Prospection

Last updated

In psychology, prospection is the generation and evaluation of mental representations of possible futures. The term therefore captures a wide array of future-oriented psychological phenomena, including the prediction of future emotion (affective forecasting), the imagination of future scenarios (episodic foresight), and planning. Prospection is central to various aspects of human cognition and motivation. Daniel Gilbert (psychologist) and Timothy Wilson coined the term in 2007. It has since become a central area of enquiry in the cognitive sciences. [1] [2] [3]

Contents

Prospection and learning

Even fundamental learning processes are, in some sense, forms of prospection. [4] [5] Associative learning enables individual animals to track local regularities in their environments and adapt their behaviour accordingly, in order to maximise their chances of positive outcomes and minimise risks. Animals that are capable of positive and negative states (for example pleasure and pain) can eventually learn about the consequences of their actions and thereby predict imminent rewards and punishments before they occur. This enables animals to change their current actions accordingly in line with prospective consequences.

Relationship with mental time travel

Mental time travel refers to the ability to mentally reconstruct personal events from the past (known as episodic memory), as well as to imagine personal future events (known as episodic foresight). Mental time travel into the future (episodic foresight or episodic future thinking) is therefore one of several types of 'prospection' that refers to the capacity to simulate or imagine personal future events. [6] [7]

Examples of the functions of prospection

Episodic foresight

Episodic foresight is the capacity to imagine personal future scenarios and shape current action accordingly. [8]

Predicting future emotions (affective forecasting)

The feelings evoked during episodic foresight enable people to infer how they would really feel if the event were to happen in reality. This thereby enables people to anticipate whether future events are desirable or undesirable, and ability called 'affective forecasting'. [9]

Prospective intentions

Simulating the future enables people to create intentions for future actions. Prospective memory is the form of memory that involves remembering to perform these planned intentions, or to recall them at some future point in time. [10] Prospective memory tasks are common in everyday life, ranging from remembering to post a letter to remembering to take one's medication.

Deliberate practice

People anticipate that it is possible to shape their future self. To acquire new knowledge or additional skills, people therefore engage in repeated actions driven by the goal to improve these future capacities. This deliberate practice is essential not only for elite performance but also in the acquirement of numerous everyday feats.

Flexible decision-making

Intertemporal choices are choices with outcomes that play out over time. [11] Such decisions are ubiquitous in everyday life, ranging from routine decisions about what to eat for lunch (i.e. whether to adhere to a diet) to more profound decisions about climate change (i.e. whether to reduce current energy expenditure to avoid delayed costs). The ability to imagine future scenarios and adjust decisions accordingly may be important for making intertemporal choices in a flexible manner that accords with delayed consequences. Accumulating evidence suggests that cuing people to imagine the future in vivid detail can encourage preferences for delayed outcomes over immediate ones. [12] [13] This has been extended into real-world decisions such as in reducing the consumption of high-calorie food [14] and increasing pro-environmental behaviours. [15]

Clinical impairment

In recent years there have been a range of investigations into variation in prospection and its functions in clinical populations. Deficits to the mechanisms and functions of prospection have been observed in Alzheimer's disease and other age-related dementias, [16] Schizophrenia, and after brain damage (especially to the medial temporal lobes). [17]

Shifts in the content and modes of prospection have been observed in affective disorders. [18] [19] For example, in both clinical depression and anxiety there is an overrepresentation of possible negative future events. In depression, there is additionally a reduction in the generation of possible positive future events. There are also a range of changes to the representational format (i.e. whether people tend to represent the future in episodic or semantic detail) in affective disorders. [20] [21]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive psychology</span> Subdiscipline of psychology

Cognitive psychology is the scientific study of mental processes such as attention, language use, memory, perception, problem solving, creativity, and reasoning.

Hindsight bias, also known as the knew-it-all-along phenomenon or creeping determinism, is the common tendency for people to perceive past events as having been more predictable than they actually were. People often believe that after an event has occurred, they would have predicted or perhaps even would have known with a high degree of certainty what the outcome of the event would have been before the event occurred. Hindsight bias may cause distortions of memories of what was known or believed before an event occurred, and is a significant source of overconfidence regarding an individual's ability to predict the outcomes of future events. Examples of hindsight bias can be seen in the writings of historians describing outcomes of battles, physicians recalling clinical trials, and in judicial systems as individuals attribute responsibility on the basis of the supposed predictability of accidents.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Daydream</span> Aspect of human thought and consciousness

Daydreaming is the stream of consciousness that detaches from current, external tasks when attention drifts to a more personal and internal direction. This phenomenon is common in people's daily life shown by a large-scale study in which participants spend 47% of their waking time on average on daydreaming. There are various names of this phenomenon including mind wandering, fantasy, spontaneous thoughts, etc. Daydreaming is the term used by Jerome L. Singer whose research laid the foundation for nearly all the subsequent research today. The terminologies assigned by researchers today puts challenges on identifying the common features of daydreaming, and on building collective work among researchers.

Episodic memory is the memory of everyday events that can be explicitly stated or conjured. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at particular times and places; for example, the party on one's 7th birthday. Along with semantic memory, it comprises the category of explicit memory, one of the two major divisions of long-term memory.

Explicit memory is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts. This type of memory is dependent upon three processes: acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval. Explicit memory can be divided into two categories: episodic memory, which stores specific personal experiences, and semantic memory, which stores factual information. Explicit memory requires gradual learning, with multiple presentations of a stimulus and response.

Choice-supportive bias or post-purchase rationalization is the tendency to retroactively ascribe positive attributes to an option one has selected and/or to demote the forgone options. It is part of cognitive science, and is a distinct cognitive bias that occurs once a decision is made. For example, if a person chooses option A instead of option B, they are likely to ignore or downplay the faults of option A while amplifying or ascribing new negative faults to option B. Conversely, they are also likely to notice and amplify the advantages of option A and not notice or de-emphasize those of option B.

Metamemory or Socratic awareness, a type of metacognition, is both the introspective knowledge of one's own memory capabilities and the processes involved in memory self-monitoring. This self-awareness of memory has important implications for how people learn and use memories. When studying, for example, students make judgments of whether they have successfully learned the assigned material and use these decisions, known as "judgments of learning", to allocate study time.

Autobiographical memory is a memory system consisting of episodes recollected from an individual's life, based on a combination of episodic and semantic memory. It is thus a type of explicit memory.

Emotion can have a powerful effect on humans and animals. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.

In psychology, mental time travel is the capacity to mentally reconstruct personal events from the past as well as to imagine possible scenarios in the future. The term was coined by Thomas Suddendorf and Michael Corballis, building on Endel Tulving's work on episodic memory.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Effects of alcohol on memory</span> Health effect of alcohol consumption

Ethanol is the type of alcohol found in alcoholic beverages. It is a volatile, flammable, colorless liquid that acts as a central nervous system depressant. Ethanol can impair different types of memory.

Foresight is the ability to predict, or the action of predicting, what will happen or what is needed in the future. Studies suggest that much of human daily thought is directed towards potential future events. Because of this and its role in human control on the planet, the nature and evolution of foresight is an important topic in psychology. Thinking about the future is also studied under the label prospection. Recent neuroscientific, developmental, and cognitive studies have identified many commonalities to the human ability to recall past episodes. Science magazine selected new evidence for such commonalities one of the top ten scientific breakthroughs of 2007. However, there are fundamental differences between mentally travelling through time into the future versus mentally travelling through time into the past.

Autonoetic consciousness is the human ability to mentally place oneself in the past and future or in counterfactual situations, and to thus be able to examine one's own thoughts.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Memory</span> Faculty of mind to store and retrieve data

Memory is the faculty of the mind by which data or information is encoded, stored, and retrieved when needed. It is the retention of information over time for the purpose of influencing future action. If past events could not be remembered, it would be impossible for language, relationships, or personal identity to develop. Memory loss is usually described as forgetfulness or amnesia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Embodied cognition</span> Interdisciplinary theory

Embodied cognition is the theory that many features of cognition, whether human or otherwise, are shaped by aspects of an organism's entire body. Sensory and motor systems are seen as fundamentally integrated with cognitive processing. The cognitive features include high-level mental constructs and performance on various cognitive tasks. The bodily aspects involve the motor system, the perceptual system, the bodily interactions with the environment (situatedness), and the assumptions about the world built into the organism's functional structure.

Episodic-like memory is the memory system in animals that is comparable to human episodic memory. The term was first described by Clayton & Dickinson referring to an animal's ability to encode and retrieve information about 'what' occurred during an episode, 'where' the episode took place, and 'when' the episode happened. This ability in animals is considered 'episodic-like' because there is currently no way of knowing whether or not this form of remembering is accompanied by conscious recollection—a key component of Endel Tulving's original definition of episodic memory.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Eleanor Maguire</span> Irish neuropsychologist

Eleanor Anne Maguire is an Irish neuroscientist. Since 2007, she has been Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience at University College London where she is also a Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellow.

Sex differences in cognition are widely studied in the current scientific literature. Biological and genetic differences in combination with environment and culture have resulted in the cognitive differences among men and women. Among biological factors, hormones such as testosterone and estrogen may play some role mediating these differences. Among differences of diverse mental and cognitive abilities, the largest or most well known are those relating to spatial abilities, social cognition and verbal skills and abilities.

Social cognitive neuroscience is the scientific study of the biological processes underpinning social cognition. Specifically, it uses the tools of neuroscience to study "the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the social world". Social cognitive neuroscience uses the epistemological foundations of cognitive neuroscience, and is closely related to social neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience employs human neuroimaging, typically using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Human brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation are also used. In nonhuman animals, direct electrophysiological recordings and electrical stimulation of single cells and neuronal populations are utilized for investigating lower-level social cognitive processes.

Hyperphantasia is the condition of having extremely vivid mental imagery. It is the opposite condition to Aphantasia, where mental visual imagery is not present. The experience of Hyperphantasia is more common than aphantasia, and has been described as being "as vivid as real seeing". Hyperphantasia constitutes all five senses within vivid mental imagery; however, visual mental imagery research dominates the literature and there is a lack of research on the other four senses.

References

  1. Seligman, Martin E. P.; Railton, Peter; Baumeister, Roy F.; Sripada, Chandra (27 February 2013). "Navigating Into the Future or Driven by the Past". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 8 (2): 119–141. doi:10.1177/1745691612474317. ISSN   1745-6916. PMID   26172493. S2CID   17506436.
  2. Szpunar, Karl K.; Spreng, R. Nathan; Schacter, Daniel L. (30 December 2014). "A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition: Fig. 1". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (52): 18414–18421. Bibcode:2014PNAS..11118414S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417144111 . PMC   4284580 . PMID   25416592.
  3. Suddendorf, T; Bulley, A; Miloyan, B (December 2018). "Prospection and natural selection". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 24: 26–31. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.019. S2CID   53180176.
  4. Suddendorf, Thomas; Corballis, Michael C. (29 October 2007). "The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans?" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30 (3): 299–313, discussion 313–51. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001975. PMID   17963565.
  5. Gilbert, D. T.; Wilson, T. D. (7 September 2007). "Prospection: Experiencing the Future". Science. 317 (5843): 1351–1354. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1351G. doi:10.1126/science.1144161. PMID   17823345. S2CID   19753427.
  6. Suddendorf, T.; Corballis, M. C. (1997). "Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind". Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 123 (2): 133–167. ISSN   8756-7547. PMID   9204544.
  7. Szpunar, Karl K.; Spreng, R. Nathan; Schacter, Daniel L. (30 December 2014). "A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition: Fig. 1". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (52): 18414–18421. Bibcode:2014PNAS..11118414S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417144111 . PMC   4284580 . PMID   25416592.
  8. Suddendorf, Thomas; Moore, Chris (October 2011). "Introduction to the special issue: The development of episodic foresight". Cognitive Development. 26 (4): 295–298. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.001.
  9. Wilson, Timothy D.; Gilbert, Daniel T. (23 June 2016). "Affective Forecasting". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 14 (3): 131–134. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00355.x. S2CID   18373805.
  10. Einstein, Gilles O.; McDaniel, Mark A. (1990). "Normal aging and prospective memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 16 (4): 717–726. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717. PMID   2142956.
  11. Berns, Gregory S.; Laibson, David; Loewenstein, George (November 2007). "Intertemporal choice – toward an integrative framework" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (11): 482–488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011. PMID   17980645. S2CID   22282339.
  12. Bulley, Adam; Henry, Julie; Suddendorf, Thomas (2016). "Prospection and the present moment: The role of episodic foresight in intertemporal choices between immediate and delayed rewards". Review of General Psychology. 20 (1): 29–47. doi:10.1037/gpr0000061. S2CID   84178870.
  13. Rung, Jillian M.; Madden, Gregory J. (September 2018). "Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review and meta-analysis". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 147 (9): 1349–1381. doi:10.1037/xge0000462. PMC   6112163 . PMID   30148386.
  14. Dassen, Fania C.M.; Jansen, Anita; Nederkoorn, Chantal; Houben, Katrijn (January 2016). "Focus on the future: Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking". Appetite. 96: 327–332. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.032. PMID   26431684. S2CID   205612811.
  15. Lee, Pei-Shan; Sung, Yu-Hsien; Wu, Chia-Chun; Ho, Liang-Chu; Chiou, Wen-Bin (26 July 2018). "Using Episodic Future Thinking to Pre-Experience Climate Change Increases Pro-Environmental Behavior". Environment and Behavior. 52: 60–81. doi:10.1177/0013916518790590. S2CID   149526997.
  16. Irish, Muireann; Piolino, Pascale (March 2016). "Impaired capacity for prospection in the dementias - Theoretical and clinical implications". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 55 (1): 49–68. doi:10.1111/bjc.12090. PMID   26014112.
  17. Henry, Julie D.; Addis, Donna Rose; Suddendorf, Thomas; Rendell, Peter G. (March 2016). "Introduction to the Special Issue: Prospection difficulties in clinical populations". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 55 (1): 1–3. doi:10.1111/bjc.12108. PMID   26857430.
  18. MacLeod, Andrew (30 June 2016). "Prospection, well-being and memory". Memory Studies. 9 (3): 266–274. doi:10.1177/1750698016645233. S2CID   147765991.
  19. Roepke, Ann Marie; Seligman, Martin E. P. (March 2016). "Depression and prospection". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 55 (1): 23–48. doi:10.1111/bjc.12087. PMID   26096347.
  20. Miloyan, Beyon; Pachana, Nancy A.; Suddendorf, Thomas (9 December 2013). "The future is here: A review of foresight systems in anxiety and depression". Cognition and Emotion. 28 (5): 795–810. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.863179. PMID   24320101. S2CID   947624.
  21. Bulley, Adam; Henry, Julie D.; Suddendorf, Thomas (March 2017). "Thinking about threats: Memory and prospection in human threat management". Consciousness and Cognition. 49: 53–69. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2017.01.005. PMID   28157585. S2CID   3616477.