meowing and Yowling". Virtual Pet Behaviorist. ASPCA . Retrieved 28 May 2012.
  24. "caterwaul". Dictionary.com. Retrieved 28 May 2012.
  25. "Stages of Kitten Development". Alley Cat Rescue. Retrieved 23 September 2021.
  26. Schötz, S. (June 2015). "Agonistic vocalisations in domestic cats: A case study". Proceedings from Fonetik 2015: Lund, June 8–10, 2015. Lund, Sweden: Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University. pp. 85–90.
  27. "Test to determine how well you know feline body language". Archived from the original on 20 June 2018. Retrieved 12 September 2016.
  28. An Ethogram for Behavioral Studies of the Domestic Cat. Animal Welfare Research Report No 8. UFAW, UK Cat Behavior Working Group. 1995.
  29. "Reading Your Cat". Animal Planet . Warner Bros. Discovery. Archived from the original on 21 November 2011. Retrieved 22 December 2011.
  30. Little, Susan E. (2011). "Female Reproduction". In Little, Susan E. (ed.). The Cat: Clinical Medicine and Management. Saint Louis: W. B. Saunders. pp. 1195–1227. doi:10.1016/b978-1-4377-0660-4.00040-5. ISBN   9781437706604. PMC   7158189 .{{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  31. Fraser, Andrew (2012). Feline Behaviour and Welfare. Wallingford, Oxfordshire: CAB International. p. 58.
  32. Helgren, J. Anne (1999). Communicating with Your Cat . Barron's Educational Series. ISBN   9780764108556 via Internet Archive.
  33. Morris, Desmond (1986). Catwatching. Three Rivers Press. ISBN   9780517880531.
  34. "Aggression in Cats". ASPCA.org. American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals.
  35. JacobP (2023-06-28). "Cat Slow Blinking: Why Your Cat Blinks Slowly at You". Veterinary Healthcare Associates. Retrieved 2024-03-18.
  36. "Cat articles on Iams website". Archived from the original on 25 February 2010. Retrieved 30 November 2009.
  37. "Common Cat Behaviors". Best Cat Tips. www.best-cat-tips.com. Archived from the original on 3 May 2012. Retrieved 28 May 2012.
  38. Vine, Louis L. (1992). Common Sense Book of Complete Cat Care. Quill/William Morrow. p. 66. ISBN   9780688116187 . Retrieved 31 May 2021.
  39. Spielman, Bari. "Panting in Cats: Is It Normal?". PetPlace.com. Retrieved 7 January 2010.
  40. "Cat Panting Explained". The Cat Health Guide. Retrieved 2 July 2011.
  41. "Why Does My Cat Lick Me? 7 Common Reasons". www.petmd.com. Retrieved 2024-03-18.
  42. Eckstein, Robert A.; Hart, Benjamin L. (2000). "Grooming and Control of Fleas in Cats". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 68 (2): 141–150. doi:10.1016/s0168-1591(00)00095-2. PMID   10771322.
  43. Schwartz, Stefanie (June 2003). "Separation anxiety syndrome in dogs and cats". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 222 (11): 1526–32. doi:10.2460/javma.2003.222.1526. PMID   12784957. S2CID   37324864.
  44. McPherson, F.J; Chenoweth, P.J (April 2012). "Mammalian sexual dimorphism". Animal Reproduction Science. 131 (3–4): 109–122. doi:10.1016/j.anireprosci.2012.02.007. PMID   22482798.
  45. Johnson-Bennett, Pam (3 May 2012). "Bunting Behavior" . Retrieved 30 March 2015.
  46. White, Mary. "Cat Behavior Tips". LifeTips. Retrieved 28 May 2012.
  47. McGonagle, Joanne (2024-01-12). "Cat Nose Tap: Why Do Cats Touch Noses". The Tiniest Tiger. Retrieved 2024-03-18.
  48. "Play Therapy Pt. 2". Cats International. Archived from the original on 19 April 2007. Retrieved 22 May 2007.
  49. "Communication – How do cats communicate?". VetWest Animal Hospitals. 2 February 2008. Retrieved 5 November 2013.
  50. "Cat Pheromones – Synthetic Feline Facial Pheromones". PetMD.com. 20 June 2018. Archived from the original on 20 June 2018. Retrieved 11 November 2019.
  51. "Scent Glands on Cats |" . Retrieved 28 December 2019.
  52. Turner, Dennis C.; Bateman, Patrick, eds. (2000). The Domestic Cat (2nd ed.). University Press, Cambridge. pp. 69–70. ISBN   9780521636483 . Retrieved 28 May 2012.
  53. Miyazaki, M.; Yamashita, T.; Suzuki, Y.; Saito, Y.; Soeta, S.; Taira, H.; Suzuki, A. (October 2006). "A major urinary protein of the domestic cat regulates the production of felinine, a putative pheromone precursor". Chemistry & Biology. 13 (10): 1071–9. doi: 10.1016/j.chembiol.2006.08.013 . PMID   17052611.
  54. Spotte, Stephen (2014). Free-ranging Cats: Behaviour, Ecology & Management. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 49–59. ISBN   9781118884010.
  55. Litchfield, Carla A.; Quinton, Gillian; Tindle, Hayley; Chiera, Belinda; Kikillus, K. Heidy; Roetman, Philip (23 August 2017). "The 'Feline Five': An exploration of personality in pet cats (Felis catus)". PLOS ONE. 12 (8): e0183455. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1283455L. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183455 . PMC   5568325 . PMID   28832622.
  56. 1 2 3 Turner, Dennis C.; Bateson, Patrick (1988). The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour . Cambridge University Press. pp.  112–113, 159–168. ISBN   9780521354479 via Internet Archive.
  57. 1 2 3 Bernstein, Penny L. (2007). "The Human–Cat Relationship". The Welfare of Cats. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. pp. 47–89. ISBN   9781402032271.
  58. 1 2 3 4 5 Bradshaw, John W. S. (1992). The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat. Wallingford, Oxfordshire: CAB International. pp. 78, 198–200. ISBN   085198715X.
  59. Heath, Sarah E. (2007). "Behaviour Problems and Welfare". The Welfare of Cats. Springer, Dordrecht. pp. 91–107. ISBN   9781402032271.
  60. 1 2 3 4 Casey, Rachel; Bradshaw, John (November 2008). "The effects of additional socialisation for kittens in a rescue centre on their behaviour and suitability as a pet". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 114 (1–2): 196–205. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.003.
  61. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Crowell-Davis, Sharon L. (2007). "Cat Behaviour: Social Organization, Communication & Development". The Welfare of Cats. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. ISBN   9781402032271.
  62. 1 2 Alder, H. E. (1955). "Some Factors of Observational Learning". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 86 (1): 159–177. doi:10.1080/00221325.1955.10532903. PMID   14354164. ProQuest   1297110751.
  63. Caro, T. M. (1980). "Effects of the Mother, Object Play, and Adult Experience on Predation in Cats". Behavioral and Neural Biology. 29 (1): 29–51. doi:10.1016/S0163-1047(80)92456-5. PMID   7387584.
  64. 1 2 Beadle, Muriel (1977). The Cat: History, Biology, and Behaviour . New York: Simon and Schuster. pp.  100–111. ISBN   9780671224516 via Internet Archive.
  65. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Leyhausen, Paul (1979). Cat Behaviour: The Predatory & Social Behaviour of Domestic & Wild Cats. New York: Garland Publishing. pp. 164–216, 227–231. ISBN   9780824070175.