Palatability

Last updated
Advertisement of castor oil as a medicine by Scott & Bowne company, 19th century Scott & Bowne's Palatable Castor Oil.jpg
Advertisement of castor oil as a medicine by Scott & Bowne company, 19th century

Palatability (or palatableness) is the hedonic reward (which is pleasure of taste in this case) provided by foods or drinks that are agreeable to the "palate", which often varies relative to the homeostatic satisfaction of nutritional and/or water needs. [1] The palatability of a dish or beverage, unlike its flavor or taste, varies with the state of an individual: it is lower after consumption and higher when deprived. It has increasingly been appreciated that this can create a hunger that is independent of homeostatic needs. [2]

Contents

Brain mechanism

The palatability of a substance is determined by opioid receptor-related processes in the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum. [3] The opioid processes involve mu opioid receptors and are present in the rostromedial shell part of the nucleus accumbens [4] on its spiny neurons. [5] This area has been called the "opioid eating site". [6]

The rewardfulness of consumption associated with palatability is dissociable from desire or incentive value which is the motivation to seek out a specific commodity. [3] Desire or incentive value is processed by opioid receptor-related processes in the basolateral amygdala. [3] Unlike the liking palatability for food, the incentive salience wanting is not downregulated by the physiological consequences of food consumption and may be largely independent of homoeostatic processes influencing food intake. [7]

Though the wanting of incentive salience may be informed by palatability, it is independent and not necessarily reduced to it. [3] It has been suggested that a third system exists that links opioid processes in the two parts of the brain: "Logically this raises the possibility that a third system, with which the accumbens shell, ventral pallidum, and basolateral amygdala are associated, distributes the affective signals elicited by specific commodities across distinct functional systems to control reward seeking... At present we do not have any direct evidence for a system of this kind, but indirect evidence suggests it may reside within the motivationally rich circuits linking hypothalamic and brainstem viscerogenic structures such as the parabrachial nucleus. [3]

It has also been suggested that hedonic hunger can be driven both in regard to "wanting" and "liking" [2] and that a palatability subtype of neuron may also exist in the basolateral amygdala. [8]

Satiety and palatability

Appetite is controlled by a direct loop and an indirect one. In both the direct and indirect loops there are two feedback mechanisms. First a positive feedback involving its stimulation by palatability food cues, and second, a negative feedback due to satiation and satiety cues following ingestion. [9] In the indirect loop these cues are learnt by association such as meal plate size and work by modulating the potency of the cues of the direct loop. [10] The influence of these processes can exist without subjective awareness. [11]

The cessation of a desire to eat after a meal "satiation" is likely to be due to different processes and cues. [12] More palatable foods reduce the effects of such cues upon satiation causing a larger food intake, exploited in hyperpalatable food. [13] [14] In contrast, unpalatability of certain foods can serve as a deterrent from feeding on those foods in the future. For example, the variable checkerspot butterfly contains iridoid compounds that are unpalatable to avian predators, thus reducing the risk of predation. [15]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Amygdala</span> Each of two small structures deep within the temporal lobe of complex vertebrates

The amygdala is a paired nuclear complex present in the cerebral hemispheres of vertebrates. It is considered part of the limbic system. In primates, it is located medially within the temporal lobes. It consists of many nuclei, each made up of further subnuclei. The subdivision most commonly made is into the basolateral, central, cortical, and medial nuclei together with the intercalated cell clusters. The amygdala has a primary role in the processing of memory, decision-making, and emotional responses. The amygdala was first identified and named by Karl Friedrich Burdach in 1822.

The mesolimbic pathway, sometimes referred to as the reward pathway, is a dopaminergic pathway in the brain. The pathway connects the ventral tegmental area in the midbrain to the ventral striatum of the basal ganglia in the forebrain. The ventral striatum includes the nucleus accumbens and the olfactory tubercle.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nucleus accumbens</span> Region of the basal forebrain

The nucleus accumbens is a region in the basal forebrain rostral to the preoptic area of the hypothalamus. The nucleus accumbens and the olfactory tubercle collectively form the ventral striatum. The ventral striatum and dorsal striatum collectively form the striatum, which is the main component of the basal ganglia. The dopaminergic neurons of the mesolimbic pathway project onto the GABAergic medium spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens and olfactory tubercle. Each cerebral hemisphere has its own nucleus accumbens, which can be divided into two structures: the nucleus accumbens core and the nucleus accumbens shell. These substructures have different morphology and functions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dopaminergic pathways</span> Projection neurons in the brain that synthesize and release dopamine

Dopaminergic pathways in the human brain are involved in both physiological and behavioral processes including movement, cognition, executive functions, reward, motivation, and neuroendocrine control. Each pathway is a set of projection neurons, consisting of individual dopaminergic neurons.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ventral tegmental area</span> Group of neurons on the floor of the midbrain

The ventral tegmental area (VTA), also known as the ventral tegmental area of Tsai, or simply ventral tegmentum, is a group of neurons located close to the midline on the floor of the midbrain. The VTA is the origin of the dopaminergic cell bodies of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system and other dopamine pathways; it is widely implicated in the drug and natural reward circuitry of the brain. The VTA plays an important role in a number of processes, including reward cognition and orgasm, among others, as well as several psychiatric disorders. Neurons in the VTA project to numerous areas of the brain, ranging from the prefrontal cortex to the caudal brainstem and several regions in between.

Motivational salience is a cognitive process and a form of attention that motivates or propels an individual's behavior towards or away from a particular object, perceived event or outcome. Motivational salience regulates the intensity of behaviors that facilitate the attainment of a particular goal, the amount of time and energy that an individual is willing to expend to attain a particular goal, and the amount of risk that an individual is willing to accept while working to attain a particular goal.

The amygdalofugal pathway is one of the three major efferent pathways of the amygdala, meaning that it is one of the three principal pathways by which fibers leave the amygdala. It leads from the basolateral nucleus and central nucleus of the amygdala. The amygdala is a limbic structure in the medial temporal lobe of the brain. The other main efferent pathways from the amygdala are the stria terminalis and anterior commissure.

Kent C. Berridge is an American academic, currently working as a professor of psychology (biopsychology) and neuroscience at the University of Michigan. Berridge was a joint winner of the 2018 Grawemeyer Award for Psychology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reward system</span> Group of neural structures responsible for motivation and desire

The reward system is a group of neural structures responsible for incentive salience, associative learning, and positively-valenced emotions, particularly ones involving pleasure as a core component. Reward is the attractive and motivational property of a stimulus that induces appetitive behavior, also known as approach behavior, and consummatory behavior. A rewarding stimulus has been described as "any stimulus, object, event, activity, or situation that has the potential to make us approach and consume it is by definition a reward". In operant conditioning, rewarding stimuli function as positive reinforcers; however, the converse statement also holds true: positive reinforcers are rewarding.The reward system motivates animals to approach stimuli or engage in behaviour that increases fitness. Survival for most animal species depends upon maximizing contact with beneficial stimuli and minimizing contact with harmful stimuli. Reward cognition serves to increase the likelihood of survival and reproduction by causing associative learning, eliciting approach and consummatory behavior, and triggering positively-valenced emotions. Thus, reward is a mechanism that evolved to help increase the adaptive fitness of animals. In drug addiction, certain substances over-activate the reward circuit, leading to compulsive substance-seeking behavior resulting from synaptic plasticity in the circuit.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Basolateral amygdala</span> The lateral, basal, and accessory-basal nuclei of the amygdala

The basolateral amygdala, or basolateral complex, consists of the lateral, basal and accessory-basal nuclei of the amygdala. The lateral nuclei receives the majority of sensory information, which arrives directly from the temporal lobe structures, including the hippocampus and primary auditory cortex. The basolateral amygdala also receives dense neuromodulatory inputs from ventral tegmental area (VTA), locus coeruleus (LC), and basal forebrain, whose integrity are important for associative learning. The information is then processed by the basolateral complex and is sent as output to the central nucleus of the amygdala. This is how most emotional arousal is formed in mammals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Frisson</span> Psychophysiological response to rewarding auditory or visual stimuli

Frisson, also known as aesthetic chills or psychogenic shivers, is a psychophysiological response to rewarding stimuli that often induces a pleasurable or otherwise positively-valenced affective state and transient paresthesia, sometimes along with piloerection and mydriasis . The sensation commonly occurs as a mildly to moderately pleasurable emotional response to music with skin tingling; piloerection and pupil dilation not necessarily occurring in all cases.

Hunger is a sensation that motivates the consumption of food. The sensation of hunger typically manifests after only a few hours without eating and is generally considered to be unpleasant. Satiety occurs between 5 and 20 minutes after eating. There are several theories about how the feeling of hunger arises. The desire to eat food, or appetite, is another sensation experienced with regard to eating.

The ventral pallidum (VP) is a structure within the basal ganglia of the brain. It is an output nucleus whose fibres project to thalamic nuclei, such as the ventral anterior nucleus, the ventral lateral nucleus, and the medial dorsal nucleus. The VP is a core component of the reward system which forms part of the limbic loop of the basal ganglia, a pathway involved in the regulation of motivational salience, behavior, and emotions. It is involved in addiction.

Animal models of depression are research tools used to investigate depression and action of antidepressants as a simulation to investigate the symptomatology and pathophysiology of depressive illness or used to screen novel antidepressants. These models provide insights into molecular, genetic, and epigenetic factors associated with depression. Criteria for valid animal models include face, construct, and predictive validity. Endophenotypes, such as anhedonia, behavioral despair, changes in appetite, neuroanatomical alterations, neuroendocrine disturbances, alterations in sleep architecture, and anxiety-related behaviors, are evaluated in these models. Antidepressant screening tests are employed to assess the effects of genetic, pharmacological, or environmental manipulations. Stress models, including learned helplessness, chronic mild stress, and social defeat stress, simulate the impact of stressors on depression. Early life stress models, psychostimulant withdrawal models, olfactory bulbectomy, and genetically engineered mice contribute to a comprehensive understanding of depression's etiology and potential therapeutic interventions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Central nucleus of the amygdala</span> Nucleus within the amygdala

The central nucleus of the amygdala is a nucleus within the amygdala. It "serves as the major output nucleus of the amygdala and participates in receiving and processing pain information."

Hedonic hunger or hedonic hyperphagia is the "drive to eat to obtain pleasure in the absence of an energy deficit". Particular foods may have a high "hedonic rating" or individuals may have increased susceptibility to environmental food cues. Weight loss programs may aim to control or to compensate for hedonic hunger. Therapeutic interventions may influence hedonic eating behavior.

Syrian hamster behavior refers to the ethology of the Syrian hamster.

Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT) is a psychological phenomenon that occurs when a conditioned stimulus that has been associated with rewarding or aversive stimuli via classical conditioning alters motivational salience and operant behavior. Two distinct forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer have been identified in humans and other animals – specific PIT and general PIT – with unique neural substrates mediating each type. In relation to rewarding stimuli, specific PIT occurs when a CS is associated with a specific rewarding stimulus through classical conditioning and subsequent exposure to the CS enhances an operant response that is directed toward the same reward with which it was paired. General PIT occurs when a CS is paired with one reward and it enhances an operant response that is directed toward a different rewarding stimulus.

Ann Elizabeth Kelley (1954–2007) was an American neuroscientist, who specialized in the neuroscience of reward and behavior. She was a professor at the University of Wisconsin.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Kate Wassum</span> American neuroscientist

Kate Wassum is an American neuroscientist and professor of behavioral neuroscience at the University of California, Los Angeles. Wassum probes the neural circuits underlying appetitive associative learning the circuit dynamics that give rise to diverse motivated behaviors.

References

  1. Friedman, Mark I.; Stricker, Edward M. (1976). "The physiological psychology of hunger: A physiological perspective". Psychological Review. 83 (6): 409–431. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.6.409. ISSN   1939-1471. PMID   1005583.
  2. 1 2 Lowe, Michael R.; Butryn, Meghan L. (2007-07-24). "Hedonic hunger: A new dimension of appetite?". Physiology & Behavior. 91 (4): 432–439. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006. ISSN   0031-9384. PMID   17531274. S2CID   10704679.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 Wassum, K. M.; Ostlund, S. B.; Maidment, N. T.; Balleine, B. W. (2009-07-28). "Distinct opioid circuits determine the palatability and the desirability of rewarding events". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (30): 12512–12517. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10612512W. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905874106 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   2718390 . PMID   19597155.
  4. Peciña, Susana; Berridge, Kent C. (2005-12-14). "Hedonic Hot Spot in Nucleus Accumbens Shell: Where Do μ-Opioids Cause Increased Hedonic Impact of Sweetness?". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (50): 11777–11786. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2329-05.2005. ISSN   0270-6474. PMC   6726018 . PMID   16354936.
  5. Kelley, A. E.; Bakshi, V. P.; Haber, S. N.; Steininger, T. L.; Will, M. J.; Zhang, M. (2002-07-01). "Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum". Physiology & Behavior. 76 (3): 365–377. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00751-5. ISSN   0031-9384. PMID   12117573. S2CID   44258056.
  6. Peciña, Susana; Berridge, Kent C. (2000-04-28). "Opioid site in nucleus accumbens shell mediates eating and hedonic 'liking' for food: Map based on microinjection Fos plumes". Brain Research. 863 (1–2): 71–86. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02102-8. ISSN   0006-8993. PMID   10773195. S2CID   1805234.
  7. Finlayson, Graham; King, Neil; Blundell, John (2008-01-01). "The role of implicit wanting in relation to explicit liking and wanting for food: implications for appetite control" (PDF). Appetite. 50 (1): 120–127. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.06.007. ISSN   0195-6663. PMID   17655972. S2CID   54347866.
  8. Fontanini, Alfredo; Grossman, Stephen E.; Figueroa, Joshua A.; Katz, Donald B. (2009-02-25). "Distinct subtypes of basolateral amygdala taste neurons reflect palatability and reward". The Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (8): 2486–2495. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3898-08.2009. ISSN   1529-2401. PMC   2668607 . PMID   19244523.
  9. Smith, Gerard P. (2000-10-01). "The controls of eating: a shift from nutritional homeostasis to behavioral neuroscience". Nutrition. 16 (10): 814–820. doi:10.1016/s0899-9007(00)00457-3. ISSN   0899-9007. PMID   11054585.
  10. Smith, Gerard P. (1996-01-01). "The direct and indirect controls of meal size". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 20 (1): 41–46. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00038-g . ISSN   0149-7634. PMID   8622828. S2CID   8313724.
  11. Berridge, Kent C. (1996-01-01). "Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 20 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-b. ISSN   0149-7634. PMID   8622814. S2CID   18707849.
  12. Dalton, M.; Finlayson, G. (2013-01-01), Blundell, John E.; Bellisle, France (eds.), "Hedonics, satiation and satiety", Satiation, Satiety and the Control of Food Intake, Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition, Woodhead Publishing, pp. 221–237, doi:10.1533/9780857098719.4.221, ISBN   978-0-85709-543-5
  13. Yeomans, M. R.; Lee, M. D.; Gray, R. W.; French, S. J. (2001-08-01). "Effects of test-meal palatability on compensatory eating following disguised fat and carbohydrate preloads". International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 25 (8): 1215–1224. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801653. PMID   11477507. S2CID   37835293.
  14. Robinson, Tristan M.; Gray, Richard W.; Yeomans, Martin R.; French, Stephen J. (2005-02-15). "Test-meal palatability alters the effects of intragastric fat but not carbohydrate preloads on intake and rated appetite in healthy volunteers". Physiology & Behavior. 84 (2): 193–203. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.11.004. ISSN   0031-9384. PMID   15708771. S2CID   33711162.
  15. Bowers, M. Deane (1981). "Unpalatability as a Defense Strategy of Western Checkerspot Butterflies (Euphydryas scudder, Nymphalidae)". Evolution. 35 (2): 367–375. doi:10.2307/2407845. ISSN   0014-3820. JSTOR   2407845. PMID   28563381.