Political cognition

Last updated

Political cognition refers to the study of how individuals come to understand the political world, and how this understanding leads to political behavior. Some of the processes studied under the umbrella of political cognition include attention, interpretation, judgment, and memory. Most of the advancements in the area have been made by scholars in the fields of social psychology, political science, and communication studies.

Contents

History

In the early 20th century, the psychological study of cognition encountered significant push back from behaviorism. According to behaviorists, if social psychology was to be considered a serious science, it should study observable and measurable phenomena. [1] Since the processes of the mind are not observable and thus are hard to measure, behaviorist believed that these were not worth studying. However, as Gestalt psychology was introduced to the US by European immigrants, the dominance of the behaviorist approach began to declined. Questions related to perception, judgment, impression formation, and attitude change began to attract more researchers. In the 1950s, the development of new methodological tools ignited the Cognitive Revolution. In 1984, Susan Fiske and Shelley E. Taylor published the first social cognition book, Social Cognition. [2]

Early theories of social cognition

Naïve scientist

First proposed by Fritz Heider in 1958, the Naïve scientist model [3] of cognition conceptualizes individuals as actors with limited information that want to derive an accurate understanding of the world. Much of the work done within this model focused on examining how people perceive and explain why others behave the way they do. This work served as the basis for the development of modern theories of attribution, advanced independently by Harold Kelley and Bernard Weiner. Kelley's attribution theory included the interaction between three variables: consistency, consensus, and distinctiveness. [4] This interaction was summarized in Kelley's Covariation model, also known as Kelley's cube. Consistency refers to whether the person exhibit the behavior across time. The more the person exhibits the behavior across time, the more representative this behavior is of the person. Consensus refers to whether other individuals exhibits the same behavior when presented in the same situation. If many individuals exhibit the same behavior, then the behavior is less informativeness of the person. Distinctiveness refers to whether the person exhibits similar behavior in other situations. The more the person exhibits the behavior in other situation, the less the behavior is representative of the individual. [5]

Cognitive miser

The cognitive miser model argues that, when individuals are attempting to understand the world, they tend to prefer methods that allow them to reduce the amount of cognitive work required to process information. [6] This preference for efficiency leads to the development of biases and heuristics. In the past, political psychologists have identified a wide range of biases and heuristicssuch as partisan heuristics and the Black utility heuristicthat people use to make political decisions. [7]

Motivated reasoning

Motivated reasoning is a cognitive phenomena that occurs when an individual changes a peripheral attitude that is inconsistent with a more central element of the self. [8] The purpose of these cognitive biases is to maintain a positive sense of self-esteem. In the past, they have been referred to as cognitive adaptions and positive illusions. [9] [10] Motivated reasoning has been extensively studied in political psychology. One of the most significant contributions of this area of research is the identification of cases in which voters adopt their preferred candidates' or party' policy positions. [11] [12] [13]

Study of public opinion

In political science, the study of political cognition was facilitated by the emergence of survey research and a growing interest in understanding how individual make voting decisions. [14]

In the 1930s, however, the explosion of commercial polling agencies facilitated the collection of data at the individual level. [15] The availability of this new type of data increased an interest in understanding what individuals know about politics, what attitudes individual have towards political objects, and how individuals make political decisions. In 1940, Paul Lazarsfeld, Bernard Berelson, and Hazel Gaudet carried out one of the earliest studies examining how individual-level factors influence political decisions. [16] The study took place in Erie County, Ohio. Lazarsfeld, Berelson, and Gaudet were interested in identifying what sources of information influence an individual's political attitudes during an electoral campaign. They found that, among those who were less interested in politics, had not decided who to support, or change their voting intentions during the campaign, personal influences—such as the opinion of a friend or a family member—played a more significant role than the media. In 1948, by Bernard Berelson, Paul F. Lazarsfeld, and William N. McPhee carried out a similar study in Elmira, New York. [17]

Both the Erie County and the Elmira studies had been significantly influential to the study of American public opinion. Their findings have been replicated repeatedly and still explain the ways people develop political attitudes today. The biggest influence of these studies, however, was their methodological approach. These two studies were the first studies that followed an individual's political attitudes and voting intentions throughout a campaign with an interview-reinterview approach. [15]

In 1944, the National Opinion Research Center (NOR) at the University of Chicago was the first organization ever to collect panel data at a national level in the United States. In the elections of 1944 and 1948, the Survey Research Center of the University of Michigan performed similar panel-data studies at the national level. These studies were characterized by unstructured questions that allowed participants to express what they knew about politics, as well as what attitudes they had towards political actors and policies. [15]

Major theories of voting behavior (1950s–1980s)

The rational voter

In his 1957 book, An Economic Theory of Democracy , Anthony Downs argues that individuals are rational voters—i.e., they decide who to support by calculating which candidate will maximize the benefits they receive from the government, while minimizing the costs. [18] This rational calculation is performed by taking into consideration the individual's interest, what the party in office has done in the past, and what the party in office and the party out of office could do in the future. [19] :138

Downs defines political parties as coalitions of political elites, whose primary goal is to be elected to office. Because they know voters behave rationally, parties adopt the policy positions of most voters to maximize their chances of being elected into office. The interaction between the rational behavior of voters and the rational behavior of the political elite facilitates the development of a two-party system when voters are normally distributed along the liberal-conservative spectrum. The reason for this is that each party will try to maximize the number of voters it can appeal to while still maintaining significant distinctions from the other party. This results in a party heuristic: voters start to consistently support the party that is closest to their beliefs along this liberal-conservative lines. [18]

Since its publication, the theory of rational voter has encountered numerous empirical challenges, as research suggests that the average voter is not equipped with the necessary information to make rational decisions as defined by Downs. Specifically, most American voters are unable to think in ideological terms—i.e., to articulate their political positions using coherent belief systems. [20] [21] Drawing from social cognition theories, some scholars have argued voters might be still able to make rational decisions even if they are incapable of putting their perceptions, beliefs, and rationales into the formal language of political elites. Specifically, these critics believe that, instead of expecting the average voter to present high levels of political sophistication, political scientists should take into account individual-level variations of information acquisition and processing. They propose that partisan biases motivate individuals to seek out and reject particular sets of information that then lead to candidate evaluations, and then voting. Thus, these critics advance a theory of rational voter that incorporates both cognitive processes and economic utility calculations. [22]

Party attachment

In 1960, Angus Campbell, Philip E. Converse, Warren E. Miller, and Donald E. Stokes published The American Voter. Unlike most prior work, The American Voter was the first book to systematically analyze quantitative data at the national level from three presidential elections (Truman-Dewey in 1948, Eisenhower-Stevenson in 1952, and Eisenhower-Stevenson in 1956). This data was collected by the Survey Research Center of the University of Michigan. The theoretical framework derived from these studies is thus known as Michigan model. [15] The American Voter is also one of the first works to ever look for observable implications of the rational choice theory of voting behavior—a body of work that claims voters are aware of political events, have well-developed political attitudes, and thus are able to aligned their votes with the candidate that is closest to their political dispositions. Lastly, this book was also one of the first works to incorporate a social psychological perspective to the study of politics.

Taken as a system, these [attitudinal] variables were seen to constitute a field of forces operating on the individual as he deliberates over his vote decision. [15] :16

As described by the quote above, the authors of this seminal work that a Lewinian view of the political world. They conceptualize attitudes towards political objects as field forces that led an individual to decide who to support in an election. According to Campbell and colleagues, the most significant of these forces is partisan identification, which the authors defined as a psychological attachment to a party. These psychological attachments are developed early in life and remain stable throughout adulthood. Today, partisan identification is still the strongest and most reliable predictor of vote choice. [23] [24] [25] [26] According to Campbell and colleagues, these partisan attachments function as lenses that paint the way people perceive political information about issues and political actors. Specifically, voters accept and endorse information that is consistent with their partisan beliefs, and reject information that is inconsistent with their partisan views. Additionally, since most voters do not have the time to acquire and process all political information available, they use these partisan attachments as heuristics, or shortcuts, when deciding who to support. [15]

Political information and voter sophistication

Ideology

In his widely known book chapter, "The Nature of Belief Systems", Philip E. Converse examines the nature of abstract political thought among American voters. Converse defined a belief system as a set of idea-elements that were interconnected by logical, psychological, or social constraints. These belief systems could vary in terms of how central certain idea-elements are to the belief system relative to other idea-elements. The centrality of each idea-element influences whether an individual changes her belief based on external changes in the political world. Idea-elements that are peripheral to an individual's belief system are more likely to change than idea-elements that are central to an individual's belief system. [21]

To perform this study, Converse relied on the analysis of open-ended questions. He specifically examines two things. First, he examines what types of political information is associated with what type of political attitudes. Second, he examines whether voters are able to provide an abstract reason to explain this association by referring to the liberal-conservative spectrum. He found that most voters do not think in coherent ideological terms. He divided voters into five types based on their ideological sophistication: Ideologues, Near Ideologues, Group Interest, Nature of the Times, and No issue content. About less than 10% of the voters are considered ideologues or near ideologues. [21] Converse's findings provided further evidence against the theory of rational voting, as voters seem not to be aware of political events, seem to lack well-developed political attitudes, and thus might be unequipped to make rational and informed decisions. [27]

Political attitudes

 In 1992, political scientist John Zaller published his book, The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. In this work, the author examines the processes by which people develop and report their political opinions. According to Zaller, the study of political opinion must be understood through the lens of political awareness and political values, which is summarized in his Receive-Accept-Sample (RAS) model. This model contends that individuals receive political information, decide what to accept and store in memory based on their political values, and when asked to express their opinions about a topic they use a sample of whatever relevant information is accessible in memory to construct their opinions on the spot. [20]

Since most people do not have a direct experience with the political world, they often rely on the political elitewhich includes both politicians and the mediato acquire political information. [28] Zaller argues that voters vary greatly in terms of political awareness either because of a lack of interest in politics or because of a lack of time to pay attention. Consequently, the average voter tends to score low on measures of political knowledge. Zaller observes that this lack of political information is associated with the high level of attitude instability that is exhibited among voters. According to Zaller, this instability is a sign of voters constructing their opinion statements on the spot based on relevant information that happens to be available in memory, rather than the complete in-existence of an attitude (as suggested by Converse) or measurement error. [29] [20]

When voters receive information from the political elite, they almost never receive a complete and neutral account of facts. They receive an oversimplified version of the relevant information that often comes with a political frame, which interacts with the voter's predispositions. [30] [20] If the information is consistent with the voter's prior beliefs, then the information is accepted and stored in memory. If the information is inconsistent with the voter's prior beliefs, then the information is not accepted. [31]

In political science, Zaller's work has been instrumental in the examination of two major types of evaluations: 1) on-line evaluations; and 2) memory-based evaluations. [32] [33] On-line evaluation model asserts that individuals update their evaluations of political objects every time they acquire new information. The memory-based evaluation model asserts that individuals construct their evaluations on the spot based on information available in the working memory. Because most voters fail to recall the content of information they are exposed to during a campaign, many political scientists believed that voters rely on memory-based processes to make political judgments. [33] On the contrary, other scholars believed that voters do update their evaluations of political objects as they acquire new information, but these updates take the form of affective evaluations. [34] [35] Specifically, when voters receive political information—either from a political campaign or from the media—the voter processes that information and turns it into an affective evaluation that is then stored in memory. These stored affective evaluations are then used to make political decisions along with memory-based information. This process is known as the dual processing model. [36]

Priming and agenda setting

Priming is a cognitive process that occurs when a stimulus changes the attitudinal or behavioral response of an individual. This process is facilitated by the activation of information related to the stimulus in the working memory without the individual's awareness. In the study of politics, priming effects have been primarily studied with relation to the media and political campaigns. In 1987, Shanto Iyengar and Donald Kinder published News That Matters: Televised and American Opinion. [30] This work reported the results of a series experiments designed to assess the role of the media on political attitudes. They found that the primarily role of the media is to set the agenda for political evaluations. According to the authors, the media is able to achieve this by priming—or in this case, by making more salient—certain political issues. These salient issues are then used to make political evaluations. Additional work has illustrated that priming only occurs among topics in which the voter has already well-established predispositions. [37]

Persuasion

Most research about political persuasion has taken place within the context of campaign effect. Early work finds that campaigns that use various forms of personal contact (e.g., canvassing and telephone calls) to deliver information are more effective than campaigns that use non-personal contact (e.g., mailing information) at mobilizing voters. [38] Contemporary research suggests that whenever persuasion—defined as an attitude change—is achieved, its effects are relatively small and fade away rapidly. [33] Additionally, well-developed political predisposition are not easily persuadable, while less developed attitudes move around fairly easily. [37]

Social identities and inter-group relations

Linked fate: black utility heuristic

In his 1994 book, Behind the Mule: Race and Class in African-American Politics, Michael Dawson argues African American voters use evaluations of their group-level interests as short-cuts to determine the policy positions, vote choice, and political engagement that would safeguard their individual-level interest. [39] According to Dawson, this political heuristic was developed as a consequence of the historical oppression of African Americans, which facilitated the development of the belief that individual interest was linked to the racial group interest among African Americans. Consequently, this black utility heuristic is known as linked fate. Dawson argues that racial issues override class-based differences which results in the political homogeneity of African Americans. Additional work suggests that other groups—including Whites, Asian Americans, Latinxs, and women—also exhibit linked fate. [40] [41] [42]

Other researchers have advocated for the revision of the current linked fate measure, as it seems to be inconsistently associated with group identification and with political engagement. [43]

See also

Related Research Articles

Social psychology is the scientific study of how thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by the actual, imagined, or implied presence of others. Social psychologists typically explain human behavior as a result of the relationship between mental states and social situations, studying the social conditions under which thoughts, feelings, and behaviors occur, and how these variables influence social interactions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive bias</span> Systematic pattern of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment

A cognitive bias is a systematic pattern of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment. Individuals create their own "subjective reality" from their perception of the input. An individual's construction of reality, not the objective input, may dictate their behavior in the world. Thus, cognitive biases may sometimes lead to perceptual distortion, inaccurate judgment, illogical interpretation, and irrationality.

In the field of psychology, cognitive dissonance is the perception of contradictory information and the mental toll of it. Relevant items of information include a person's actions, feelings, ideas, beliefs, values, and things in the environment. Cognitive dissonance is typically experienced as psychological stress when persons participate in an action that goes against one or more of those things. According to this theory, when two actions or ideas are not psychologically consistent with each other, people do all in their power to change them until they become consistent. The discomfort is triggered by the person's belief clashing with new information perceived, wherein the individual tries to find a way to resolve the contradiction to reduce their discomfort.

The bandwagon effect is the tendency for people to adopt certain behaviors, styles, or attitudes simply because others are doing so. More specifically, it is a cognitive bias by which public opinion or behaviours can alter due to particular actions and beliefs rallying amongst the public. It is a psychological phenomenon whereby the rate of uptake of beliefs, ideas, fads and trends increases with respect to the proportion of others who have already done so. As more people come to believe in something, others also "hop on the bandwagon" regardless of the underlying evidence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Attitude (psychology)</span> Concept linking cognitive processes to behavior

An attitude "is a summary evaluation of an object of thought. An attitude object can be anything a person discriminates or holds in mind." Attitudes include beliefs (cognition), emotional responses (affect) and behavioral tendencies. In the classical definition an attitude is persistent, while in more contemporary conceptualizations, attitudes may vary depending upon situations, context, or moods.

Social cognition is a topic within psychology that focuses on how people process, store, and apply information about other people and social situations. It focuses on the role that cognitive processes play in social interactions.

In psychology, the false consensus effect, also known as consensus bias, is a pervasive cognitive bias that causes people to "see their own behavioral choices and judgments as relatively common and appropriate to existing circumstances". In other words, they assume that their personal qualities, characteristics, beliefs, and actions are relatively widespread through the general population.

Political psychology is an interdisciplinary academic field, dedicated to understanding politics, politicians and political behavior from a psychological perspective, and psychological processes using socio-political perspectives. The relationship between politics and psychology is considered bidirectional, with psychology being used as a lens for understanding politics and politics being used as a lens for understanding psychology. As an interdisciplinary field, political psychology borrows from a wide range of disciplines, including: anthropology, economics, history, international relations, journalism, media, philosophy, political science, psychology, and sociology.

Metacognition is an awareness of one's thought processes and an understanding of the patterns behind them. The term comes from the root word meta, meaning "beyond", or "on top of". Metacognition can take many forms, such as reflecting on one's ways of thinking and knowing when and how to use particular strategies for problem-solving. There are generally two components of metacognition: (1) knowledge about cognition and (2) regulation of cognition. A metacognitive model differs from other scientific models in that the creator of the model is per definition also enclosed within it. Scientific models are often prone to distancing the observer from the object or field of study whereas a metacognitive model in general tries to include the observer in the model.

Attitudes are associated beliefs and behaviors towards some object. They are not stable, and because of the communication and behavior of other people, are subject to change by social influences, as well as by the individual's motivation to maintain cognitive consistency when cognitive dissonance occurs—when two attitudes or attitude and behavior conflict. Attitudes and attitude objects are functions of affective and cognitive components. It has been suggested that the inter-structural composition of an associative network can be altered by the activation of a single node. Thus, by activating an affective or emotional node, attitude change may be possible, though affective and cognitive components tend to be intertwined.

In media studies, mass communication, media psychology, communication theory, and sociology, media influence and themedia effect are topics relating to mass media and media culture's effects on individuals' or audiences' thoughts, attitudes, and behaviors. Through written, televised, or spoken channels, mass media reach large audiences. Mass media's role in shaping modern culture is a central issue for the study of culture.

Salience is the state or condition of being prominent. The Oxford English Dictionary defines salience as "most noticeable or important." The concept is discussed in communication, semiotics, linguistics, sociology, psychology, and political science. It has been studied with respect to interpersonal communication, persuasion, politics, and its influence on mass media.

The elaboration likelihood model (ELM) of persuasion is a dual process theory describing the change of attitudes. The ELM was developed by Richard E. Petty and John Cacioppo in 1980. The model aims to explain different ways of processing stimuli, why they are used, and their outcomes on attitude change. The ELM proposes two major routes to persuasion: the central route and the peripheral route.

In psychology, a dual process theory provides an account of how thought can arise in two different ways, or as a result of two different processes. Often, the two processes consist of an implicit (automatic), unconscious process and an explicit (controlled), conscious process. Verbalized explicit processes or attitudes and actions may change with persuasion or education; though implicit process or attitudes usually take a long amount of time to change with the forming of new habits. Dual process theories can be found in social, personality, cognitive, and clinical psychology. It has also been linked with economics via prospect theory and behavioral economics, and increasingly in sociology through cultural analysis.

The priming theory states that media images stimulate related thoughts in the minds of audience members.

Selective exposure is a theory within the practice of psychology, often used in media and communication research, that historically refers to individuals' tendency to favorite information which reinforces their pre-existing views while avoiding contradictory information. Selective exposure has also been known and defined as "congeniality bias" or "confirmation bias" in various texts throughout the years.

In psychology, the human mind is considered to be a cognitive miser due to the tendency of humans to think and solve problems in simpler and less effortful ways rather than in more sophisticated and effortful ways, regardless of intelligence. Just as a miser seeks to avoid spending money, the human mind often seeks to avoid spending cognitive effort. The cognitive miser theory is an umbrella theory of cognition that brings together previous research on heuristics and attributional biases to explain when and why people are cognitive misers.

Implicit attitudes are evaluations that occur without conscious awareness towards an attitude object or the self. These evaluations are generally either favorable or unfavorable and come about from various influences in the individual experience. The commonly used definition of implicit attitude within cognitive and social psychology comes from Anthony Greenwald and Mahzarin Banaji's template for definitions of terms related to implicit cognition: "Implicit attitudes are introspectively unidentified traces of past experience that mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling, thought, or action toward social objects". These thoughts, feelings or actions have an influence on behavior that the individual may not be aware of.

Motivated reasoning is a cognitive and social response in which individuals, consciously or unconsciously, allow emotion-loaded motivational biases to affect how new information is perceived. Individuals tend to favor evidence that coincides with their current beliefs and reject new information that contradicts them, despite contrary evidence.

References

  1. Hamilton, David E.; Carlston, Donal (2013-08-22). "The Emergence of Social Cognition". The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199730018.013.0002.
  2. Fiske, Susan T.; Taylor, Shelley E. (1984). Social Cognition. Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.
  3. Heider, Fritz (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.
  4. Kelley, Harold (1973). "The processes of causal attribution". American Psychologist. 28 (2): 107–128. doi:10.1037/h0034225.
  5. Hesketh, Beryl (1984). "Attribution theory and unemployment: Kelley's covariation model, self-esteem, and locus of control". Journal of Vocational Behavior. 24: 94–109. doi:10.1016/0001-8791(84)90069-1.
  6. Tversky, Amos (1973). "Availability: A Heuristic for Judging Frequency and Probability". Cognitive Psychology. 5 (2): 207–232. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9. S2CID   41668623.
  7. Johnson, Theodore R. (7 September 2015). "Can the Democratic Party Retain Its Hold on Black Voters?". The Atlantic. Retrieved 3 January 2019.
  8. Kunda, Ziva (1990). "The Case for Motivated Reasoning". Psychological Bulletin. 108 (3): 480–498. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480. PMID   2270237. S2CID   9703661.
  9. Taylor, Shelley E. (1983). "Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation". American Psychologist. 38 (11): 1161–1173. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1161.
  10. Taylor, Shelley E.; Brown, Jonathon D. (1988). "Illusion and well-being: perspective on mental health". Psychological Bulletin. 103 (2): 193–210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193. PMID   3283814. S2CID   762759.
  11. Tesler, Michael (2015). "Priming Predispositions and Changing Policy Positions: An Account of When Mass Opinion is Primed or Changed". American Journal of Political Science. 4. 59 (4): 806–824. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12157 .
  12. Nyhan, Brendan (2010). "When Correction Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions". Political Behavior. 32 (2): 303–330. doi:10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2. S2CID   10715114.
  13. Lenz, Gabriel (2009). "Learning and Opinion Change, not Priming: Reconsidering the Priming Hypothesis". American Journal of Political Science. 53 (4): 821–837. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2009.00403.x.
  14. Huddy, Leonie; Sears, David O.; Levy, Jack S. (2013). The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology. Oxford. p. 5.
  15. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Cambell, Angus; Converse, Philip E.; Miller, Warren E.; Stokes, Donald E. (1960). The American Voter. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  16. Lazarsfeld, Paul F.; Berelson, Bernard; Gaudet, Hazel (1944). The People's Choice: How the Voter Makes Up His Mind in a Presidential Campaign. Columbia University Press.
  17. Berelson, Bernard R.; Lazarsfeld, Paul F.; McPhee, William (1954). Voting: A Study of Opinion Formation in a Presidential Campaign. University of Chicago Press.
  18. 1 2 Downs, Anthony (1957). An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper.
  19. Downs, Anthony (1957). "An Economic Theory of Political Action in a Democracy". Journal of Political Economy. 65 (2): 135–150. doi:10.1086/257897. S2CID   154363730.
  20. 1 2 3 4 Zaller, John (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge University Press.
  21. 1 2 3 Converse, Philip E. (2006). "The nature of belief systems in mass publics (1964)". Critical Review. 18 (1–3): 1–74. doi: 10.1080/08913810608443650 . S2CID   140857433.
  22. Shapiro, Michael J. (1969). "Rational Political Man: A Synthesis of Economic and Social-Psychological Perspectives". American Political Science Review. 63 (4): 1106–1119. doi:10.1017/S0003055400263223.
  23. Miller, Warren E. (1991). "Party Identification, Realignment, and Party Voting: Back to the Basics". The American Political Science Review. 85 (2): 557–568. doi:10.2307/1963175. JSTOR   1963175. S2CID   15054585.
  24. Lascher, Edward L.; Korey, John L. (2011). "The Myth of the Independent Voter, California Style". California Journal of Politics and Policy. 3: 1–20. doi: 10.5070/P20W2R .
  25. Bartels, Larry M. (2000). "Partisanship and Voting Behavior, 1952-1996". American Journal of Political Science. 44 (1): 35–50. doi:10.2307/2669291. JSTOR   2669291. S2CID   153746527.
  26. Abramowitz, Alan I.; Webster, Steven (2016). "The rise of negative partisanship and the nationalization of U.S. Elections in the 21st century". Electoral Studies. 41: 12–22. doi:10.1016/j.electstud.2015.11.001.
  27. Menand, Louis (30 August 2004). "The Unpolitical Animal". The New Yorker. Retrieved 30 December 2019.
  28. Markus, Gregory B. (1994). "John R. Zaller. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992. 367 pp". Public Opinion Quarterly. 58 (4): 633. doi:10.1086/269451.
  29. Taylor, Jerry (23 February 2015). "How to Change Public Opinion". Niskanen Center. Retrieved 30 December 2019.
  30. 1 2 Iyengar, Shanto; Kinder, Donald (1987). News That Matters: Television and American Opinion. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  31. Zaller, John (2012). "What Nature and Origins Leaves Out" (PDF). Critical Review. 24 (4): 569–642. doi:10.1080/08913811.2012.807648. S2CID   144980145.
  32. "The Psychology of Voting and Election Campaigns: About the Psychology of Voting". ANES | American National Election Studies. Retrieved 2019-11-25.
  33. 1 2 3 Hill, Seth J.; Lo, James; Vavreck, Lynn; Zaller, John (2013). "How Quickly We Forget: The Duration of Persuasion Effects from Mass Communication". Political Communication. 30 (4): 521–547. doi:10.1080/10584609.2013.828143. S2CID   143732717.
  34. Lodge, Milton; Taber, Charles; Weber, Christopher (2006). "First Steps Toward a Dual-Process Accessibility Model of Political Beliefs, Attitudes, and Behavior". Feeling Politics. pp. 11–30. doi:10.1057/9781403983114_2. ISBN   978-1-349-53320-6.
  35. Lodge, Milton; Steenbergen, Marco R.; Brau, Shawn (1995). "The Responsive Voter: Campaign Information and the Dynamics of Candidate Evaluation". The American Political Science Review. 89 (2): 309–326. doi:10.2307/2082427. JSTOR   2082427. S2CID   37154768.
  36. Taber, Charles S. (2011). "Political Cognition and Public Opinion". The Oxford Handbook of American Public Opinion and the Media. pp. 368–383. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199545636.003.0023. ISBN   978-0199545636.
  37. 1 2 Tesler, Michael (2015). "Priming Predispositions and Changing Policy Positions: An Account of When Opinion is Primed or Changed". American Journal of Political Science. 59 (4): 806–824. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12157 .
  38. Eldersveld, Samuel J. (1956). "Experimental Propaganda Techniques and Voting Behavior". The American Political Science Review. 50 (1): 154–165. doi:10.2307/1951603. JSTOR   1951603. S2CID   145299563.
  39. Dawson, Michael (1994). Behind the Mule: Race and Class in African-American Politics. University of Chicago Press.
  40. Gay, Claudine; Tate, Katherine (1998). "Doubly Bound: The Impact of Gender and Race on the Politics of Black Women". Political Psychology. 19 (1): 169–184. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00098. JSTOR   3792120.
  41. Simien, Evelyn M. (2005). "Race, Gender, and Linked Fate". Journal of Black Studies. 35 (5): 529–550. doi:10.1177/0021934704265899. S2CID   144094338.
  42. Sanchez, Gabriel R.; Masuoka, Natalie (2010). "Brown-Utility Heuristic? The Presence and Contributing Factors of Latino Linked Fate". Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 32 (4): 519–531. doi:10.1177/0739986310383129. S2CID   144615737.
  43. Gay, Claudine; Hochschild, Jennifer; White, Ariel (2016). "Americans' Belief in Linked Fate: Does the Measure Capture the Concept?". Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics. 1 (1): 117–144. doi:10.1017/rep.2015.3. S2CID   145039190.