Explicit memory

Last updated

Explicit memory (or declarative memory) is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts. [1] This type of memory is dependent upon three processes: acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval. [2] [3]

Contents

Explicit memory can be divided into two categories: episodic memory, which stores specific personal experiences, and semantic memory, which stores factual information. [4] Explicit memory requires gradual learning, with multiple presentations of a stimulus and response.

The type of knowledge that is stored in explicit memory is called declarative knowledge, the counterpart to explicit memory is known as implicit memory, refers to memories acquired and used unconsciously such as skills (e.g. knowing how to get dressed) or perception. Unlike explicit memory, implicit memory learns rapidly, even from a single stimulus, and it is influenced by other mental systems.

Sometimes a distinction is made between explicit memory and declarative memory. In such cases, explicit memory relates to any kind of conscious memory, and declarative memory relates to any kind of memory that can be described in words; however, if it is assumed that a memory cannot be described without being conscious and vice versa, then the two concepts are identical.

Types

Episodic memory

Episodic memory consists of the storage and recollection of observational information attached to specific life-events. These can be memories that happened to the subject directly or just memories of events that happened around them. Episodic memory is what people generally think of when they talk about memory. Episodic memory allows for recalling various contextual and situational details of one's previous experiences.

Some examples of episodic memory include the memory of entering a specific classroom for the first time, the memory of storing your carry-on baggage while boarding a plane, headed to a specific destination on a specific day and time, the memory of being notified that one are being terminated from their job, or the memory of notifying a subordinate that they are being terminated from their job. The retrieval of these episodic memories can be thought of as the action of mentally reliving in detail the past events that they concern. [4] Episodic memory is believed to be the system that provides the basic support for semantic memory.

Semantic memory

Semantic memory refers to general world knowledge (facts, ideas, meaning and concepts) that can be articulated and is independent of personal experience. [5] This includes world knowledge, object knowledge, language knowledge, and conceptual priming. Semantic memory is distinct from episodic memory, which is the memory of experiences and specific events that occur during people's lives, from which they can recreate at any given point. [6] For instance, semantic memory might contain information about what a cat is, whereas episodic memory might contain a specific memory of petting a particular cat. Humans can learn about new concepts by applying their knowledge learned from things in the past. [7]

Other examples of semantic memory include types of food, capital cities of a geographic region, facts about people, dates, and the lexicon of flowers; a language, such as a one's vocabulary or a person's final vocabulary [4] both exemplify semantic memory.

Hybrid types

Autobiographical memory is a memory system consisting of episodes recollected from an individual's life, based on a combination of episodic (personal experiences and specific objects, people and events experienced at particular time and place) and semantic (general knowledge and facts about the world) memory. [8]

Spatial memory is the part of memory responsible for recording information about one's environment and its spatial orientation. For example, a person's spatial memory is required in order to navigate around a familiar city, just as a rat's spatial memory is needed to learn the location of food at the end of a maze. It is often argued that in both humans and animals, spatial memories are summarized as a cognitive map. Spatial memory has representations within working, short-term and long-term memory. Research indicates that there are specific areas of the brain associated with spatial memory. Many methods are used for measuring spatial memory in children, adults, and animals.

Examples

Long-Term MemorySubtypeDescriptionExample
Declarative (explicit)Conscious memory of facts and events
Semantic Factual informationThe capital of Germany is Berlin
Episodic Specific personal experiencesYour 10th birthday
Non-declarative (implicit)Modes of learning that are nonconscious - one learning to perform a sequence of actions that do not necessarily invoke knowledge
Priming Also known as pattern completion, where one has the ability to complete a pattern they have once seen before. This priming differs from priming in Psychology.If you were given a picture of half of a letter from the alphabet and you recognized which letter it is, you would be able to complete the letter.
Perceptual learning Perceptual ability to differentiate sensories through experience of stimuliDifferentiating between categories such as smells, colors, tastes
Category learning "...the process of establishing a memory trace that improves the efficiency of assigning novel objects to contrasting groups" [9] Movie genres, breeds of dogs, types of fruits
Emotional learning "...retention of classically conditioned emotional relationships that cannot be voluntarily recollected or reported" [10] Being afraid of dogs yet you cannot explain why
Procedural learning The formation of skills and habits Learning how to ride a bike

The model of language

Declarative and procedural memory fall into two categories of human language. Declarative memory system is used by the lexicon. Declarative memory stores all arbitrary, unique word-specific knowledge, including word meanings, word sounds, and abstract representations such as word category. In other words, declarative memory is where random bits and pieces of knowledge about language that are specific and unpredictable are stored. Declarative memory includes representations of simple words (e.g. cat), bound morphemes (morphemes that have to go together), irregular morphological forms, verb complements, and idioms (or non-compositional semantic units). Irregular morphological structures fall into the declarative system; the irregularities (such as went being the past form of go or idioms) are what we have to memorize.

Declarative memory supports a superposition associative memory, which allows for generalizations across representations. For example, the memorization of phonologically similar stem-irregular past tense pairs (e.g. spring-sprung, sing-sang) may allow for memory-based generalization to new irregularities, either from real words (bring-brought) or from novel ones (spring-sprung). This ability to generalize could underlie some degree of productivity within the memory system.

While declarative memory deals with irregularities of morphology, procedural memory uses regular phonology and regular morphology. Procedural memory system is used by grammar, where grammar is defined by the building of a rule governed structure. Language's ability to use grammar comes from procedural memory, making grammar like another procedure. It underlies the learning of new, and already learned, rule-based procedures that oversee the regularities of language, particularly those procedures related to combining items into complex structures that have precedence and hierarchical relations- precedence in the sense of left to right and hierarchical in the sense of top to bottom. Procedural memory builds rule-governed structure (merging or series) of forms and representations into complex structures such as:

  1. Phonology
  2. Inflectional and derivational morphology
  3. Compositional semantics (the meaning of composition of words into complex structures)
  4. Syntax

Broca and Wernicke's Brain Region

Broca's area is important to procedural memory, because, "Broca's area is involved in the expressive aspects of spoken and written language (production of sentences constrained by the rules of grammar and syntax)." [11] Broca's area corresponds to parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, presumably Brodmann's area 44 and 45. Procedural memory is affected by Broca's aphasia. Agrammatism is apparent in Broca's aphasia patients, where a lack of fluency and omission of morphology and function words occur. While those with Broca's aphasia are still able to understand or comprehend speech, they have difficulty producing it. Speech production becomes more difficult when sentences are complex; for example, the passive voice is a grammatically complex structure that is harder for those with Broca's aphasia to comprehend. Wernicke's area is crucial for language development, focusing on the comprehension of speech, rather than speech production. Wernicke's aphasia affects declarative memory. Opposite of Broca's aphasia, paragrammatism is apparent, which causes normal or excessive fluency and use of inappropriate words (neologisms). Those with Wernicke's aphasia struggle to understand the meaning of words and may not recognize their mistakes in speech.

History

The study of human memory stretches back over the last 2000 years. An early attempt to understand memory can be found in Aristotle's major treatise, On the Soul, in which he compares the human mind to a blank slate. [12] He theorized that all humans are born free of any knowledge and are the sum of their experiences. It was only in the late 1800s, however, that a young German philosopher by the name of Herman Ebbinghaus developed the first scientific approach to studying memory. [13] While some of his findings have endured and remain relevant to this day (Learning Curve), his greatest contribution to the field of memory research was demonstrating that memory can be studied scientifically. In 1972, Endel Tulving proposed the distinction between episodic and semantic memory. [4] This was quickly adopted and is now widely accepted. Following this, in 1985, Daniel Schacter proposed a more general distinction between explicit (declarative) and implicit (procedural) memory [14]

With the recent advances in neuroimaging technology, there have been a multitude of findings linking specific brain areas to declarative memory. Despite those advances in cognitive psychology, there is still much to be discovered in terms of the operating mechanisms of declarative memory. [15] It is unclear whether declarative memory is mediated by a particular memory system, or if it is more accurately classified as a type of knowledge. Also it is unknown how or why declarative memory evolved in the first place. [15]

Neuropsychology

Normal brain function

Hippocampus

Hippocampus as seen in red Hippocampus.gif
Hippocampus as seen in red

Although many psychologists believe that the entire brain is involved with memory, the hippocampus, and surrounding structures appear to be most important in declarative memory specifically. [16] The ability to retain and recall episodic memories is highly dependent on the hippocampus, [16] whereas the formation of new declarative memories relies on both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus. [17] Other studies have found that the parahippocampal cortices were related to superior recognition memory. [17]

The Three Stage Model was developed by Eichenbaum, et al. (2001), and proposes that the hippocampus does three things with episodic memory:

  1. Mediates the recording of episodic memories
  2. Identifies common features between episodes
  3. Links these common episodes in a memory space.

To support this model, a version of Piaget's Transitive Inference Task was used to show that the hippocampus is in fact used as the memory space. [16]

When experiencing an event for the first time, a link is formed in the hippocampus allowing us to recall that event in the future. Separate links are also made for features related to that event. For example, when you meet someone new, a unique link is created for them. More links are then connected to that person's link so you can remember what colour their shirt was, what the weather was like when you met them, etc. Specific episodes are made easier to remember and recall by repeatedly exposing oneself to them (which strengthens the links in the memory space) allowing for faster retrieval when remembering. [16]

Hippocampal cells ( neurons ) are activated depending on what information one is exposed to at that moment. Some cells are specific to spatial information, certain stimuli (smells, etc.), or behaviours as has been shown in a Radial Maze Task. [16] It is therefore the hippocampus that allows us to recognize certain situations, environments, etc. as being either distinct or similar to others. However, the Three Stage Model does not incorporate the importance of other cortical structures in memory.

The anatomy of the hippocampus is largely conserved across mammals, and the role of these areas in declarative memory are conserved across species as well. The organization and neural pathways of the hippocampus are very similar in humans and other mammal species. In humans and other mammals, a cross-section of the hippocampus shows the dentate gyrus as well as the dense cell layers of the CA fields. The intrinsic connectivity of these areas are also conserved. [18]

Results from an experiment by Davachi, Mitchell, and Wagner (2003) and subsequent research (Davachi, 2006) shows that activation in the hippocampus during encoding is related to a subject's ability to recall prior events or later relational memories. These tests did not differentiate between individual test items later seen and those forgotten. [19] [20]

Prefrontal cortex

The lateral Prefrontal cortex (PFC) is essential for remembering contextual details of an experience rather than for memory formation. [17] The PFC is also more involved with episodic memory than semantic memory, although it does play a small role in semantics. [21]

Using PET studies and word stimuli, Endel Tulving found that remembering is an automatic process. [22] It is also well documented that a hemispheric asymmetry occurs in the PFC: When encoding memories, the Left Dorsolateral PFC (LPFC) is activated, and when retrieving memories, activation is seen in the Right Dorsolateral PFC (RPFC). [22]

Studies have also shown that the PFC is extremely involved with autonoetic consciousness (See Tulving's theory). [23] This is responsible for humans' recollective experiences and 'mental time travelling' abilities (characteristics of episodic memory).

Amygdala as seen in red Amygdala.png
Amygdala as seen in red

Amygdala

The amygdala is believed to be involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotionally charged memories. Much of the evidence for this has come from research on a phenomenon known as flashbulb memories. These are instances in which memories of powerful emotional events are more highly detailed and enduring than regular memories (e.g. September 11 attacks, assassination of JFK). These memories have been linked to increased activation in the amygdala. [24] Recent studies of patients with damage to the amygdala suggest that it is involved in memory for general knowledge, and not for specific information. [25] [26]

Other structures involved

The regions of the diencephalon have shown brain activation when a remote memory is being recovered [21] and the occipital lobe, ventral temporal lobe, and fusiform gyrus all play a role in memory formation. [17]

Lesion studies

Lesion studies are commonly used in cognitive neuroscience research. Lesions can occur naturally through trauma or disease, or they can be surgically induced by researchers. In the study of declarative memory, the hippocampus and the amygdala are two structures frequently examined using this technique.

Hippocampal lesion studies

The Morris water maze MorrisWaterMaze.jpg
The Morris water maze

The Morris water navigation task tests spatial learning in rats. [27] In this test rats learn to escape from a pool by swimming toward a platform submerged just below the surface of the water. Visual cues that surround the pool (e.g. a chair or window) help the rat to locate the platform on subsequent trials. The rats' use of specific events, cues, and places are all forms of declarative memory. [28] Two groups of rats are observed: a control group with no lesions and an experimental group with hippocampal lesions. In this task created by Morris, rats are placed in the pool at the same position for 12 trials. Each trial is timed and the path taken by the rats is recorded. Rats with hippocampal lesions successfully learn to find the platform. If the starting point is moved, the rats with hippocampal lesions typically fail to locate the platform. The control rats, however, are able to find the platform using the cues acquired during the learning trials. [27] This demonstrates the involvement of the hippocampus in declarative memory. [28]

The Odor-odor Recognition Task, devised by Bunsey and Eichenbaum, involves a social encounter between two rats (a subject and a demonstrator). The demonstrator, after eating a specific type of food, interacts with the subject rat, who then smells the food odor on the other's breath. The experimenters then present the subject rat with a decision between two food options; the food previously eaten by the demonstrator, and a novel food. The researchers found that when there was no time delay, both control rats and rats with lesions chose the familiar food. After 24 hours, however, the rats with hippocampal lesions were just as likely to eat both types of food, while control rats chose the familiar food. [29] This can be attributed to the inability to form episodic memories due to lesions in the hippocampus. The effects of this study can be observed in humans with amnesia, indicating the role of the hippocampus in developing episodic memories that can be generalized to similar situations. [28]

Henry Molaison, previously known as H.M., had parts of both his left and right medial temporal lobes (hippocampi) removed which resulted in the loss of the ability to form new memories. [30] The long-term declarative memory was crucially affected when the structures from the medial temporal lobe were removed, including the ability to form new semantic knowledge and memories. [31] The dissociation in Molaison between the acquisition of declarative memory and other kinds of learning was seen initially in motor learning. [32] Molaison's declarative memory was not functioning, as was seen when Molaison completed the task of repetition priming.

His performance does improve over trials, however, his scores were inferior to those of control participants. [33] In the condition of Molaison the same results from this priming task are reflected when looking at the other basic memory functions like remembering, recall and recognizing. [30] Lesions should not be interpreted as an all-or-nothing condition, in the case of Molaison not all memory and recognition is lost, although the declarative memory is severely damaged he still has a sense of self and memories that were developed before the lesion occurred. [34]

Patient R.B. was another clinical case reinforcing the role of the hippocampus in declarative memory. After suffering an ischemic episode during a cardiac bypass operation, Patient R.B. awoke with a severe anterograde amnesic disorder. IQ and cognition were unaffected, but declarative memory deficits were observed (although not to the extent of that seen in Molaison). Upon death, an autopsy revealed that Patient R.B. had bilateral lesions of the CA1 cell region along the whole length of the hippocampus.

Amygdala lesion studies

Adolph, Cahill and Schul completed a study showing that emotional arousal facilitates the encoding of material into long term declarative memory. [35] They selected two subjects with bilateral damage to the amygdala, as well as six control subjects and six subjects with brain damage. All subjects were shown a series of twelve slides accompanied by a narrative. The slides varied in the degree to which they evoked emotion – slides 1 through 4 and slides 9 through 12 contain non-emotional content. Slides 5 through 8 contain emotional material, and the seventh slide contained the most emotionally arousing image and description (a picture of surgically repaired legs of a car crash victim). [35]

The emotionally arousing slide (slide 7) was remembered no better by the bilateral damage participants than any of the other slides. All other participants notably remembered the seventh slide the best and in most detail out of all the other slides. [35] This shows that the amygdala is necessary to facilitate encoding of declarative knowledge regarding emotionally arousing stimuli, but is not required for encoding knowledge of emotionally neutral stimuli. [36]

Affecting factors

Stress

Stress may have an effect on the recall of declarative memories. Lupien, et al. completed a study that had 3 phases for participants to take part in. Phase 1 involved memorizing a series of words, phase 2 entailed either a stressful (public speaking) or non-stressful situation (an attention task), and phase 3 required participants to recall the words they learned in phase 1. There were signs of decreased declarative memory performance in the participants that had to complete the stressful situation after learning the words. [37] Recall performance after the stressful situation was found to be worse overall than after the non-stressful situation. It was also found that performance differed based on whether the participant responded to the stressful situation with an increase in measured levels of salivary cortisol.

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) emerges after exposure to a traumatic event eliciting fear, horror or helplessness that involves bodily injury, the threat of injury, or death to one's self or another person. [38] The chronic stress in PTSD contributes to an observed decrease in hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits. [39]

Stress can alter memory functions, reward, immune function, metabolism and susceptibility to different diseases. [40] Disease risk is particularly pertinent to mental illnesses, whereby chronic or severe stress remains a common risk factor for several mental illnesses. [41] One system suggests there are five types of stress labeled acute time-limited stressors, brief naturalistic stressors, stressful event sequences, chronic stressors, and distant stressors. An acute time-limited stressor involves a short-term challenge, while a brief natural stressor involves an event that is normal but nevertheless challenging. A stressful event sequence is a stressor that occurs, and then continues to yield stress into the immediate future. A chronic stressor involves exposure to a long-term stressor, and a distant stressor is a stressor that is not immediate. [42]

Neurochemical factors of stress on the brain

Cortisol is the primary glucocorticoid in the human body. In the brain, it modulates the ability of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to process memories. [43] Although the exact molecular mechanism of how glucocorticoids influence memory formation is unknown, the presence of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex tell us these structures are some of its many targets. [43] It has been demonstrated that cortisone, a glucocorticoid, impaired blood flow in the right parahippocampal gyrus, left visual cortex and cerebellum. [43]

A study by Damoiseaux et al. (2007) evaluated the effects of glucocorticoids on hippocampal and prefrontal cortex activation during declarative memory retrieval. They found that administration of hydrocortisone (name given to cortisol when it is used as a medication) to participants one hour before retrieval of information impairs free recall of words, yet when administered before or after learning they had no effect on recall. [43] They also found that hydrocortisone decreases brain activity in the above-mentioned areas during declarative memory retrieval. [43] Therefore, naturally occurring elevations of cortisol during periods of stress lead to impairment of declarative memory. [43]

It is important to note that this study involved only male subjects, which may be significant as sex steroid hormones may have different effects in response to cortisol administration. Men and women also respond to emotional stimuli differently and this may affect cortisol levels. This was also the first Functional magnetic resonance imaging(fMRI) study done utilising glucocorticoids, therefore more research is necessary to further substantiate these findings. [43]

Consolidation during sleep

It is believed that sleep plays an active role in consolidation of declarative memory. Specifically, sleep's unique properties enhance memory consolidation , such as the reactivation of newly learned memories during sleep. For example, it has been suggested that the central mechanism for consolidation of declarative memory during sleep is the reactivation of hippocampal memory representations. This reactivation transfers information to neocortical networks where it is integrated into long-term representations. [44] Studies on rats involving maze learning found that hippocampal neuronal assemblies that are used in the encoding of spatial information are reactivated in the same temporal order. [45] Similarly, positron emission tomography (PET) has shown reactivation of the hippocampus in slow-wave sleep (SWS) after spatial learning. [46] Together these studies show that newly learned memories are reactivated during sleep and through this process new memory traces are consolidated. [47] In addition, researchers have identified three types of sleep (SWS, sleep spindle and REM) in which declarative memory is consolidated.

Slow-wave sleep, often referred to as deep sleep, plays the most important role in consolidation of declarative memory and there is a large amount of evidence to support this claim. One study found that the first 3.5 hours of sleep offer the greatest performance enhancement on memory recall tasks because the first couple of hours are dominated by SWS. Additional hours of sleep do not add to the initial level of performance. Thus this study suggests that full sleep may not be important for optimal performance of memory. [48] Another study shows that people who experience SWS during the first half of their sleep cycle compared to subjects who did not, showed better recall of information. However this is not the case for subjects who were tested for the second half of their sleep cycle, as they experience less SWS. [49]

Another key piece of evidence regarding SWS's involvement in declarative memory consolidation is a finding that people with pathological conditions of sleep, such as insomnia, exhibit both reduction in Slow-Wave Sleep and also have impaired consolidation of declarative memory during sleep. [50] Another study found that middle aged people compared to young group had a worse retrieval of memories. This in turn indicated that SWS is associated with poor declarative memory consolidation but not with age itself. [51]

Some researchers suggest that sleep spindle , a burst of brain activity occurring during stage 2 sleep, plays a role in boosting consolidation of declarative memories. [52] Critics point out that spindle activity is positively correlated with intelligence. [53] In contrast, Schabus and Gruber point out that sleep spindle activity only relates to performance on newly learned memories and not to absolute performance. This supports the hypothesis that sleep spindle helps to consolidate recent memory traces but not memory performance in general. [54] The relationship between sleep spindles and declarative memory consolidation is not yet fully understood. [54]

There is a relatively small body of evidence that supports the idea that REM sleep helps consolidate highly emotional declarative memories. For instance Wagner, et al. compared memory retention for emotional versus neutral text over two instances; early sleep that is dominated by SWS and late sleep that is dominated by REM phase. [55] This study found that sleep improved memory retention of emotional text only during late sleep phase, which was primarily REM. Similarly, Hu & Stylos-Allen, et al. performed a study with emotional versus neutral pictures and concluded that REM sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional declarative memories. [56]

The view that sleep plays an active role in declarative memory consolidation is not shared by all researchers. For instance Ellenbogen, et al. argue that sleep actively protects declarative memory from associative interference. [57] Furthermore, Wixted believes that the sole role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation is nothing more but creating ideal conditions for memory consolidation. [58] For example, when awake, people are bombarded with mental activity which interferes with effective consolidation. However, during sleep, when interference is minimal, memories can be consolidated without associative interference. More research is needed to make a definite statement whether sleep creates favourable conditions for consolidation or it actively enhances declarative memory consolidation. [47]

Encoding and retrieval

The encoding of explicit memory depends on conceptually driven, top-down processing, in which a subject reorganizes the data to store it. [59] The subject makes associations with previously related stimuli or experiences. [60] This was termed deep encoding by Fergus Craik and Robert Lockhart. [61] This way a memory persists longer and will be remembered well. The later recall of information is thus greatly influenced by the way in which the information was originally processed. [59]

The depth-of-processing effect is the improvement in subsequent recall of an object about which a person has given thought to its meaning or shape. Simply put: To create explicit memories, you have to do something with your experiences: think about them, talk about them, write them down, study them, etc. The more you do, the better you will remember. Testing of information while learning has also shown to improve encoding in explicit memory. If a student reads a text book and then tests themselves afterward, their semantic memory of what was read is improved. This study – test method improves encoding of information. This Phenomenon is referred to as the Testing Effect. [62]

Retrieval: Because a person has played an active role in processing explicit information, the internal cues that were used in processing it can also be used to initiate spontaneous recall. [59] When someone talks about an experience, the words they use will help when they try to remember this experience at a later date. The conditions in which information is memorized can affect recall. If a person has the same surroundings or cues when the original information is presented, they are more likely to remember it. This is referred to as encoding specificity and it also applies to explicit memory. In a study where subjects were asked to perform a cued recall task participants with a high working memory did better than participants with a low working memory when the conditions were maintained. When the conditions were changed for recall both groups dropped. The subjects with higher working memory declined more. [63] This is thought to happen because matching environments activates areas of the brain known as the left inferior frontal gyrus and the hippocampus. [64]

Neural structures involved

Several neural structures are proposed to be involved in explicit memory. Most are in the temporal lobe or closely related to it, such as the amygdala, the hippocampus, the rhinal cortex in the temporal lobe, and the prefrontal cortex. [59] Nuclei in the thalamus also are included, because many connections between the prefrontal cortex and temporal cortex are made through the thalamus. [59] The regions that make up the explicit memory circuit receive input from the neocortex and from brainstem systems, including acetylcholine, serotonin, and noradrenaline systems. [65]

Traumatic brain injury

While the human brain is certainly regarded for its plasticity, there is some evidence that shows traumatic brain injury (TBI) in young children can have negative effects on explicit memory. Researchers have looked at children with TBI in early childhood (i.e. infancy) and late childhood. Findings showed that children with severe TBI in late childhood experienced impaired explicit memory while still maintaining implicit memory formation. Researchers also found that children with severe TBI in early childhood had both increased chance of having both impaired explicit memory and implicit memory. While children with severe TBI are at risk for impaired explicit memory, the chances of impaired explicit memory in adults with severe TBI is much greater. [66]

Memory loss

Alzheimer's disease has a profound effect on explicit memory. Mild cognitive impairment is an early sign of Alzheimer's disease. People with memory conditions often receive cognitive training. When an fMRI was used to view brain activity after training, it found increased activation in various neural systems that are involved with explicit memory. [67] People with Alzheimer's have problems learning new tasks. However, if the task is presented repeatedly they can learn and retain some new knowledge of the task. This effect is more apparent if the information is familiar. The person with Alzheimer's must also be guided through the task and prevented from making errors. [68] Alzheimer's also has an effect on explicit spatial memory. This means that people with Alzheimer's have difficulty remembering where items are placed in unfamiliar environments. [69] The hippocampus has been shown to become active in semantic and episodic memory. [70]

The effects of Alzheimer's disease are seen in the episodic part of explicit memory. This can lead to problems with communication. A study was conducted where Alzheimer's patients were asked to name a variety of objects from different periods. The results shown that their ability to name the object depended on frequency of use of the item and when the item was first acquired. [71] This effect on semantic memory also has an effect on music and tones. Alzheimer's patients have difficulty distinguishing between different melodies they have never heard before. People with Alzheimer's also have issues with picturing future events. This is due to a deficit in episodic future thinking. [72] There are many other reasons why adults and others may begin to have memory loss.

Amnesia is frequently portrayed in television and movies. Some of the better-known examples include:

In the romantic comedy 50 First Dates (2004), Adam Sandler plays veterinarian Henry Roth, who falls for Lucy Whitmore, played by Drew Barrymore. Having lost her short-term memory in a car crash, Lucy can only remember the current day's events until she falls asleep. When she wakes up the next morning, she has no recollection of the previous day's experiences. [73] Those experiences would normally be transferred into declarative knowledge and allow them to be recalled in the future. The movie is not the most accurate representation of a true amnesic patient, but it is useful to inform viewers of the detrimental effects of amnesia.

Memento (2000) a film inspired by the case of Henry Molaison (H.M.). [74] Guy Pearce plays a former insurance investigator suffering from severe anterograde amnesia, which was caused by a head injury. Unlike most other amnesiacs, Leonard retains his identity and the memories of events that occurred before the injury but has lost all ability to form new memories. That loss of ability indicates that the head injury affected the medial temporal lobe of the brain, which has resulted in his inability to form declarative memory.

Finding Nemo features a reef fish named Dory with an inability to develop declarative memory. That prevents her from learning or retaining any new information such as names or directions. The exact origin of Dory's impairment is not mentioned in the film, but her memory loss accurately portrays the difficulties facing amnesiacs. [73]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Entorhinal cortex</span> Area of the temporal lobe of the brain

The entorhinal cortex (EC) is an area of the brain's allocortex, located in the medial temporal lobe, whose functions include being a widespread network hub for memory, navigation, and the perception of time. The EC is the main interface between the hippocampus and neocortex. The EC-hippocampus system plays an important role in declarative (autobiographical/episodic/semantic) memories and in particular spatial memories including memory formation, memory consolidation, and memory optimization in sleep. The EC is also responsible for the pre-processing (familiarity) of the input signals in the reflex nictitating membrane response of classical trace conditioning; the association of impulses from the eye and the ear occurs in the entorhinal cortex.

Long-term memory (LTM) is the stage of the Atkinson–Shiffrin memory model in which informative knowledge is held indefinitely. It is defined in contrast to sensory memory, the initial stage, and short-term or working memory, the second stage, which persists for about 18 to 30 seconds. LTM is grouped into two categories known as explicit memory and implicit memory. Explicit memory is broken down into episodic and semantic memory, while implicit memory includes procedural memory and emotional conditioning.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hippocampus</span> Vertebrate brain region involved in memory consolidation

The hippocampus is a major component of the brain of humans and other vertebrates. Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each side of the brain. The hippocampus is part of the limbic system, and plays important roles in the consolidation of information from short-term memory to long-term memory, and in spatial memory that enables navigation. The hippocampus is located in the allocortex, with neural projections into the neocortex, in humans as well as other primates. The hippocampus, as the medial pallium, is a structure found in all vertebrates. In humans, it contains two main interlocking parts: the hippocampus proper, and the dentate gyrus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Limbic system</span> Set of brain structures involved in emotion and motivation

The limbic system, also known as the paleomammalian cortex, is a set of brain structures located on both sides of the thalamus, immediately beneath the medial temporal lobe of the cerebrum primarily in the forebrain.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Henry Molaison</span> American memory disorder patient

Henry Gustav Molaison, known widely as H.M., was an American who had a bilateral medial temporal lobectomy to surgically resect the anterior two thirds of his hippocampi, parahippocampal cortices, entorhinal cortices, piriform cortices, and amygdalae in an attempt to cure his epilepsy. Although the surgery was partially successful in controlling his epilepsy, a severe side effect was that he became unable to form new memories. His unique case also helped define ethical standards in neurological research, emphasizing the need for patient consent and the consideration of long-term impacts of medical interventions. Furthermore, Molaison's life after his surgery highlighted the challenges and adaptations required for living with significant memory impairments, serving as an important case study for healthcare professionals and caregivers dealing with similar conditions.

In neurology, anterograde amnesia is the inability to create new memories after an event that caused amnesia, leading to a partial or complete inability to recall the recent past, while long-term memories from before the event remain intact. This is in contrast to retrograde amnesia, where memories created prior to the event are lost while new memories can still be created. Both can occur together in the same patient. To a large degree, anterograde amnesia remains a mysterious ailment because the precise mechanism of storing memories is not yet well understood, although it is known that the regions of the brain involved are certain sites in the temporal cortex, especially in the hippocampus and nearby subcortical regions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fornix (neuroanatomy)</span> Bundle of nerve fibers in the brain

The fornix is a C-shaped bundle of nerve fibers in the brain that acts as the major output tract of the hippocampus. The fornix also carries some afferent fibers to the hippocampus from structures in the diencephalon and basal forebrain. The fornix is part of the limbic system. While its exact function and importance in the physiology of the brain are still not entirely clear, it has been demonstrated in humans that surgical transection—the cutting of the fornix along its body—can cause memory loss. There is some debate over what type of memory is affected by this damage, but it has been found to most closely correlate with recall memory rather than recognition memory. This means that damage to the fornix can cause difficulty in recalling long-term information such as details of past events, but it has little effect on the ability to recognize objects or familiar situations.

Episodic memory is the memory of everyday events that can be explicitly stated or conjured. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at particular times and places; for example, the party on one's 7th birthday. Along with semantic memory, it comprises the category of explicit memory, one of the two major divisions of long-term memory.

An engram is a unit of cognitive information imprinted in a physical substance, theorized to be the means by which memories are stored as biophysical or biochemical changes in the brain or other biological tissue, in response to external stimuli.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Spatial memory</span> Memory about ones environment and spatial orientation

In cognitive psychology and neuroscience, spatial memory is a form of memory responsible for the recording and recovery of information needed to plan a course to a location and to recall the location of an object or the occurrence of an event. Spatial memory is necessary for orientation in space. Spatial memory can also be divided into egocentric and allocentric spatial memory. A person's spatial memory is required to navigate around a familiar city. A rat's spatial memory is needed to learn the location of food at the end of a maze. In both humans and animals, spatial memories are summarized as a cognitive map.

In neurology, retrograde amnesia (RA) is the inability to access memories or information from before an injury or disease occurred. RA differs from a similar condition called anterograde amnesia (AA), which is the inability to form new memories following injury or disease onset. Although an individual can have both RA and AA at the same time, RA can also occur on its own; this 'pure' form of RA can be further divided into three types: focal, isolated, and pure RA. RA negatively affects an individual's episodic, autobiographical, and declarative memory, but they can still form new memories because RA leaves procedural memory intact. Depending on its severity, RA can result in either temporally graded or more permanent memory loss. However, memory loss usually follows Ribot's law, which states that individuals are more likely to lose recent memories than older memories. Diagnosing RA generally requires using an Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI) and observing brain structure through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), a computed tomography scan (CT), or electroencephalography (EEG).

Retrospective memory is the memory of people, words, and events encountered or experienced in the past. It includes all other types of memory including episodic, semantic and procedural. It can be either implicit or explicit. In contrast, prospective memory involves remembering something or remembering to do something after a delay, such as buying groceries on the way home from work. However, it is very closely linked to retrospective memory, since certain aspects of retrospective memory are required for prospective memory.

Memory consolidation is a category of processes that stabilize a memory trace after its initial acquisition. A memory trace is a change in the nervous system caused by memorizing something. Consolidation is distinguished into two specific processes. The first, synaptic consolidation, which is thought to correspond to late-phase long-term potentiation, occurs on a small scale in the synaptic connections and neural circuits within the first few hours after learning. The second process is systems consolidation, occurring on a much larger scale in the brain, rendering hippocampus-dependent memories independent of the hippocampus over a period of weeks to years. Recently, a third process has become the focus of research, reconsolidation, in which previously consolidated memories can be made labile again through reactivation of the memory trace.

The neuroanatomy of memory encompasses a wide variety of anatomical structures in the brain.

Amnesia is a deficit in memory caused by brain damage or brain diseases, but it can also be temporarily caused by the use of various sedative and hypnotic drugs. The memory can be either wholly or partially lost due to the extent of damage that is caused.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sleep and memory</span> Relationship between sleep and memory

The relationship between sleep and memory has been studied since at least the early 19th century. Memory, the cognitive process of storing and retrieving past experiences, learning and recognition, is a product of brain plasticity, the structural changes within synapses that create associations between stimuli. Stimuli are encoded within milliseconds; however, the long-term maintenance of memories can take additional minutes, days, or even years to fully consolidate and become a stable memory that is accessible. Therefore, the formation of a specific memory occurs rapidly, but the evolution of a memory is often an ongoing process.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Effects of stress on memory</span> Overview of the effects of stress on memory

The effects of stress on memory include interference with a person's capacity to encode memory and the ability to retrieve information. Stimuli, like stress, improved memory when it was related to learning the subject. During times of stress, the body reacts by secreting stress hormones into the bloodstream. Stress can cause acute and chronic changes in certain brain areas which can cause long-term damage. Over-secretion of stress hormones most frequently impairs long-term delayed recall memory, but can enhance short-term, immediate recall memory. This enhancement is particularly relative in emotional memory. In particular, the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex and the amygdala are affected. One class of stress hormone responsible for negatively affecting long-term, delayed recall memory is the glucocorticoids (GCs), the most notable of which is cortisol. Glucocorticoids facilitate and impair the actions of stress in the brain memory process. Cortisol is a known biomarker for stress. Under normal circumstances, the hippocampus regulates the production of cortisol through negative feedback because it has many receptors that are sensitive to these stress hormones. However, an excess of cortisol can impair the ability of the hippocampus to both encode and recall memories. These stress hormones are also hindering the hippocampus from receiving enough energy by diverting glucose levels to surrounding muscles.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Memory</span> Faculty of mind to store and retrieve data

Memory is the faculty of the mind by which data or information is encoded, stored, and retrieved when needed. It is the retention of information over time for the purpose of influencing future action. If past events could not be remembered, it would be impossible for language, relationships, or personal identity to develop. Memory loss is usually described as forgetfulness or amnesia.

The between-systems memory interference model describes the inhibition of non-hippocampal systems of memory during concurrent hippocampal activity. Specifically, Fraser Sparks, Hugo Lehmann, and Robert Sutherland found that when the hippocampus was inactive, non-hippocampal systems located elsewhere in the brain were found to consolidate memory in its place. However, when the hippocampus was reactivated, memory traces consolidated by non-hippocampal systems were not recalled, suggesting that the hippocampus interferes with long-term memory consolidation in other memory-related systems.

The hippocampus participates in the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of memories. The hippocampus is located in the medial temporal lobe (subcortical), and is an infolding of the medial temporal cortex. The hippocampus plays an important role in the transfer of information from short-term memory to long-term memory during encoding and retrieval stages. These stages do not need to occur successively, but are, as studies seem to indicate, and they are broadly divided in the neuronal mechanisms that they require or even in the hippocampal areas that they seem to activate. According to Gazzaniga, "encoding is the processing of incoming information that creates memory traces to be stored." There are two steps to the encoding process: "acquisition" and "consolidation". During the acquisition process, stimuli are committed to short term memory. Then, consolidation is where the hippocampus along with other cortical structures stabilize an object within long term memory, which strengthens over time, and is a process for which a number of theories have arisen to explain the underlying mechanism. After encoding, the hippocampus is capable of going through the retrieval process. The retrieval process consists of accessing stored information; this allows learned behaviors to experience conscious depiction and execution. Encoding and retrieval are both affected by neurodegenerative and anxiety disorders and epilepsy.

References

  1. Ullman, MT (2004). "Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 231–70. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008. PMID   15037131. S2CID   14611894.
  2. Lazzarim, Mayla K, Targa, Adriano, Sardi, Natalia F, et al. Pain impairs consolidation, but not acquisition or retrieval of a declarative memory. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2020;31(8):707-715. doi:10.1097/FBP.0000000000000576.
  3. Kandel ER, Dudai, Y, Mayford MR. (2014). The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell157:163–186
  4. 1 2 3 4 Tulving E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of Memory, ed. E Tulving, W Donaldson, pp. 381–403. New York: Academic
  5. McRae, Ken; Jones, Michael (2013). Reisberg, Daniel (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. pp. 206–216. ISBN   9780195376746.
  6. Tulving, Endel (2002). "Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain". Annual Review of Psychology . 53: 1–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. PMID   11752477.
  7. Saumier, D.; Chertkow, H. (2002). "Semantic Memory". Current Science. 2 (6): 516–522. doi:10.1007/s11910-002-0039-9. PMID   12359106. S2CID   14184578.
  8. Williams, H. L., Conway, M. A., & Cohen, G. (2008). Autobiographical memory. In G. Cohen & M. A. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the Real World (3rd ed., pp. 21-90). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  9. Ell, Shawn; Zilioli, Monica (2012), "Categorical Learning", in Seel, Norbert M. (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Springer US, pp. 509–512, doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_98, ISBN   978-1-4419-1428-6
  10. Baars, Bernard J.; Gage, Nicole M. (2010), "Emotion", Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness, Elsevier, pp. 420–442, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-375070-9.00013-9, ISBN   978-0-12-375070-9 , retrieved 2024-01-24
  11. "Broca's Area - an overview | ScienceDirect Topics". www.sciencedirect.com. Retrieved 2019-12-17.
  12. Aristotle, On the Soul (De Anima), in Aristotle, Volume 4, Loeb Classical Library, William Heinemann, London, UK, 1936.
  13. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. Teachers College, Columbia University
  14. Graf, P.; Schacter, D. L. (1985). "Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 11 (3): 501–518. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501. PMID   3160813.
  15. 1 2 Eichenbaum, Howard (1997). "Declarative memory: Insights from cognitive neurobiology". Annual Review of Psychology . 48: 547–572. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.547. PMID   9046568.
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 Eichenbaum, Howard (2001). "The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Codes". Behavioural Brain Research. 127 (1–2): 199–207. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00365-5. PMID   11718892. S2CID   20843130.
  17. 1 2 3 4 Gabrieli, J.; Kao, Y. (2007). "Development of the Declarative Memory System in the Human Brain". Nature Neuroscience. 10 (9): 1198–1205. doi:10.1038/nn1950. PMID   17676059. S2CID   15637865.
  18. Manns, Joseph; Eichenbaum, Howard (September 2006). "Evolution of Declarative Memory". Hippocampus. 16 (9): 795–808. doi:10.1002/hipo.20205. PMID   16881079. S2CID   39081299.
  19. Davachi, L.; Mitchell, J.P.; Wagner, A.D. (2003). "Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (4): 2157–2162. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.2157D. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337195100 . PMC   149975 . PMID   12578977.
  20. Davachi, Dobbins (2008). "Declarative Memory". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (2): 112–118. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00559.x. PMC   2790294 . PMID   20011622.
  21. 1 2 Graham, S.; Levine, B. (2004). "The Fundamental Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16 (9): 1633–1646. doi:10.1162/0898929042568587. PMID   15601525. S2CID   32682464.
  22. 1 2 Craik, F.I.M.; Houle, S. (1994). "Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Human Episodic Memory: Lessons From PET Studies". Biol. Psychiatry. 42: 75S–76S. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(97)87185-5. S2CID   54399679.
  23. Stuss, D.T.; Tulving, E. (1997). "Toward a Theory of Episodic Memory: The Frontal Lobes & Autonoetic Consciousness". Psychological Bulletin. 121 (3): 331–354. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.331. PMID   9136640.
  24. Sharot, T; Martorella, EA; Delgado, MR; Phelps, EA (2007). "How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11". Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 104 (1): 389–394. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104..389S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609230103 . PMC   1713166 . PMID   17182739.
  25. Adolphs, R; Tranel, D; Buchanan, TW (2005). "Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli" (PDF). Nat Neurosci. 8 (4): 512–518. doi:10.1038/nn1413. PMID   15735643. S2CID   205430564.
  26. Adolphs, R; Denburg, NL; Tranel, D (2001). "The amygdala's role in long-term declarative memory for gist and detail". Behav Neurosci. 115 (5): 983–992. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.115.5.983. PMID   11584931.
  27. 1 2 Eichenbaum, H.; Stewart, C.; Morris, R. G. M. (1990). "Hippocampal representation in spatial learning". J. Neurosci. 10 (11): 331–339. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-11-03531.1990 . PMC   6570096 . PMID   2230943.
  28. 1 2 3 Eichenbaum, H (2000). "A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 1 (1): 41–50. doi:10.1038/35036213. PMID   11252767. S2CID   5242470.
  29. Bunsey, M.; Eichenbaum, H. (1995). "Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus-stimulus association". Hippocampus. 5 (6): 546–556. doi:10.1002/hipo.450050606. PMID   8646281. S2CID   37972779.
  30. 1 2 Corkin, S (2002). "Perspectives: What's new with the amnesic patient H.M.?". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3 (2): 153–160. doi:10.1038/nrn726. PMID   11836523. S2CID   5429133.
  31. Gabrieli, J. D. E.; Cohen, N. J.; Corkin, S. (1988). "The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection". Brain Cogn. 7 (2): 157–177. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(88)90027-9. PMID   3377896. S2CID   23026117.
  32. Corkin, S (1968). "Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 255–264. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90024-9.
  33. Milner, B.; Corkin, S.; Teuber, H.-L. (1968). "Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M.". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 215–234. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3.
  34. Aggleton, J. P.; Brown, M. W. (1999). "Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis" (PDF). Behav. Brain Sci. 22 (3): 425–489. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99002034. PMID   11301518. S2CID   11258997.
  35. 1 2 3 Adolphs, R.; Cahill, L.; Schul, R.; Babinsky, R. (1997). "Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans". Learning and Memory. 4 (3): 291–300. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.3.291 . PMID   10456070.
  36. Babinsky, R.; Calabrese, P.; Durwen, H.; Markowitsch, H.; Brechtelsbuauer, D. (1993). "The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory". Behav. Neurol. 6 (3): 167–170. doi: 10.1155/1993/684234 . PMID   24487116.
  37. Lupien, S.; Gaudreau, S.; Tchiteya, B.; Maheu, F.; Sharma, S.; Nair, N.; et al. (1997). "Stress-Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects: Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 82 (7): 2070–2075. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.7.4075 . PMID   9215274.
  38. Cabeza, R.; LaBar, K.S. (2006). "Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (1): 54–64. doi:10.1038/nrn1825. PMID   16371950. S2CID   1829420.
  39. Baker, D.G.; et al. (2005). "Higher levels of basal CSF cortisol in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder". American Journal of Psychiatry. 162 (5): 992–994. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.992. PMID   15863803.
  40. Stephens, Mary Ann C.; Wand, Gary (2012). "Stress and the HPA Axis". Alcohol Research: Current Reviews. 34 (4): 468–483. ISSN   2168-3492. PMC   3860380 . PMID   23584113.
  41. Notaras, Michael; van den Buuse, Maarten (2020-01-03). "Neurobiology of BDNF in fear memory, sensitivity to stress, and stress-related disorders". Molecular Psychiatry. 25 (10): 2251–2274. doi:10.1038/s41380-019-0639-2. ISSN   1476-5578. PMID   31900428. S2CID   209540967.
  42. Segerstrom, Suzanne C.; Miller, Gregory E. (July 2004). "Psychological Stress and the Human Immune System: A Meta-Analytic Study of 30 Years of Inquiry". Psychological Bulletin. 130 (4): 601–630. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601. ISSN   0033-2909. PMC   1361287 . PMID   15250815.
  43. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Damoiseaux, J.S.; Elzinga, B.M. (2007). "Glucocorticoids Decrease Hippocampal and Prefrontal Activation during Declarative Memory Retrieval in Young Men". Brain Imaging and Behavior. 1 (1–2): 31–41. doi:10.1007/s11682-007-9003-2. PMC   2780685 . PMID   19946603.
  44. McClelland, J.L.; McNaughton, B.L.; O'Reilly, R.C. (1995). "Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory". Psychol. Rev. 102 (3): 419–457. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.3.419. PMID   7624455. S2CID   2832081.
  45. Ji, D.; Wilson, M. A. (2007). "Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep". Nat. Neurosci. 10 (1): 100–7. doi:10.1038/nn1825. PMID   17173043. S2CID   205431067.
  46. Peigneux, P.; et al. (2004). "Are Spatial Memories Strengthened in the Human Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep?". Neuron. 44 (3): 535–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.007 . PMID   15504332. S2CID   1424898.
  47. 1 2 Ellenbogen, J; Payne, D; et al. (2006). "The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 16 (6): 716–722. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.006. PMID   17085038. S2CID   15514443.
  48. Tucker A, Fishbein W, (2009) The Impact of sleep duration and subject intelligence on declarative and motor memory performance: how much is enough? J. Sleep Res., 304-312
  49. Plihal, W; Born, J (1997). "Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory". J Cogn Neurosci. 9 (4): 534–547. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.534. PMID   23968216. S2CID   3300300.
  50. Backhaus, J.; Junghanns, K.; Born, J.; Hohaus, K.; Faasch, F.; Hohagen, F. (2006). "Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary insomnia: Influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release". Biol. Psychiatry. 60 (12): 1324–1330. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.051. PMID   16876140. S2CID   32826396.
  51. Backhaus, J; Born, J; et al. (2007). "Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep". Learning & Memory. 14 (5): 336–341. doi:10.1101/lm.470507. PMC   1876757 . PMID   17522024.
  52. Gais, S; Molle, M; Helms, K; Born, J (2002). "Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density". J Neurosci. 22 (15): 6830–6834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06830.2002 . PMC   6758170 . PMID   12151563.
  53. Schabus, M; Hodlmoser, K; Gruber, G; Sauter, C; Anderer, P; Klosch, G; Parapatics, S; Saletu, B; Klimesch, W; Zeitlhofer, J (2006). "Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities". Eur J Neurosci. 23 (7): 1738–1746. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04694.x. PMID   16623830. S2CID   3168339.
  54. 1 2 Schabus, M; Gruber, G; Parapatics, S; et al. (2004). "Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation". Sleep. 27 (8): 1479–85. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1479 . PMID   15683137.
  55. Wagner, U; Gais, S; Born, J (2001). "Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep". Learn Mem. 8 (2): 112–119. doi:10.1101/lm.36801. PMC   311359 . PMID   11274257.
  56. Hu, P; Stylos-Allen, M; Walker, MP (2006). "Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing declarative memory". Psychol Sci. 17 (10): 891–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01799.x. PMID   17100790. S2CID   13535490.
  57. Ellenbogen, J; Hulbert, J; et al. (2006). ""Interfering with Theories of Sleep and Memory " Sleep, Declarative Memory, and Associative Interference". Current Biology. 16 (13): 1290–1294. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.024 . PMID   16824917. S2CID   10114241.
  58. Wixted, JT (2004). "The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting". Annu Rev Psychol. 55: 235–269. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555. PMID   14744216. S2CID   3057114.
  59. 1 2 3 4 5 Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2003). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Worth Publishers. pp. 454–455. ISBN   978-071675300-1.
  60. Kandel, E.R.; Schwartz, J.H.; Jessell, T.M.; Siegelbaum, S.A.; Hudspeth, A.J. (2013). Principles of neural science. McGraw Hill Medical. ISBN   978-0-07-139011-8.
  61. Craik, F.I.M.; Lockhart, R.S. (1972). "Levels of processing: a framework for memory research". J Verb Learn Verb Behav. 11 (6): 671–684. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X. S2CID   14153362.
  62. Einstein, G.O.; Mullet, H.G.; Harrison, T.L. (2012). "The testing effect: Illustrating a fundamental concept and changing study strategies". Teaching of Psychology. 39 (3): 190–193. doi:10.1177/0098628312450432. S2CID   14352611.
  63. Unsworth, N.; Brewer, G. A.; Spillers, G. J. (2011). "Variation in working memory capacity and episodic memory: Examining the importance of encoding specificity". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18 (6): 1113–1118. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0165-y . PMID   21912997.
  64. Staresina, B. P.; Gray, J. C.; Davachi, L. (2009). "Event congruency enhances episodic memory encoding through semantic elaboration and relational binding". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (5): 1198–1207. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn165. PMC   2665161 . PMID   18820289.
  65. H.L. Petri and M. Mishkin: Behaviorism, cognitivism, and the neuropsychology of memory, in: American scientist, 82:30-37, 1994
  66. Lah, S.; Epps, A.; Levick, W.; Parry, L. (2011). "Implicit and explicit memory outcome in children who have sustained severe traumatic brain injury: Impact of age at injury (preliminary findings)". Brain Injury. 25 (1): 44–52. doi:10.3109/02699052.2010.531693. PMID   21117914. S2CID   11678379.
  67. Hampstead, B. M.; Stringer, A.Y.; Stilla, R.F.; Deshpande, G.; Hu, X.; Moore, A.; Sathian, K.K. (2011). "Activation and effective connectivity changes following explicit-memory training for face–name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A pilot study". Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 25 (3): 210–222. doi:10.1177/1545968310382424. PMC   3595021 . PMID   20935339.
  68. Metzler-Baddeley, C.; Snowden, J. S. (2005). "Brief report: Errorless versus errorful learning as a memory rehabilitation approach in Alzheimer's disease". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 27 (8): 1070–1079. doi:10.1080/13803390490919164. PMID   16207625. S2CID   26207056.
  69. Kessels, R. C.; Feijen, J. J.; Postma, A. A. (2005). "Implicit and Explicit Memory for Spatial Information in Alzheimer's Disease". Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 20 (2–3): 184–191. doi:10.1159/000087233. PMID   16088143. S2CID   1855801.
  70. Hoscheidt, S. M.; Nadel, L.; Payne, J.; Ryan, L. (2010). "Hippocampal activation during retrieval of spatial context from episodic and semantic memory". Behavioural Brain Research. 212 (2): 121–132. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.010. PMID   20385169. S2CID   12588078.
  71. Small, J. A.; Sandhu, N. (2008). "Episodic and semantic memory influences on picture naming in Alzheimer's disease". Brain and Language. 104 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.12.002. PMID   17223189. S2CID   5866604.
  72. Irish, M.; Addis, D.; Hodges, J. R.; Piguet, O. (2012). "Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: Evidence from semantic dementia". Brain. 135 (7): 2178–2191. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws119 . PMID   22614246.
  73. 1 2 Baxtendale, S (2004). "Memories aren't made of this: Amnesia at the movies". British Medical Journal. 329 (7480): 1480–1483. doi:10.1136/bmj.329.7480.1480. PMC   535990 . PMID   15604191.
  74. Chun, M (2005). "Drug-induced amnesia impairs implicit relational memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (8): 355–357. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.015. PMID   16006177. S2CID   6053734.