Pueblo linguistic area

Last updated

The Pueblo linguistic area (or Pueblo Sprachbund, Pueblo convergence area) is a Sprachbund (group of languages with similarities due to language contact) consisting of the languages spoken in and near North American Pueblo locations. There are also many shared cultural practices in this area. For example, these cultures share many ceremonial vocabulary terms meant for prayer or song. [1]

Contents

Language membership

The languages of the linguistic area are the following:

The languages belong to five different families: Zuni, Tanoan, Keresan, Uto-Aztecan (Hopi), and Athabaskan (Navajo, from the Apachean subfamily). Zuni is a language isolate. Navajo is only a marginal member of the Sprachbund and does not share all its linguistic features. This is because the ancestors of the Navajo originate from the Athabaskan region (located in modern-day Canada and Alaska), and as a result they were relatively late in arriving to the Southwest compared to their Puebloan neighbors. Languages in the Tanoan and Apachean families, and additionally Hopi, can be compared to relatives not affected by this particular region's areal features as a reference for changes due to contact. [1]

Tanoan consists of Taos, Picurís, Tewa, and Jemez. Keresan consists of Eastern Keres and Western Keres. [2]

Shared linguistic traits and areal features

The following are the shared linguistic traits of the Pueblo Sprachbund:

Most languages have ejectives. Zuni may have developed ejectives due to contact with Tanoan and Keresan which both have complete series of ejectives: Zuni has /tsʼ,tʃʼ,kʼ,kʷʼ/ but lacks the ejectives /pʼ,tʼ/ found in the other languages. Taos and Picurís (both Tanoan) have /pʼ,tʼ,tʃʼ,kʼ,kʷʼ/; Tewa (Tanoan) has /pʼ,tʼ,tsʼ,kʼ,kʷʼ/; Jemez (Tanoan) has /pʼ,tʼ,kʼ/; [5] Keresan has /pʼ,tʼ,tsʼ,tʂʼ,tʃʼ,kʼ,sʼ,ʂʼ,ʃʼ/ (as well as glottalized sonorants /mʼ,nʼ,rʼ,wʼ,jʼ/). Hopi lacks ejectives completely. Navajo has /tʼ,tsʼ,tɬʼ,tʃʼ,kʼ,kʷʼ/. [6]

Navajo may have acquired the stop /kʷ/ from Puebloan contact. [3] [7] Sherzer suggests that contact led to Acoma Keres sharing glottalized sonorants and glides with Navajo. [8]

Zuni may have obtained its partial glottalized set and aspirants via contact with Tanoan and Keresan, which both have fully integrated series of glottals. [3]

Other shared traits include Santa Clara acquiring retroflex sounds from Keresan and the Navajo acquiring /hʷ/ from Tanoan contact. [1] Keresan may be the common source of glottalized nasals and semivowels in Navajo, and the development of /r/ in dialects of Tewa and Tiwa and possibly Hopi. [1]

Linguist Paul Kroskrity argues for diffused Apachean traits in Tewa, such as the passive marked by prefixes that are structured differently from those of other Tanoan languages. [3] [9] [10] [11] In these languages the verb is made intransitive by allowing an agent argument in the verb structure. [3] [9] [10] [11] This also includes patient subjects and cases where the subject must be animate, including raising animate objects to the place of inanimate subjects. [3] [9] [10] [11] Further, Navajo and Tewa are the only languages in this group to have a "recognizable anaphor as a relativizer" when forming relative clauses. [3] [12] Kroskrity also finds similarity in the Tewa possessive suffix -bí, which parallels the third person possessive prefix bi- found in Apachean languages. [13] [14] [10] [11] This can be seen in the following example provided by Kroskrity: [15] [14] [10] [11]

Diffused bi Morpheme
Arizona TewaNavajo

sen-bí

man-POSS

kʰaw

song

sen-bí kʰaw

man-POSS song

'(a) man's song'

bisóódi

pig

bi-tsi

POSS-flesh

bisóódi bi-tsi

pig POSS-flesh

'the pig's flesh'

This paralleling morpheme is also used in these languages' postpositional constructions. [15] [14] [10] [11] The reason for this diffusion has been attributed to trade networks and Apachean settlements near Pueblos in winter months. [15]

Vowels

Sherzer suggests that the 2-2-1 vowel system found in Tanoan languages (i u - e o - a) may be a result of contact with Zuni and Keresan language families. [3] Sherzer states, "A 2-2-1 vowel system is a Pueblo-centered regional areal trait. Its development in some Tanoan languages may be due to contact with Zuni and Keresan." [4]

See also

Notes

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Mithun 1999, p. 318.
  2. Keresan (or Keres) has variously been considered as a small family of two languages or a single language of dialect chaining.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Campbell 1997, p. 339.
  4. 1 2 Sherzer 1976, p. 133.
  5. In Jemez, Proto-Tanoan
    • c’ has merged with
    • t’ and
    • kʷ’ has merged with
    • kʷ explaining the lack of /tsʼ,kʷʼ/ in Jemez that are found in other Tanoan languages.
  6. The lack of /pʼ/ in Navajo is an inheritance from Proto-Athabascan.
  7. Sherzer 1976, p. 151-152.
  8. Sherzer 1976, p. 137.
  9. 1 2 3 Kroskrity 1982.
  10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Kroskrity 1985.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Kroskrity 1993, p. 60-66.
  12. Kroskrity 1982, p. 65.
  13. Campbell 1997, p. 339-340.
  14. 1 2 3 Kroskrity 1982, p. 66.
  15. 1 2 3 Campbell 1997, p. 340.

Bibliography

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Puebloans</span> Native Americans in the Southwestern United States

The Puebloans, or Pueblo peoples, are Native Americans in the Southwestern United States who share common agricultural, material, and religious practices. Among the currently inhabited Pueblos, Taos, San Ildefonso, Acoma, Zuni, and Hopi are some of the most commonly known. Pueblo people speak languages from four different language families, and each Pueblo is further divided culturally by kinship systems and agricultural practices, although all cultivate varieties of maize.

In phonetics, ejective consonants are usually voiceless consonants that are pronounced with a glottalic egressive airstream. In the phonology of a particular language, ejectives may contrast with aspirated, voiced and tenuis consonants. Some languages have glottalized sonorants with creaky voice that pattern with ejectives phonologically, and other languages have ejectives that pattern with implosives, which has led to phonologists positing a phonological class of glottalic consonants, which includes ejectives.

Hopi is a Uto-Aztecan language spoken by the Hopi people of northeastern Arizona, United States.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dental and alveolar ejective stops</span> Consonantal sounds represented by ⟨tʼ⟩ in IPA

The alveolar and dental ejective stops are types of consonantal sound, usually described as voiceless, that are pronounced with a glottalic egressive airstream. In the International Phonetic Alphabet, ejectives are indicated with a "modifier letter apostrophe" ⟨ʼ⟩, as in this article. A reversed apostrophe is sometimes used to represent light aspiration, as in Armenian linguistics ⟨p‘ t‘ k‘⟩; this usage is obsolete in the IPA. In other transcription traditions, the apostrophe represents palatalization: ⟨pʼ⟩ = IPA ⟨pʲ⟩. In some Americanist traditions, an apostrophe indicates weak ejection and an exclamation mark strong ejection: ⟨k̓, k!⟩. In the IPA, the distinction might be written ⟨kʼ, kʼʼ⟩, but it seems that no language distinguishes degrees of ejection.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tanoan languages</span> North American aboriginal language family

Tanoan, also Kiowa–Tanoan or Tanoan–Kiowa, is a family of languages spoken by indigenous peoples in present-day New Mexico, Kansas, Oklahoma, and Texas.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Zuni language</span> Language indigenous to New Mexico, US

Zuni is a language of the Zuni people, indigenous to western New Mexico and eastern Arizona in the United States. It is spoken by around 9,500 people, especially in the vicinity of Zuni Pueblo, New Mexico, and much smaller numbers in parts of Arizona.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Keres language</span> Language isolate of New Mexico, United States

Keres, also Keresan, is a Native American language, spoken by the Keres Pueblo people in New Mexico. Depending on the analysis, Keres is considered a small language family or a language isolate with several dialects. The varieties of each of the seven Keres pueblos are mutually intelligible with its closest neighbors. There are significant differences between the Western and Eastern groups, which are sometimes counted as separate languages.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tewa</span> Ethnic group of Pueblo Native Americans

The Tewa are a linguistic group of Pueblo Native Americans who speak the Tewa language and share the Pueblo culture. Their homelands are on or near the Rio Grande in New Mexico north of Santa Fe. They comprise the following communities:

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Chakwaina</span>

Chakwaina is a kachina which appears in Hopi, Zuni, and Keresan ceremonies, but does not appear in Tewa ceremonies. Although imagery of the kachina is varied, it is usually depicted as an ogre, with ferocious teeth and a black goatee and black mask with yellow eyes. Its spread throughout Pueblo culture is often associated with the Asa clan.

The Hopi-Tewa are a Tewa Pueblo group that resides on the eastern part of the Hopi Reservation on or near First Mesa in northeastern Arizona.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pueblo clown</span> Generic term for jesters or tricksters in the Kachina religion of the Pueblo natives

The Pueblo clowns are jesters or tricksters in the Kachina religion. It is a generic term, as there are a number of these figures in the ritual practice of the Pueblo people. Each has a unique role; belonging to separate Kivas and each has a name that differs from one mesa or pueblo to another.

Taos is a Tanoan language spoken by several hundred people in New Mexico, in the United States. The main description of its phonology was contributed by George L. Trager in a (pre-generative) structuralist framework. Earlier considerations of the phonetics-phonology were by John P. Harrington and Jaime de Angulo. Trager's first account was in Trager (1946) based on fieldwork 1935-1937, which was then substantially revised in Trager (1948). The description below takes Trager (1946) as the main point of departure and notes where this differs from the analysis of Trager (1948). Harrington's description is more similar to Trager (1946). Certain comments from a generative perspective are noted in a comparative work Hale (1967).

Picuris is a language of the Northern Tiwa branch of Tanoan spoken in Picuris Pueblo, New Mexico.

The phonology of the Zuni language as spoken in the southwestern United States is described here. Phonology is a branch of linguistics that studies how languages or dialects systematically organize their sounds.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Linguistic areas of the Americas</span>

The indigenous languages of the Americas form various linguistic areas or Sprachbunds that share various common (areal) traits.

Arizona, a state in the southwestern region of the United States of America, is known for its high population of Native Americans. Arizona has the third highest number of Native Americans of any state in the Union. Out of the entire US population of 2.9 million Native Americans, roughly 286,680 live in Arizona, representing 10% of the country's total Native American population. Only California and Oklahoma have more Native Americans than Arizona by number. Arizona also has the highest proportion of land allocated to Native American reservations, at 28%. Arizona has five of the twelve largest Indian reservations in the United States, including the largest, the Navajo Nation, and the third-largest, the Tohono O'odham Nation. Also, Arizona has the largest number of Native American language speakers in the United States.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Paul V. Kroskrity</span> American linguistic anthropologist

Paul V. Kroskrity is an American linguistic anthropologist known primarily for his contributions to establishing and developing language ideology as a field of research. He is professor of anthropology, applied linguistics, and American Indian Studies at the University of California, Los Angeles. He is the past President of the Society for Linguistic Anthropology and past Chair of the American Indian Studies program at the University of California, Los Angeles.

Bertha Pauline Dutton was an American anthropologist and ethnologist specializing in the American Southwest and Mesoamerica. She was one of the first female archeologists to work with the National Park Service.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Astialakwa</span> Former village of the ancestral Puebloan people

Astialakwa was a prehistoric and historic village built by the ancestral Puebloan people located within the Astialakwa Archeological District, in an area now known as the Jemez Springs area of Northern New Mexico. The archeological area is on the National Register of Historic Places. The location is restricted from access.