Conversation theory

Last updated

Conversation theory is a cybernetic approach to the study of conversation, cognition and learning that may occur between two participants who are engaged in conversation with each other. [1] [2] [3] It presents an experimental framework heavily utilizing human-computer interactions and computer theoretic models as a means to present a scientific theory explaining how conversational interactions lead to the emergence of knowledge between participants. [4] [5] The theory was developed by Gordon Pask, who credits Bernard Scott, [6] Dionysius Kallikourdis, Robin McKinnon-Wood, [7] and others during its initial development and implementation as well as Paul Pangaro during subsequent years. [8] [9] [Footnote 1]

Overview

Conversation theory may be described as a formal theory of conversational process, [10] as well as a theoretical methodology concerned with concept-forming and concept-sharing between conversational participants. [11] It may be viewed as a framework that may be used to examine learning and development through the means of conversational techniques by means of human-machine interactions; the results of which may then inform approaches to education, educational psychology, and epistemology. [12] While the framework is interpretable as a psychological framework with educational applications (specifically, as a general framework to think about teaching and learning), [13] Pask's motivation in developing the theory has been interpreted by some who closely worked with him develop upon certain theoretical concerns regarding the nature of cybernetic inquiry. [14]

The theory has been noted to have been influenced by a variety of psychological, pedagogical and philosophical influences such as Lev Vygotsky, R. D. Laing and George H. Mead. [15] [16] [17] With some authors suggesting that the kind of human-machine learning interactions documented in conversation theory to be mirroring Vygotsky's descriptions of the zone of proximal development, [18] and his descriptions of spontaneous and scientific concepts. [19]

The theory prioritizes learning and teaching approaches related to education. [20] A central idea of the theory is that learning occurs through conversations: For if participant A is to be conscious with participant B of a topic of inquiry, both participants must be able to converse with each other about that topic. [21] Because of this, participants engaging in a discussion about a subject matter make their knowledge claims explicit through the means of such conversational interactions.

The theory is concerned with a variety of "psychological, linguistic, epistemological, social or non-commitally mental events of which there is awareness". [22] Awareness in this sense is not of a person-specific type, i.e., it is not necessarily localized in a single participant. Instead, the type of awareness examined in conversation theory is the kind of joint awareness that may be shared between entities. [22] While there is an acknowledgment of its similarities to phenomenology, the theory extends its analysis to examine cognitive processes. [23] However, the concept of cognition is not viewed as merely being confined to an individual's brain or central nervous system. [24] Instead, cognition may occur at the level of a group of people (leading to the emergence of social awareness), or may characterize certain types of computing machines. [25]

Initial results from the theory lead to a distinction in the type of learning strategies participants used during the learning process; whereby students in general gravitated towards holistic or serialist learning strategies (with the optimal mixture producing a versatile learning strategy). [3]

Conversation

Following Hugh Dubberly and Paul Pangaro, [26] a conversation in the context of conversation theory involves an exchange between two participants whereby each participant is contextualized as a learning system whose internal states are changed through the course of the conversation. [27] What can be discussed through conversation, i.e., topics of discussion, are said to belong to a conversational domain. [28]

Conversation is distinguished from the mere exchange of information as seen in information theory, by the fact that utterances are interpreted within the context of a given perspective of such a learning system. [27] Each participant's meanings and perceptions change during the course of a conversation, and each participant can agree to commit to act in certain ways during the conversation. [29] In this way, conversation permits not only learning but also collaboration through participants coordinating themselves and designating their roles through the means of conversation. [30]

Since meanings are agreed during the course of a conversation, and since purported agreements can be illusory (whereby we think we have the same understanding of a given topic but in fact do not), an empirical approach to the study of conversation would require stable reference points during such conversational exchanges between peers so as to permit reproducible results. Using computer theoretical models of cognition, conversation theory can document these intervals of understanding that arise in the conversations between two participating individuals, such that the development of individual and collective understandings can be analyzed rigorously. [31]

In this way, Pask has been argued to have been an early pioneer in AI-based educational approaches: Having proposed that advances in computational media may enable conversational forms of interactions to take place between man and machine. [32]

Language

The types of languages that conversation theory utilizes in its approach are distinguishable based on a language's role in relation to an experiment in which a conversation is examined as the subject of inquiry; thus, it follows that conversations can be conducted at different levels depending on the role a language has in relation to an experiment. [33] The types of languages are as follows: Natural languages used for general discussions outside the experiment; object languages which are the subject of inquiry during an experiment, and finally a metalanguage which is used to talk about the design, management, and results on an experiment.

A natural language is treated as an unrestricted language used between a source (say a participant) and an interrogator or analyst (say an experimenter). [34] For this reason, it may be considered a language for general discussion in the context of conversation theory. An object language meanwhile, has some of the qualities of a natural language (which permits commands, questions, ostentation and predication), [35] but is used in conversation theory specifically as the language studied during experiments. [36] Finally, the metalanguage is an observational language used by an interrogator or analysis for describing the conversational system under observation, prescribing actions that are permitted within such a system, and posing parameters regarding what may be discussed during an experiment under observation. [35]

The object language differs from most formal languages, by virtue of being "a command and question language[,] not an assertoric language like [a] predicate calculus". [37] Moreover, is a language primarily dealing with metaphors indicating material analogies and not on the kind of propositions dealing with truth or falsity values. [37] Since conversation theory specifically focuses on learning and development within human subjects, the object language is separated into two distinct modes of conversing.

Conversation theory conceptualises learning as being the result of two integrated levels of control: The first level of control is designated by and designates a set of problem-solving procedures which attempt to attain goals or subgoals, whereas the second level of control is designated as and denotes various constructive processes that have been acquired by a student through maturation, imprinting and previous learning. [38] The object language then is demarcated in conversation theory based on these considerations, whereby it is split between and lines of inquiry such that an object language is the ordered pair of such discourse types . According to Bernard Scott, discourse of an object language may be conceptualized as the level of how, i.e., discourse that is concerned with "how to “do” a topic: how to recognize it, construct it, maintain it and so on". [39] Meanwhile, discourse may be conceptualized as the level of why, i.e., it is discourse "concerned with explaining or justifying what a topic means in terms of other topics". [39]

Concepts

A concept in conversation theory, is conceived of as the production, reproduction, and maintenance of a given topic relation from other topic relations , all belonging to a given conversational domain . [40] [41] This implies , where and are used to represent a number on a finite index of numbers. A concept must satisfy the twin condition that it must entail and be entailed by other topics. [42]

A concept in the context of conversation theory is not a class, nor description of a class, nor a stored description: Instead, a concept is specifically used to reconstruct, reproduce or stabilize relations. [43] Thus, if is the head topic of discussion, then implies that the concept of that relation produces, reproduces, and maintains that relation. [44]

Now, a concept itself is considered to consist of the ordered pair containing a program and an interpretation:

Whereby a program attempts to derive a given topic relation, while an interpretation refers to the compilation of that program. [44] In other words, given a specific topic relation, a program attempts to derive that relation through a series of other topic relations, which are compiled in such a way as to derive the initial topic relation. [44] A concept as defined above is considered to be a -procedure, which is embodied by an underlying processor called a -processor. [44]

In this way, Pask envisages concepts as mental organisations that hold a hypothesis and seek to test that hypothesis in order to confirm or deny its validity. [45] This notion of a concept has been noted as formally resembling a TOTE cycle discussed by Miller, Galanter and Pribram. [46] [Footnote 2] The contents and structure that a concept might have at a given interaction of its continuous deformation can be represented through an entailment structure.

Such conceptual forms are said to be emergent through conversational interactions. They are encapsulated through entailment structures, which is a way by which we may visualize an organized and publicly available collection of resultant knowledge. [47] Entailment structures may afford certain advantages compared to certain semantic network structures, as they force semantic relations to be expressed as belonging to coherent structures. The entailment structure is composed of a series of nodes and arrows representing a series of topic relations and the derivations of such topic relations. For example:

Derivation1.png

In the above illustration, let , such that there are topic relations that are members of a set of topic relations. Each topic relation is represented by a node, and the entailment represented by the black arc. It follows that in the case above, such that the topics P and Q entail the topic of T.

Entailment mesh.png

Assuming we use the same derivation process for all topics in above entailment structure, then we are let with the following product as illustrated above. This represents a minimal entailment mesh consisting of a triad of derivations: , , and . The solid arc indicates that a given head topic relation is derived from subordinate topics, whereas the arcs with dotted lines represent how the head topic may be used to derive other topics. Finally:

Entailment mesh2.png

Represents two solid arcs permitting alternative derivations of the topic T. This can be expressed as , which reads either the set containing P and Q, or the set containing R and S entail T. Lastly, a formal analogy is shown where two topics T and T' belonging to two entailment meshes are demonstrated to have a one-to-one correspondence with each other. The diamond shape below denotes analogy relation that can be claimed to exist between any three topics of each entailment mesh.

Analogy2.png

The relation of one topic T to another T' by an analogy can also be seen as: Being based on an isomorphism , a semantic distinction between two individual universes on interpretation . Assuming an analogy holds for two topics in two distinct entailment meshes, then it should hold for all if the analogy is to be considered coherent and stable.

Cognitive Reflector

From conversation theory, Pask developed what he called a "Cognitive Reflector". This is a virtual machine for selecting and executing concepts or topics from an entailment mesh shared by at least a pair of participants. It features an external modelling facility on which agreement between, say, a teacher and pupil may be shown by reproducing public descriptions of behaviour. [2] We see this in essay and report writing or the "practicals" of science teaching.

Lp was Pask's protolanguage which produced operators like Ap which concurrently executes the concept, Con, of a Topic, T, to produce a Description, D. Thus:

Ap(Con(T)) => D(T), where => stands for produces.

A succinct account of these operators is presented in Pask [48] Amongst many insights he points out that three indexes are required for concurrent execution, two for parallel and one to designate a serial process. He subsumes this complexity by designating participants A, B, etc. [48] In Commentary toward the end of Pask, he states:

The form not the content of the theories (conversation theory and interactions of actors theory) return to and is congruent with the forms of physical theories; such as wave particle duality (the set theoretic unfoldment part of conversation theory is a radiation and its reception is the interpretation by the recipient of the descriptions so exchanged, and vice versa). The particle aspect is the recompilation by the listener of what a speaker is saying. Theories of many universes, one at least for each participant A and one to participant B- are bridged by analogy. As before this is the truth value of any interaction; the metaphor for which is culture itself. [48]

Learning strategies

In order to facilitate learning, Pask argued that subject matter should be represented in the form of structures which show what is to be learned. These structures exist in a variety of different levels depending upon the extent of the relationships displayed. The critical method of learning according to Conversation Theory is "teachback" in which one person teaches another what they have learned. [33]

Pask identified two different types of learning strategies: [33]

The ideal is the versatile learner who is neither vacuous holist "globe trotter" nor serialist who knows little of the context of his work.

In learning, the stage where one converges or evolves, many Cyberneticians describe the act of understanding as a closed-loop. Instead of simply “taking in” new information, one goes back to look at their understandings and pulls together information that was “triggered” and forms a new connection. This connection becomes tighter and one's understanding of a certain concept is solidified or “stable” (Pangaro, 2003). Furthermore, Gordon Pask emphasized that [49] conflict is the basis for the notion of “calling for'' additional information (Pangaro, 1992).

According to Entwistle, experiments which lead to the investigation of phenomenon later denoted by the term learning strategy came about through the implementation of a variety of learning tasks. [50] Initially, this was done through utilising either CASTE, INTUITION, or the Clobbits pseudo-taxonomy. [51] [52] [53] However, given issues resulting from either the time-consuming nature or operating experiments or inexactness of experimental conditions, new tests were created in the form of the Spy Ring History test and the Smuggler's test. The former test involved a participant having to learn the history of a fictitious spy ring (in other words, the history of a fictitious espionage network); the participant, having to learn about the history of five spies in three countries over the period of five years. The comprehension learning component of the test involved learning the similarities and differences between a set of networks; whereas the operation learning aspect of the test involved learning the role each spy played and what sequence of actions that spy played over a given year. [51]

While Entwistle noted difficulties regarding the length of such tests for groups of students who were engaged in the Spy Ring History test, the results of the test did seem to correspond with the type of learning strategies discussed. [54] However, it has been noted that while Pask and associates work on learning styles has been influential in both the development of conceptual tools and methodology, the Spy Ring History test and Smuggler's test may have been biased towards STEM students than humanities in its implementation, with Entwistle arguing that the "rote learning of formulae and definitions, together with a positive reaction to solving puzzles and problems of a logical nature, are characteristics more commonly found in science than arts student". [55] [56]

Applications

One potential application of conversation theory that has been studied and developed is as an alternative approach to common types of search engine Information retrieval algorithms. [57] [58] Unlike PageRank-like algorithms, which determine the priority of a search result based on how many hyperlinks on the web link to them, conversation theory has been used to apply a discursive approach to web search requests. [57] [58]

ThoughtShuffler is an attempt to build a search engine utilizing design principles from conversation theory: [59] In this approach, terms that are input into a search request yield search results relating to other terms that derive or help provide context to the meaning of the first in a way that mimics derivations of topics in an entailment structure. [57] For example, given the input of a search term, a neighbourhood of corresponding terms that comprise the meaning of the first term may be suggested for the user to explore. In doing this, the search engine interface highlights snippets of webpages corresponding to a neighbourhood terms that help provide meaning to the first. [57]

The aim of this design, is to provide just enough information for a user to become curious about a topic in order to induce the intention to explore other subtopics related to the main term input into the search engine. [60]

See also

Footnotes

  1. Other contributors include: Brian Lewis, Robin Bailey, David Ensor, Tony Newton, Valary Robinson, Robert Newton, Elizibeth Pask, V. Midora and N. Green.
  2. Pask believed Miller's model had been "carelessly applied to analogize the mind and computer programs without considering [the] possibility of recursive circularity between problem-solvers of varied competence iteratively supplementing each other's understandings" ( Tilak 2023 , pp. 31).

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Uncertainty principle</span> Foundational principle in quantum physics

The uncertainty principle, also known as Heisenberg's indeterminacy principle, is a fundamental concept in quantum mechanics. It states that there is a limit to the precision with which certain pairs of physical properties, such as position and momentum, can be simultaneously known. In other words, the more accurately one property is measured, the less accurately the other property can be known.

Warren Sturgis McCulloch was an American neurophysiologist and cybernetician, known for his work on the foundation for certain brain theories and his contribution to the cybernetics movement. Along with Walter Pitts, McCulloch created computational models based on mathematical algorithms called threshold logic which split the inquiry into two distinct approaches, one approach focused on biological processes in the brain and the other focused on the application of neural networks to artificial intelligence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Saul Kripke</span> American philosopher and logician (1940–2022)

Saul Aaron Kripke was an American analytic philosopher and logician. He was Distinguished Professor of Philosophy at the Graduate Center of the City University of New York and emeritus professor at Princeton University. Kripke is considered one of the most important philosophers of the latter half of the 20th century. Since the 1960s, he has been a central figure in a number of fields related to mathematical and modal logic, philosophy of language and mathematics, metaphysics, epistemology, and recursion theory.

In classical deductive logic, a consistent theory is one that does not lead to a logical contradiction. The lack of contradiction can be defined in either semantic or syntactic terms. The semantic definition states that a theory is consistent if it has a model, i.e., there exists an interpretation under which all formulas in the theory are true. This is the sense used in traditional Aristotelian logic, although in contemporary mathematical logic the term satisfiable is used instead. The syntactic definition states a theory is consistent if there is no formula such that both and its negation are elements of the set of consequences of . Let be a set of closed sentences and the set of closed sentences provable from under some formal deductive system. The set of axioms is consistent when there is no formula such that and .

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Quantum decoherence</span> Loss of quantum coherence

Quantum decoherence is the loss of quantum coherence, the process in which a system's behaviour changes from that which can be explained by quantum mechanics to that which can be explained by classical mechanics. Beginning out of attempts to extend the understanding of quantum mechanics, the theory has developed in several directions and experimental studies have confirmed some of the key issues. Quantum computing relies on quantum coherence and is the primary practical applications of the concept.

Operational semantics is a category of formal programming language semantics in which certain desired properties of a program, such as correctness, safety or security, are verified by constructing proofs from logical statements about its execution and procedures, rather than by attaching mathematical meanings to its terms. Operational semantics are classified in two categories: structural operational semantics formally describe how the individual steps of a computation take place in a computer-based system; by opposition natural semantics describe how the overall results of the executions are obtained. Other approaches to providing a formal semantics of programming languages include axiomatic semantics and denotational semantics.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Self-organization</span> Process of creating order by local interactions

Self-organization, also called spontaneous order in the social sciences, is a process where some form of overall order arises from local interactions between parts of an initially disordered system. The process can be spontaneous when sufficient energy is available, not needing control by any external agent. It is often triggered by seemingly random fluctuations, amplified by positive feedback. The resulting organization is wholly decentralized, distributed over all the components of the system. As such, the organization is typically robust and able to survive or self-repair substantial perturbation. Chaos theory discusses self-organization in terms of islands of predictability in a sea of chaotic unpredictability.

In theoretical computer science, the π-calculus is a process calculus. The π-calculus allows channel names to be communicated along the channels themselves, and in this way it is able to describe concurrent computations whose network configuration may change during the computation.

In physics, the S-matrix or scattering matrix relates the initial state and the final state of a physical system undergoing a scattering process. It is used in quantum mechanics, scattering theory and quantum field theory (QFT).

Kripke semantics is a formal semantics for non-classical logic systems created in the late 1950s and early 1960s by Saul Kripke and André Joyal. It was first conceived for modal logics, and later adapted to intuitionistic logic and other non-classical systems. The development of Kripke semantics was a breakthrough in the theory of non-classical logics, because the model theory of such logics was almost non-existent before Kripke.

Second-order cybernetics, also known as the cybernetics of cybernetics, is the recursive application of cybernetics to itself and the reflexive practice of cybernetics according to such a critique. It is cybernetics where "the role of the observer is appreciated and acknowledged rather than disguised, as had become traditional in western science". Second-order cybernetics was developed between the late 1960s and mid 1970s by Heinz von Foerster and others, with key inspiration coming from Margaret Mead. Foerster referred to it as "the control of control and the communication of communication" and differentiated first order cybernetics as "the cybernetics of observed systems" and second-order cybernetics as "the cybernetics of observing systems".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Gordon Pask</span> British cybernetician and psychologist (1928–1996)

Andrew Gordon Speedie Pask was a British cybernetician, inventor and polymath who made during his lifetime multiple contributions to cybernetics, educational psychology, educational technology, epistemology, chemical computing, architecture, and the performing arts. During his life he gained three doctorate degrees. He was an avid writer, with more than two hundred and fifty publications which included a variety of journal articles, books, periodicals, patents, and technical reports. He also worked as an academic and researcher for a variety of educational settings, research institutes, and private stakeholders including but not limited to the University of Illinois, Concordia University, the Open University, Brunel University and the Architectural Association School of Architecture. He is known for the development of conversation theory.

Ranulph Glanville was an Anglo-Irish cybernetician and design theorist. He was a founding vice-president of the International Academy for Systems and Cybernetic Sciences (2006–2009) and president of the American Society for Cybernetics (2009–2014).

1975 in philosophy

New Cybernetics, as used by cybernetician Gordon Pask, is the meaningful transfer of information between coherences in all media in terms of attractions and repulsions between clockwise and anti-clockwise spins. This is a possibly defining paradigm of the new cybernetics or second-order cybernetics.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">American Society for Cybernetics</span> Organization

The American Society for Cybernetics (ASC) is an American non-profit scholastic organization for the advancement of cybernetics as a science, a discipline, a meta-discipline and the promotion of cybernetics as basis for an interdisciplinary discourse. The society does this by developing and applying cybernetics’ concepts which are presented and published via its conferences and peer-reviewed publications. As a meta-discipline, it creates bridges between disciplines, philosophies, sciences, and arts. The ASC is a full member of the International Federation for Systems Research (IFSR).

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cybernetics</span> Transdisciplinary field concerned with regulatory and purposive systems

Cybernetics is a field of systems theory that studies circular causal systems whose outputs are also inputs, such as feedback systems. It is concerned with the general principles of circular causal processes, including in ecological, technological, biological, cognitive and social systems and also in the context of practical activities such as designing, learning, and managing.

Text and conversation is a theory in the field of organizational communication illustrating how communication makes up an organization. In the theory's simplest explanation, an organization is created and defined by communication. Communication "is" the organization and the organization exists because communication takes place. The theory is built on the notion, an organization is not seen as a physical unit holding communication. Text and conversation theory puts communication processes at the heart of organizational communication and postulates, an organization doesn't contain communication as a "causal influence", but is formed by the communication within. This theory is not intended for direct application, but rather to explain how communication exists. The theory provides a framework for better understanding organizational communication.

In information theory, Interactions of actors theory is a theory developed by Gordon Pask and Gerard de Zeeuw. It is a generalisation of Pask's earlier conversation theory: The chief distinction being that conversation theory focuses on analysing the specific features that allow a conversation to emerge between two participants, whereas interaction of actor's theory focuses on the broader domain of conversation in which conversations may appear, disappear, and reappear over time.

Self-organization, a process where some form of overall order arises out of the local interactions between parts of an initially disordered system, was discovered in cybernetics by William Ross Ashby in 1947. It states that any deterministic dynamic system automatically evolves towards a state of equilibrium that can be described in terms of an attractor in a basin of surrounding states. Once there, the further evolution of the system is constrained to remain in the attractor. This constraint implies a form of mutual dependency or coordination between its constituent components or subsystems. In Ashby's terms, each subsystem has adapted to the environment formed by all other subsystems.

References

  1. ( Pask 1975a )
  2. 1 2 ( Pask 1975b )
  3. 1 2 ( Pask 1976 )
  4. ( Scott 2001 , p. 25)
  5. ( Mayne 2001 )
  6. ( Scott 2022 )
  7. ( Glanville 1996 )
  8. ( Pask 1984 )
  9. ( Pask 1976 , p. xii)
  10. ( Scott 2011 , p. 134)
  11. ( Manning 2023a , p. 47)
  12. ( Pask 1976 , p. ix)
  13. ( Koh & Doroudi 2023 , p. 110)
  14. ( Scott 2007a )
  15. ( Pask 1976 , p. 19)
  16. ( Scott 2021 )
  17. ( Tilak & Glassman 2022 )
  18. ( Tilak & Glassman 2022 , pp. 893–894)
  19. ( Laurillard 2002 , p. 87)
  20. ( Pask 1975b , p. 9)
  21. ( Pask 1975b , p. 6)
  22. 1 2 ( Pask 1975b , p. 1)
  23. ( Pask 1975b , pp. 1–2)
  24. ( Pask 1975b , p. 3)
  25. ( Pask 1975b , p. 2)
  26. ( Dubberly & Pangaro 2009 , p. 8)
  27. 1 2 ( Dubberly & Pangaro 2009 , p. 1)
  28. ( Pask 1975b , p. 44)
  29. ( Dubberly & Pangaro 2009 , pp. 2–3)
  30. ( Dubberly & Pangaro 2009 , pp. 3–4)
  31. ( Hayut 1995 )
  32. ( Sharples 2023 , p. 2)
  33. 1 2 3 ( SUNY Cortland )
  34. ( Pask 1975b , p. 557)
  35. 1 2 ( Pask 1975b , p. 558)
  36. ( Pask 1975b , p. 22)
  37. 1 2 ( Pask 1975b , p. 171)
  38. ( Pask 1975a , p. 133)
  39. 1 2 ( Scott 2007b , p. 1502)
  40. ( Pask 1975b , p. 47)
  41. ( Pask 1975b , p. 98)
  42. ( Pask 1975b , pp. 98)
  43. ( Pask 1975b , pp. 47–48)
  44. 1 2 3 4 ( Manning 2023b , pp. 71)
  45. ( Pask 1975b , pp. 244)
  46. ( Manning 2023a , pp. 49–50)
  47. ( Pask 1975b , p. 76)
  48. 1 2 3 ( Pask 1996 )
  49. ( Pangaro 1993 )
  50. ( Entwistle 1978 )
  51. 1 2 ( Entwistle 1978 , p. 258)
  52. ( Pask & Scott 1972 )
  53. ( Robertson 1978 )
  54. ( Entwistle 1978 , p. 261)
  55. ( Entwistle 1978 , p. 264)
  56. ( LSRC 2004 )
  57. 1 2 3 4 ( Pangaro 2008 )
  58. 1 2 ( Tilak et al. 2023 , pp. 134)
  59. ( Tilak et al. 2023 , pp. 138–139)
  60. ( Tilak et al. 2023 , pp. 139)

Citation Sources

  • Dubberly, Hugh; Pangaro, Paul (2009). "What is conversation? Can we design for effective conversation?" (PDF). ACM Interactions. XVI (4) via Dubberly Design Office.
  • Entwistle, N. J. (1978). "Knowledge structures and styles of learning: a summary of Pask's recent research". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 48 (3): 255–265. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1978.tb03012.x via British Psychological Society.
  • Glanville, Ranulph (1996). "Robin McKinnon-Wood and Gordon Pask: a Lifelong Conversation". Cybernetics & Human Knowing. 3 (4): 56–62.
  • Hayut, Yitzhak I (1995). "Appendix "A": Conversation Theory developed by the cybernetician Gordon Pask". thehope.com. Archived from the original on 2000-05-23.
  • Koh, Elizabeth; Doroudi, Shayan (2023). "Learning, teaching, and assessment with generative artificial intelligence: towards a plateau of productivity". Learning: Research and Practice. 9 (2): 109–166. doi: 10.1080/23735082.2023.2264086 .
  • Laurillard, Diana (2002). Rethinking University Teaching (1st ed.). London: RouteledgeFalmer. ISBN   9780415256797.
  • LSRC (2004). "Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning: A systematic and critical review" (PDF). leerbeleving. p. 91.
  • Mayne, Alan (2001). "Learning and teaching machines and conversation theory: Some contemporary implications and ideas for research". Kybernetes. 30 (5/6): 762–767. doi:10.1108/EUM0000000005699 via Emerald Insight.
  • Manning, Thomas (2023a). "What is conversation theory?". Cybernetics & Human Knowing. 30 (1–2): 47–65.
  • (2023b). "Paskian algebra: A discursive approuch to conversational multi-agent systems". Cybernetics & Human Knowing. 30 (1–2): 67–81.
  • Pangaro, Paul (1993). "Pask as Dramaturg". Systems Research. 10 (3).
  • (2008). "Instructions for Design and Designs for Conversation". In Luppicini, Rocci (ed.). Handbook of Conversation Design for Instructional Applications. IGI Global.
  • Pask, Gordon; Scott, B. C. E. (1972). "Learning strategies and individual competence". International Journal of Man-Machine Studies. 4 (3): 217–253. doi:10.1016/S0020-7373(72)80004-X.
  • (1975a). The cybernetics of human learning and performance: A guide to theory and practice. London, England: Hutchinson Educational.
  • (1975b). Conversation, cognition and learning: A cybernetic theory and methodology. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company.
  • (1976). Conversation theory: Applications in education and epistemology. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company.
  • (1984). "Review of Conversation Theory and a protologic (or protolanguage), Lp". Educational Communication and Technology. 32 (1): 3–40. doi:10.1007/BF02768767. JSTOR   30219866. S2CID   142103971 via JSTORE.
  • (1996). "Heinz von Foerster's Self-Organisation, the Progenitor of Conversation and Interaction Theories". Systems Research. 13 (3): 349–362. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1735(199609)13:3<349::AID-SRES103>3.0.CO;2-G.
  • Robertson, I. T. (1978). "Relationships between learning strategy, attention deployment and personality". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 48 (1): 86–91. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1978.tb02374.x. PMID   646945.
  • Scott, Bernard (2001). "Conversation theory: A constructivist, dialogical approach to educational technology". Cybernetics & Human Knowing. 8 (4) via ingentaconnect.
  • (2007a). "The Cybernetics of Gordon Pask". In Glanville, Ranulph, Müller, Karl H (eds.). Gordon Pask, Philosopher Mechanic: An Introduction to the Cybernetician's Cybernetician. Vienna, Austria: edition echoraum. pp. 31–51. ISBN   978-3-901941-15-3.
  • (2007b). "Cybernetic principles for learning design". Kybernetes. 36.9/10.
  • (2011). Explorations in Second-Order Cybernetics: Reflection on Cybernetics, Psychology and Education (1st ed.). Vienna: edition echoraum. ISBN   9783901941269.
  • (2021). Cybernetics for the Social Sciences. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Brill. ISBN   9789004464346.
  • (2022). "A life in cybernetics". The British Psycological Society. Retrieved 19 March 2022.
  • Sharples, Mike (2023). "Towards social generative AI for education: theory, practices and ethics". arXiv: 2306.10063 .
  • SUNY Cortland. "Conversation Theory – Gordon Pask". web.cortland.edu.
  • Tilak, Shantanu; Glassman, Michael (2022). "Gordon Pask's second-order cybernetics and Lev Vygotsky's cultural historical theory: Understanding the role of the Internet in developing human thinking". Theory and Psychology. 32 (6): 888–914. doi:10.1177/09593543221123281. S2CID   252344151.
  • (2023). "Cybernetics, Education, and Psycology: Discovering Potentials (Yet) Unearthed". Cybernetics & Human Knowing. 30 (1–2): 23–44.
  • ; Kadir, Latif; Wen, Ziye; Pangaro, Paul; Glassman, Michael (2023). "Designing Sociohistorically Sensitive Information Search: Experimental Analyses of Essay Written Using ThoughtShuffler and Google". Cybernetics and Human Knowing. 30 (1–2). Imprint Academic: 133–151.

Further reading