Processing fluency

Last updated

Processing fluency is the ease with which information is processed. Perceptual fluency is the ease of processing stimuli based on manipulations to perceptual quality. Retrieval fluency is the ease with which information can be retrieved from memory. [1]

Contents

Research

Research in cognitive neuroscience and psychology has shown that processing fluency influences different kinds of judgments. For instance, perceptual fluency can contribute to the experience of familiarity when fluent processing is attributed to the past. Repeating the presentation of a stimulus, also known as priming, is one method for enhancing fluency. Jacoby and Dallas in 1981 argued that items from past experience are processed more fluently. [2] This becomes a learned experience throughout our lifetime such that fluent items can be attributed to the past. Therefore, people sometimes take fluency as an indication that a stimulus is familiar even though the sense of familiarity is false. [3] Perceptual fluency literature has been dominated with research that posits that fluency leads to familiarity. Behavioral measures of fluency do not have the temporal resolution to properly investigate the interaction between fluency and familiarity. Event-related potentials (ERPs) are a method of averaging brainwaves that has been successful in dissociating different cognitive mechanisms due to small time scale that brainwaves are measured. [4] One study was able to use a manipulation of visual clarity to change perceptual fluency during a recognition task. This manipulation effected ERPs for fluency and familiarity at different times and locations in the brain, leading them to believe that these two mechanisms do not come from the same source. [5]

Further evidence has shown that artificial techniques can be used to trick people into believing they have encountered a stimulus previously. In one experiment, [6] participants were presented symbols which consisted of highly familiar symbols, less familiar symbols and novel symbols. Participants were required to report whether they had encountered any of the symbols presented before the experiment. A 35 millisecond flash preceded each symbol, in which the same, different or no symbol was flashed. It was found that the brief flash of stimulus boosted the fluency of the target item. When the same symbol was flashed, participants’ ratings of having encountered the symbol previously increased. This example illustrates that fluent processing can induce a feeling of familiarity.

Fluency and familiarity have been shown to lead to the mere exposure effect. Research has found that repetition of a stimulus can lead to fluent processing which leads to a feeling of liking. [7] In this experiment, participants were presented with unfamiliar faces either three or nine times. After presentation, pairs of faces were shown to participants, each consisting of an old and new face. Results showed that participants gave higher rates of liking to the repeated faces. The mere exposure effect is eliminated if fluent processing is disrupted. Topolinski et al. (2014) [8] explained that the fluency created by pronunciation plays a vital role in increasing fondness for a stimulus. In the experiment, [8] participants were shown unfamiliar adverts in a cinema. The control group watched the adverts with a sugar cube in their mouth. Therefore, there was no interference in pronunciation in the control group. The experimental group watched the adverts whilst eating popcorn, which meant there was oral interference. One week later, participants rated their liking of products. In the control group, the mere exposure effect was observed, where old products had higher ratings than new products. In the experimental group, the mere exposure effect was abolished by interfering with fluency of pronunciation during presentation of stimulus.

Later research observed that high perceptual fluency increases the experience of positive affect. [9] Research with psychophysiological methods corroborated this positive effect on affective experience: easy-to-perceive stimuli were not only judged more positively but increased activation in the zygomaticus major muscle, the so-called "smiling muscle". [10] The notion that processing fluency is inherently positive led to the processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure, [11] and it has been used to explain people's negative reactions towards migrants, who appear to be more difficult to process than non-migrants. [12]

Research relating to processing fluency and product design has shown that when the form of a product is highly unusual, it becomes difficult to process and is viewed less favourably than fluent counterparts. [13] There is significant evidence that when consumers are presented with multiple choices, they will view objects more positively and more aesthetically pleasing when surrounded by congruent imagery. [14] While consumers enjoy a moderate source of incongruity, too much disorder and unfamiliarity lead to feelings of being overwhelmed. [13] Fluent product design has shown to encourage consumers to engage in approach activities such as touching and spending extended time viewing the product. [13]

Other studies have shown that when presenting people with a factual statement, manipulations that make the statement easier to mentally process—even totally nonsubstantive changes like writing it in a cleaner font or making it rhyme or simply repeating it—can alter judgment of the truth of the statement, along with evaluation of the intelligence of the statement's author. [15] This is called the "illusion of truth effect". Multiple studies have found that subjects were more likely to judge easy-to-read statements as true. [16] [17] [18] This means that perceived beauty and judged truth have a common underlying experience, namely processing fluency. Indeed, experiments showed that beauty is used as an indication for the correctness of mathematical solutions. This supports the idea that beauty is intuitively seen as truth. [19] Processing fluency may be one of the foundations of intuition [20] and the "Aha!" experience. [21] [22]

The truth effect can be induced by colour differences in statements as well. In a study, [23] participants were presented with statements, some of which were true and the rest being false. Half these statements were presented in high contrast colours and the other half were presented in low contrast colours. Independent of truth, participants judged the high contrast statements as true. Participants were also quicker to make judgements about the high contrast statements compared to the low contrast statements. The contrast differences in statements increased fluency.

Fluency has been shown to affect judgements of humour. In one study, [24] participants were presented with jokes which were in easy to read or hard to read fonts. Participants were asked to rate which jokes they believed were more humorous. Participants gave higher ratings to jokes in easy to read fonts. It has been predicted that the jokes in easy to read fonts feel fluent, as they are easier to pronounce and this results in higher ratings. [24] The fluent processing has been misattributed to the humour of the statement.

As high processing fluency indicates that the interaction of a person with the environment goes smoothly, [25] a person does not need to pay particular attention to the environment. By contrast, low processing fluency means that there are problems in the interaction with the environment which requires more attention and an analytical processing style to solve the problem. Indeed, people process information more shallowly when processing fluency is high and employ an analytical thinking style when processing fluency is low. [26] [27]

A 2010 study demonstrated that the long-known effect of illegible handwriting in an essay on grading is mediated by a lack of processing fluency (and not, for example, negative stereotypes related to illegible writing). [28]

Applications

Basic research on processing fluency has been applied to marketing, [29] to business names, and to finance. For example, psychologists have determined that, during the week following their IPO, stocks perform better when their names are fluent/easy to pronounce and when their ticker symbols are pronounceable (e.g., KAG) vs. unpronounceable (e.g., KGH). [30]

See also

Related Research Articles

The mere-exposure effect is a psychological phenomenon by which people tend to develop a preference for things merely because they are familiar with them. In social psychology, this effect is sometimes called the familiarity principle. The effect has been demonstrated with many kinds of things, including words, Chinese characters, paintings, pictures of faces, geometric figures, and sounds. In studies of interpersonal attraction, the more often someone sees a person, the more pleasing and likeable they find that person.

The recognition heuristic, originally termed the recognition principle, has been used as a model in the psychology of judgment and decision making and as a heuristic in artificial intelligence. The goal is to make inferences about a criterion that is not directly accessible to the decision maker, based on recognition retrieved from memory. This is possible if recognition of alternatives has relevance to the criterion. For two alternatives, the heuristic is defined as:

If one of two objects is recognized and the other is not, then infer that the recognized object has the higher value with respect to the criterion.

Picture superiority effect

The picture superiority effect refers to the phenomenon in which pictures and images are more likely to be remembered than are words. This effect has been demonstrated in numerous experiments using different methods. It is based on the notion that "human memory is extremely sensitive to the symbolic modality of presentation of event information". Explanations for the picture superiority effect are not concrete and are still being debated.

Inattentional blindness or perceptual blindness occurs when an individual fails to perceive an unexpected stimulus in plain sight, purely as a result of a lack of attention rather than any vision defects or deficits. When it becomes impossible to attend to all the stimuli in a given situation, a temporary “blindness” effect can occur, as individuals fail to see unexpected but often salient objects or stimuli.

The Levels of Processing model, created by Fergus I. M. Craik and Robert S. Lockhart in 1972, describes memory recall of stimuli as a function of the depth of mental processing. Deeper levels of analysis produce more elaborate, longer-lasting, and stronger memory traces than shallow levels of analysis. Depth of processing falls on a shallow to deep continuum. Shallow processing leads to a fragile memory trace that is susceptible to rapid decay. Conversely, deep processing results in a more durable memory trace.

Artificial grammar learning (AGL) is a paradigm of study within cognitive psychology and linguistics. Its goal is to investigate the processes that underlie human language learning by testing subjects' ability to learn a made-up grammar in a laboratory setting. It was developed to evaluate the processes of human language learning but has also been utilized to study implicit learning in a more general sense. The area of interest is typically the subjects' ability to detect patterns and statistical regularities during a training phase and then use their new knowledge of those patterns in a testing phase. The testing phase can either use the symbols or sounds used in the training phase or transfer the patterns to another set of symbols or sounds as surface structure.

In psychology, a fluency heuristic is a mental heuristic in which, if one object is processed more fluently, faster, or more smoothly than another, the mind infers that this object has the higher value with respect to the question being considered. In other words, the more skillfully or elegantly an idea is communicated, the more likely it is to be considered seriously, whether or not it is logical.

The cross-race effect is the tendency to more easily recognize faces that belong to one's own racial group. In social psychology, the cross-race effect is described as the "ingroup advantage," whereas in other fields, the effect can be seen as a specific form of the "ingroup advantage" since it is only applied in interracial or inter-ethnic situations. The cross-race effect is thought to contribute to difficulties in cross-race identification, as well as implicit racial bias.

Norbert Schwarz is Provost Professor in the Department of Psychology and the USC Marshall School of Business at the University of Southern California and a co-director of the USC Dornsife Mind and Society Center.

Priming is a phenomenon whereby exposure to one stimulus influences a response to a subsequent stimulus, without conscious guidance or intention. For example, the word nurse is recognized more quickly following the word doctor than following the word bread. Priming can be perceptual, associative, repetitive, positive, negative, affective, semantic, or conceptual. Research, however, has yet to firmly establish the duration of priming effects, yet their onset can be almost instantaneous.

There is evidence suggesting that different processes are involved in remembering something versus knowing whether it is familiar. It appears that "remembering" and "knowing" represent relatively different characteristics of memory as well as reflect different ways of using memory.

Paula M. Niedenthal is a social psychologist currently working as a Professor of Psychology at the University of Wisconsin–Madison. She also completed her undergraduate studies at the University of Wisconsin at Madison where she received a Bachelor's in Psychology. She then received her Ph.D. at the University of Michigan before becoming a faculty member of the departments of Psychology at Johns Hopkins University and Indiana University. Until recently, she served as the Director of Research in the National Centre for Scientific Research at the Université Blaise Pascal in Clermont-Ferrand France. The majority of Niedenthal's research focuses on several levels of analysis of emotional processes, this would include emotion-cognition interaction and representational models of emotion. Niedenthal has authored more than 80 articles and chapters, and several books. Niedenthal is a fellow of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology.

Perceptual learning

Perceptual learning is learning better perception skills such as differentiating two musical tones from one another or categorizations of spatial and temporal patterns relevant to real-world expertise. Examples of this may include reading, seeing relations among chess pieces, and knowing whether or not an X-ray image shows a tumor.

The processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure is a theory in psychological aesthetics on how people experience beauty. Processing fluency is the ease with which information is processed in the human mind.

Rolf Reber

Rolf Reber is professor of psychology at the University of Oslo.

Assimilation and contrast effects

The assimilation effect, assimilation bias or biased assimilation is a bias in evaluative judgments towards the position of a context stimulus, while contrast effects describe a negative correlation between a judgment and contextual information.

Subliminal stimuli are any sensory stimuli below an individual's threshold for conscious perception, in contrast to supraliminal stimuli. A 2012 review of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies showed that subliminal stimuli activate specific regions of the brain despite participants' unawareness. Visual stimuli may be quickly flashed before an individual can process them, or flashed and then masked to interrupt processing. Audio stimuli may be played below audible volumes or masked by other stimuli.

Unconscious cognition is the processing of perception, memory, learning, thought, and language without being aware of it.

The illusory truth effect is the tendency to believe false information to be correct after repeated exposure. This phenomenon was first identified in a 1977 study at Villanova University and Temple University. When truth is assessed, people rely on whether the information is in line with their understanding or if it feels familiar. The first condition is logical, as people compare new information with what they already know to be true. Repetition makes statements easier to process relative to new, unrepeated statements, leading people to believe that the repeated conclusion is more truthful. The illusory truth effect has also been linked to hindsight bias, in which the recollection of confidence is skewed after the truth has been received.

The word frequency effect is a psychological phenomenon where recognition times are faster for words seen more frequently than for words seen less frequently. Word frequency depends on individual awareness of the tested language. The phenomenon can be extended to different characters of the word in non-alphabetic languages such as Chinese.

References

  1. Alter, A. L.; Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). "Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation" (PDF). Personality and Social Psychology Review. 13 (3): 219–235. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.586.5634 . doi:10.1177/1088868309341564. PMID   19638628. S2CID   2696881. Archived from the original on 2016-04-26.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  2. Jacoby, Larry L.; Dallas, Mark (1981). "On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 110 (3): 306–340. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.3.306.
  3. Whittlesea, Bruce W. A. (1993). "Illusions of familiarity". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 19 (6): 1235–1253. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1235.
  4. Rugg, Michael D.; Curran, Tim (2007). "Event-related potentials and recognition memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (6): 251–257. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.004. PMID   17481940. S2CID   1249891.
  5. Leynes, P. Andrew; Zish, Kevin (2012). "Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for fluency-based recognition memory". Neuropsychologia. 50 (14): 3240–3249. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.004. PMID   23063967. S2CID   24815127.
  6. Brown, Alan S.; Marsh, Elizabeth J. (2009-05-01). "Creating Illusions of Past Encounter Through Brief Exposure". Psychological Science. 20 (5): 534–538. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02337.x. PMID   19492436. S2CID   16445449.
  7. Newell, Ben R.; Shanks, David R. (2007-01-01). "Recognising what you like: Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recognition". European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 19 (1): 103–118. doi:10.1080/09541440500487454. ISSN   0954-1446. S2CID   16327127.
  8. 1 2 Topolinski, Sascha; Lindner, Sandy; Freudenberg, Anna (2014-04-01). "Popcorn in the cinema: Oral interference sabotages advertising effects". Journal of Consumer Psychology. Sensory perception, embodiment, and grounded cognition: Implications for consumer behavior. 24 (2): 169–176. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.008.
  9. Reber, R.; Winkielman, P.; Schwarz, N. (1998). "Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective Judgments". Psychological Science. 9: 45–48. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.232.8868 . doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00008. S2CID   238063.
  10. Winkielman, Piotr; Cacioppo, John T. (2001). "Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (6): 989–1000. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989. PMID   11761320.
  11. Reber, Rolf; Schwarz, Norbert; Winkielman, Piotr (2004). "Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience?". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 8 (4): 364–382. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3. hdl: 1956/594 . PMID   15582859. S2CID   1868463.
  12. Rubin, Mark; Paolini, Stefania; Crisp, Richard J. (2010). "A processing fluency explanation of bias against migrants" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46: 21–28. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.006. hdl: 1959.13/930247 . Archived from the original on 2016-06-13.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  13. 1 2 3 Bloch, Peter H. (1995). "Seeking the Ideal Form: Product Design and Consumer Response". Journal of Marketing. 59 (3): 16–29. doi:10.2307/1252116. ISSN   0022-2429. JSTOR   1252116.
  14. Reber, Rolf; Schwarz, Norbert (1999-09-01). "Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Judgments of Truth". Consciousness and Cognition. 8 (3): 338–342. doi:10.1006/ccog.1999.0386. ISSN   1053-8100. PMID   10487787. S2CID   2626302.
  15. Bennett, Drake (January 31, 2010). "Easy=True: How "cognitive fluency" shapes what we believe, how we invest, and who will become a supermodel". The Boston Globe. Archived from the original on June 13, 2016.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  16. Reber, Rolf; Schwarz, Norbert (1999). "Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Judgments of Truth". Consciousness and Cognition. 8 (3): 338–342. doi:10.1006/ccog.1999.0386. PMID   10487787. S2CID   2626302.
  17. Newman, Eryn J.; Garry, Maryanne; Bernstein, Daniel M.; Kantner, Justin; Lindsay, D. Stephen (2012-10-01). "Nonprobative photographs (or words) inflate truthiness". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 19 (5): 969–974. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0292-0. ISSN   1531-5320. PMID   22869334. S2CID   8677269.
  18. Waldman, Katy (2014-09-03). "The Science of Truthiness". Slate. ISSN   1091-2339 . Retrieved 2016-10-03.
  19. Reber, Rolf; Brun, Morten; Mitterndorfer, Karoline (2008). "The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment" (PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 15 (6): 1174–1178. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1174. hdl: 1956/2734 . PMID   19001586. S2CID   5297500.
  20. Topolinski, Sascha; Strack, Fritz (2009). "The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence and judgments of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 138 (1): 39–63. doi:10.1037/a0014678. PMID   19203169. Archived from the original on 2016-06-13.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  21. Topolinski, S.; Reber, R. (2010). "Gaining Insight into the "Aha" Experience" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 19 (6): 402–405. doi:10.1177/0963721410388803. S2CID   145057045. Archived from the original on 2016-06-13.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  22. Wray, H. (January 2011). "Aha! The 23-Across Phenomenon". APS Observer. Vol. 24, no. 1. Association for Psychological Science. Archived from the original on 2016-03-04.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  23. Unkelbach, Christian (2007-01-01). "Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpretation of processing fluency in judgments of truth". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33 (1): 219–230. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.219. ISSN   1939-1285. PMID   17201563.
  24. 1 2 Topolinski, Sascha (2014-07-04). "A processing fluency-account of funniness: Running gags and spoiling punchlines". Cognition and Emotion. 28 (5): 811–820. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.863180. ISSN   0269-9931. PMID   24320137. S2CID   25039892.
  25. Winkielman, P.; Schwarz, N.; Reber, R.; Fazendeiro, T. (2003). "The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment" (PDF). In Musch, Jochen; Klauer, Karl C. (eds.). The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. pp. 189–217. ISBN   9781135640590. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-02-27.
  26. Alter, Adam L.; Oppenheimer, Daniel M.; Epley, Nicholas; Eyre, Rebecca N. (2007). "Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 136 (4): 569–576. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569. PMID   17999571. Archived from the original on 2016-06-02.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  27. Song, Hyunjin; Schwarz, Norbert (2008). "Fluency and the Detection of Misleading Questions: Low Processing Fluency Attenuates the Moses Illusion". Social Cognition. 26 (6): 791–799. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.6.791.
  28. Greifeneder, R.; Alt, A.; Bottenberg, K.; Seele, T.; Zelt, S.; Wagener, D. (2010). "On Writing Legibly: Processing Fluency Systematically Biases Evaluations of Handwritten Material". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 1 (3): 230–237. doi:10.1177/1948550610368434. S2CID   57062084.
  29. Schwarz, Norbert (2004). "Metacognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision Making". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 14 (4): 332–348. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_2. hdl: 2027.42/142285 . SSRN   532222.
  30. Alter, A.L.; Oppenheimer, D.M. (2006). "Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency" (PDF). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (24): 9369–9372. Bibcode:2006PNAS..103.9369A. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601071103 . PMC   1482615 . PMID   16754871. Open Access logo PLoS transparent.svg

Further reading