Goal setting

Last updated

Goal setting involves the development of an action plan designed in order to motivate and guide a person or group toward a goal. [1] Goals are more deliberate than desires and momentary intentions. Therefore, setting goals means that a person has committed thought, emotion, and behavior towards attaining the goal. In doing so, the goal setter has established a desired future state which differs from their current state thus creating a mismatch which in turn spurs future actions. [2] Goal setting can be guided by goal-setting criteria (or rules) such as SMART criteria. [3] Goal setting is a major component of personal-development and management literature. Studies by Edwin A. Locke and his colleagues, most notably, Gary Latham [4] have shown that more specific and ambitious goals lead to more performance improvement than easy or general goals. Difficult goals should be set ideally at the 90th percentile of performance, [1] assuming that motivation and not ability is limiting attainment of that level of performance. [5] As long as the person accepts the goal, has the ability to attain it, and does not have conflicting goals, there is a positive linear relationship between goal difficulty and task performance. [6]

Contents

The theory of Locke and colleagues states that the simplest, most direct motivational explanation of why some people perform better than others is because they have different performance goals. The essence of the theory is: [7]

  1. Difficult specific goals lead to significantly higher performance than easy goals, no goals, or even the setting of an abstract goal such as urging people to do their best.
  2. Holding ability constant, and given that there is goal commitment, the higher the goal the higher the performance.
  3. Variables such as praise, feedback, or the participation of people in decision-making about the goal only influence behavior to the extent that they lead to the setting of and subsequent commitment to a specific difficult goal.

History

Goal setting theory has been developed through both in the field and laboratory settings. Cecil Alec Mace carried out the first empirical studies in 1935. [8]

Edwin A. Locke began to examine goal setting in the mid-1960s and continued researching goal setting for more than 30 years. [6] [9] [10] He found that individuals who set specific, difficult goals performed better than those who set general, easy goals. [5] Locke derived the idea for goal-setting from Aristotle's form of final causality. Aristotle speculated that purpose can cause action; thus, Locke began researching the impact goals have on human activity. Locke developed and refined his goal-setting theory in the 1960s, publishing his first article on the subject, "Toward a Theory of Task Motivation and Incentives", in 1968. [11] This article established the positive relationship between clearly identified goals and performance.

Main idea

After controlling for ability, goals that are difficult to achieve and specific tend to increase performance far more than easy goals, no goals or telling people to do their best. It therefore follows that the simplest motivational explanation of why some individuals outperform others is that they have different goals. [12] A goal can be made more specific by:

Setting goals can affect outcomes in four ways: [13]

Choice
Goals may narrow someone's attention and direct their efforts toward goal-relevant activities and away from goal-irrelevant actions.
Effort
Goals may make someone more effortful. For example, if someone usually produces 4 widgets per hour but wants to produce 6 widgets per hour, then they may work harder to produce more widgets than without that goal.
Persistence
Goals may make someone more willing to work through setbacks.
Cognition
Goals may cause someone to develop and change their behavior.

Secondary ideas

Goal commitment

People perform better when they are committed to achieving certain goals. Through an understanding of the effect of goal setting on individual performance, organizations are able to use goal setting to benefit organizational performance. [5] In addition, another aspect that goes with goal commitment is also goal acceptance. This is an individual's willingness to pursue their specific goal. [1] Locke and Latham (2002) have indicated three moderators that indicate goal setting success:

  1. The importance of the expected outcomes of goal attainment,
  2. Self-efficacy: one's belief that they are able to achieve their goals,
  3. Commitment to others: promises or engagements to others can strongly improve commitment.

Expanding the three from above, the level of commitment is influenced by external factors. Such as the person assigning the goal, setting the standard for the person to achieve/perform. This influences the level of commitment by how compliant the individual is with the one assigning the goal. [ citation needed ] An external factor can also be the role models of the individual.[ citation needed ] For example, say an individual looks up to their manager and cares about their opinion, the individual is more likely to listen to goal-setting strategies from that individual, and ultimately become more committed to their desired goal. [5]

Internal factors can derive from their participation level in the work to achieve the goal. What they expect from themselves can either flourish their success, or destroy it. Also, the individual may want to appear superior to their peers or competitors. [14] They want to achieve the goal the best and be known for it. The self-reward of accomplishing a goal is usually one of the main keys that keeps individuals committed.[ citation needed ] For example, if an individual was working toward becoming the president of their company, if they achieve their goal, they could reward themselves with something of importance to them. [10]

Another route individuals can take to set their goals is to follow (STD) that is, setting their goals to be Specific, Time-bound, and Difficult. Specifically, an individual's goal should be set at the 90th percentile of difficulty. [1]

Goal–performance relationship

Locke and colleagues (1981) examined the behavioral effects of goal-setting, concluding that 90% of laboratory and field studies involving specific and challenging goals led to higher performance than did easy or no goals. [15] This is because if an individual is intrinsically motivated by a goal, they will want to conquer the goal to receive internal rewards, and will be satisfied because of it. [1]

Locke and Latham (2006) argue that it is not sufficient to urge employees to "do their best". "Doing one's best" has no external reference, which makes it useless in eliciting specific behavior. To elicit some specific form of behavior from another person, it is important that this person has a clear view of what is expected from them. A goal is thereby of vital importance because it helps an individual to focus their efforts in a specified direction. In other words, goals canalize behavior. [6] However, when faced with complex tasks and directions that are difficult to specify, telling someone to "do their best", with a focus on learning, can sometimes lead to the discovery of better strategies whereby specific goals can then be set. [6] :707 A solution to this apparent contradiction where the "do your best" condition can lead to greater task performance than a high specific performance goal under certain conditions is resolved when task complexity is taken into account. Specifically, in a complex task where the prerequisite skills and knowledge to perform the task are not yet in place, the "do your best" condition can outperform the performance goal condition. If a high, specific learning goal is set instead then the goal-performance relationship is maintained and the (learning) goal setting condition outperforms the "do your best" condition. [16]

Feedback

Feedback and goal setting are highly interrelated and more effective when used in conjunction with each other. [5] :708 Feedback cannot be given without goals in the same way that goals can not be established without providing feedback.

Goal setting can lead to the creation of feedback loops, either negative or positive comparison of the output to the goal. Negative feedback loops lead to increasing the input associated with goal attainment to improve output in the next loop cycle. Positive feedback loops, if not sufficiently reinforced, can lead to subsequent setting of goals at a less difficult level. Negative feedback can be reframed and errors seen as beneficial to the learning and goal achievement process and in turn increase participant resilience.[ citation needed ] This reframing process can be taught through error management training and with clear instructions about how to engage with errors. Error management training involves participants practicing metacognitive activities of planning, monitoring, and evaluation. [17]

Negative feedback also interacts with goal type, perceived tension and conscientiousness. People with high conscientiousness and performance goals experience high tension following negative feedback which leads to lower performance.[ citation needed ] This is not the case with learning goals where the effect of negative feedback is less detrimental. [18]

Without proper feedback channels it is impossible for employees to adapt or adjust to the required behavior. Managers should keep track of performance to allow employees to see how effective they have been in attaining their goals. [19] Providing feedback on short-term objectives helps to sustain motivation and commitment to the goal. There are two forms of feedback in which the employee can receive (Outcome and Process feedback). [5] Outcome feedback is after the goal or activity is finished, and process feedback is during the completion of a goal. [1] Feedback should be provided on the strategies followed to achieve the goals and on the final outcomes achieved.

Honing goal setting using temporal motivation theory

Locke and Latham (2004) note that goal-setting theory lacks "the issue of time perspective". [20] Taking this into consideration, Steel and Konig (2006) utilize their temporal motivation theory (TMT) to account for goal setting's effects, and suggest new hypotheses regarding a pair of its moderators: goal difficulty and proximity. [21] The effectiveness of goal setting can be explained by two aspects of TMT: the principle of diminishing returns and temporal discounting. [21] Similar to the expression "the sum of the parts can be greater than the whole", a division of a project into several, immediate, subgoals appears to take advantage of these two elements. [21]

Superordinate goals

Goals can be modelled as forming a hierarchy, within such a hierarchy superordinate goals are the goals at a higher level of abstraction. Superordinate goals tend to less concrete and lack a specific endpoint but have some advantages of more concrete subordinate goal. [22]

Self-regulated learning

While the literature on self-regulated learning covers a broad variety of theoretical perspectives and concepts such as control theory, self-efficacy, action regulation, and resource allocation, goal-setting is a crucial component of virtually all of these approaches as the initiator of self-regulation mechanisms such as planning, monitoring, metacognition, attention, learning strategies, persistence, time management, environmental structuring, help seeking, emotion control, motivation, effort, and self-efficacy. [23]

Goal setting in groups

Objectives and key results

Based initially on Drucker's management by objectives (MBO) model, a popular applied version of goal setting theory for business is the objectives and key results model (OKR). Originally developed at Intel by Andy Grove, [24] the tool was designed to set individual and collaborative goal team goals that are specific, concrete, challenging, ambitious and have measurable and time bound key results. [25] OKRs were later introduced to many other companies and foundations such as Google [26] and the Gates Foundation by Grove disciple John Doerr whose book Measure what Matters outlines the use of OKRs across a wide range of organizational settings. [27]

Goal setting in sports and fitness

The tenets of Goal setting theory generally hold true in physical domains. In a study of high school students using sit up tests all students set a specific and challenging goal out performed students with a non-specific goal supporting the principles of goal specificity and goal difficulty from general goal setting theory. [28] Goal setting appears to be especially beneficial to athletes with self-inflated narcissism by protecting athletes with that personality trait from their tendency to underperform in tedious but necessary training with no audience present. [29]

Goal setting in workplaces

In business, goal setting remains a popular evidence based approach to align efforts across organizations, communicate objectives, and improve motivation as well as task performance for individuals and groups. [30] Goal setting encourages participants to put in substantial effort over and above a "do your best condition". Also, because every member has defined expectations for their role, little room is left for inadequate, marginal effort to go unnoticed

Employee motivation

The more employees are motivated, the more they are stimulated and interested in accepting goals. These success factors are interdependent. For example, the expected outcomes of goals are positively influenced when employees are involved in the goal setting process. Not only does participation increase commitment in attaining the goals that are set, participation influences self-efficacy as well. Additionally, feedback is necessary to monitor one's progress. When feedback is not presented, an employee might think they are not making enough progress. This can reduce self-efficacy and thereby harm the performance outcomes in the long run. [31]

  • Goal-commitment, the most influential moderator,[ citation needed ] becomes especially important when dealing with difficult or complex goals. If people lack commitment to goals, they lack motivation to reach them. To commit to a goal, one must believe in its importance or significance.
  • Attainability: individuals must also believe that they can attain—or at least partially reach—a defined goal. If they think no chance exists of reaching a goal, they may not even try.
  • Self-efficacy: the higher someone's self-efficacy regarding a certain task, the more likely they will set higher goals, and the more persistence they will show in achieving them. [32]

Workplace training

Goal setting is used to improve training outcomes. For example, Tomokazu Kishiki and colleagues performed a randomized controlled trial on surgical trainees to determine whether or not their participation in a goal-setting program would improve performance and testing scores; the addition of achievable goals appeared to be beneficial to the trainees. [33] When goal setting is applied optimally during training processes, both employee motivation and organizational commitment can increase. [34]

Furthermore, training in goal setting has been linked to higher levels of performance among adults and children with mild to severe intellectual disability. [35]

Work-life balance

Goal setting is also used by some companies with a stated aim of ensuring that employee work life balance is maintained. The idea behind this is that employees set a non-work related goal to improve their well-being, and managers help team members stick to those goals. An example of this in practice is "One Simple Thing", a goal-setting well-being practice employed by Google. [36]

Impact on individual performance

Managers cannot constantly drive motivation, or keep track of an employee's work on a continuous basis. Goals are therefore an important tool for managers, since goals have the ability to function as a self-regulatory mechanism that helps employees prioritize tasks. [5] [37]

Four mechanisms through which goal setting can affect individual performance are:

  1. Goals focus attention toward goal-relevant activities and away from goal-irrelevant activities.
  2. Goals serve as an energizer: Higher goals induce greater effort, while low goals induce lesser effort.
  3. Goals affect persistence; constraints with regard to resources affect work pace.
  4. Goals activate cognitive knowledge and strategies that help employees cope with the situation at hand.

Interventions in education

Domain general benefits from goal setting

Goal setting research has shown positive results as an effective, and inexpensive to implement intervention for a broad range of academic purposes across a range of age groups. Beginning with struggling undergraduate students, there is some evidence that when compared to a control group, students who had set general rather than domain specific academic goals experienced the following benefits in the semester after the intervention namely, i) increased GPA, ii) a higher probability of maintaining a full course load, iii) a reduction in self reported negative affect. While goal setting research tends to be domain specific, these results among others, does suggest that benefits from goal setting may have broader benefits for goal setters even beyond the domain of the goal itself. [38] [39]

Further work with undergraduates has suggested that these broader benefits apply even if non-academic goals are set. This could imply that the original goal setting theory can be modified to include a more domain general "spread" effect from superordinate life goals and that writing about general life goals regardless of domain can improve academic performance. Developing these life goals can include linked procedures such as i) reflecting on/envisaging one's best possible life, ii) listing good quality goals relevant to achieving this best possible life, iii) strategizing on how to achieve their attainment, iv) reflect in writing about anticipated obstacles, v) developing specific plans for overcoming these anticipated obstacles. [40]

Closing achievement gaps based on gender and ethnicity

Goal setting interventions have shown promising and scalable results in terms of closing persistent gender (ongoing male underperformance) and ethnicity achievement gaps in the areas of academic achievement and increased retention rates. In one study persistent male underperformance in tertiary education was almost entirely closed to achieve parity with females after one year of the intervention. Similar albeit slower positive impacts in closing the ethnicity achievement gap by the second year of the same goal setting intervention were reported. These findings suggest that a goal setting intervention early in students' academic careers can significantly and substantially reduce gender and ethnic minority inequalities in academic achievement at least at the tertiary level. [41] More work remains to be done to see if similar conclusions can be drawn at the level of secondary education.

Use of reflection diaries for goal setting

Goal-setting activities with final-year university students focused around self-reflective and personal growth through setting three growth goals and recording progress in reflective diaries shows that goal setting and making progress towards the goals can have many positive impacts. These include increased self-esteem, time and improved stress management and self-monitoring skills as well as motivating, and energizing effects. These suggest that, at least with undergraduates, the setting of growth goals with a reflective diary element can be a useful addition to academic programs. [42]

Online learning and massive open online courses (MOOCs)

Goal-setting activities including the setting of both performance and learning goals have been associated with both increased performance and completion rates for MOOC participants. Students who completed a goal setting writing activity at the start of a course achieved more over a longer period of time than those who did not set goals. [43] For online learning more generally, students who have a better understanding of the tasks set better more detailed goals and in turn achieve higher performance suggesting that instructional time spent explaining learning tasks can be beneficial. [44]

Behavior management

Properly implemented taught goal-setting programs are effective in K-12 schools for behavior and emotional management interventions. [45] In particular, programs that included student input on the goal setting process as well as the collection of targeting data to monitor progress and ensure the delivery of high quality feedback to students on progress towards goals were more effective behavior management interventions. [46] In order to ensure a properly designed goal setting intervention for behavior management some variation of a checklist can be an effective addition to behavioral management programs. A typical such checklist could include the following factors:

  1. Identify and define behavior
  2. Establish a behavior monitoring plan
  3. Collect baseline data
  4. Set goals
  5. Monitor progress towards goals
  6. Review data [47]

Goal-setting also works effectively either or its own or as part of a package of other behavior management interventions. [48]

In personal life

Identifying sub-goals

Common personal goals include losing weight, achieving good grades, and saving money. The strategy for goal setting begins with the big picture; taking a look at the big picture before breaking it into smaller components allows one to focus on the primary goal. Once the main goal is set, breaking it up into smaller, more achievable components helps in the planning portion of setting the goal. [49] These smaller, more obtainable objectives promote self-esteem and provide instant feedback to keep the individual on task. [50] [51]

Time and task management

Time management is the practice of systematically finishing tasks assigned by superiors or one's self in an efficient and timely manner. Time management steps require identifying the objective and laying out a plan that maximizes efficiency and execution of the objective. [52] There are many useful mobile apps that help with personal goal setting; some of the categories include budgeting, wellness, calendar and productivity apps. [53] [54]

The book What They Don't Teach You in the Harvard Business School is known for citing a study which found that written goals have a significant effect on financial success, but in 1996 Fast Company determined that this study did not occur. [55] In 2015, a research study on goals found those who wrote them down accomplished them at a significantly higher rate than those who did not. [55]

Life goals

There is evidence that setting and reflecting on progress life goals are an effective intervention to provide both a sense of purpose and increase happiness. [56] In particular, setting life goals based on others leads to more positive emotions and therefore has a more positive impact on happiness than goals focused on oneself. Further evidence for this effect is provided by the more broader personal benefits of prosocial behavior and acts of kindness towards others rather than self care/focusing on oneself. [57]

Recovery from illness and injury

There is evidence from randomized control trials that goal setting treatments improved executive function, attention/working memory, and learning in stroke patients. As well as suggesting that there is a motivational element to vascular cognitive impairment caused by strokes, or at least in terms of recovering from them, goal setting does appear to be a useful, easy to implement and cost effective solution to improve cognitive outcomes in stroke patients. [58]

Limitations and side effects

Goal-setting has limitations and there is some evidence of potentially harmful side effects to both individuals and organizations from misuse of goals and in particular the use of performance/outcome goals.

Leader and organization goal misalignment

In an organization, a goal of a manager may not align with the goals of the organization as a whole. In such cases, the goals of an individual may come into direct conflict with the employing organization. Without clearly aligning goals between the organization and the individual, overall performance may suffer.

Unethical behavior

Additionally, there is evidence that suggests that goal-setting can foster unethical behavior when people do not achieve their desired goals. [59] Schweitzer et al. found empirical support for their hypotheses that specific goals, rather than "do your best" goals, would lead participants to overstate performance if their true performance fell short of the goal, with the overstatement frequency increasing as the performance-goal gap narrowed. Niven and Healy found that a subset of the population having a relatively high tendency to morally justify behavior was more likely to engage in the kind of cheating identified by Schweitzer et al. [60] Particular side effects associated with goal setting include a narrow focus that neglects non-goal areas, more unethical behavior, distorted risk preferences, damage to organizational culture, and reduced intrinsic motivation. [61] High performance goal seem particularly likely to induce unethical behaviour under certain circumstances by creating the desire to achieve the goal but also altering moral reasoning processes and in particular, moral disengagement and encourage moral motivated reasoning due to the focus on attaining the goal. [62]

Goals may also result in overly singleminded competition within organizations if two or more people have goals that encourage competition rather than cooperation. This can include withholding information or ideas, obstructing the goal progress of others or becoming indifferent to their progress and so withdrawing completely from interaction with other organization members. A solution to some of these potential issues is to set a unifying organizational vision or superordinate goal. [63] :180–181

Tunnel vision

Goal setting may have the drawback of inhibiting implicit learning if the required knowledge and strategic awareness are not in place: goal setting may encourage simple focus on an outcome without openness to exploration, understanding, or growth and result in lower performance than simply encouraging people to "do their best". [63] :68–69 A solution to this limitation is to set learning goals as well as performance goals, so that learning is expected as part of the process of reaching goals. [64] [65] The section on learning goals has more information on this effect and how to counter it. Goal setting also may impair performance in certain situations. Such situations include when an individual becomes overly focused on accomplishing a previously-set goal that they end up underperforming on current tasks. [66]

Narrow focus

Goal setting theory has been criticized for being too narrow in focus to be a complete theory of work motivation as goals alone are not sufficient to address all aspects of workplace motivation. In particular, it does not address why some people choose goals they dislike or how to increase intrinsic rather than extrinsic motivation. [67] This raises the possibility that goal setting may in turn be subsumed within a more comprehensive theory of work motivation in time.

Developments in theory

Self efficacy

An important addition to goal setting theory was the incorporation of self-efficacy from Bandura's social cognitive theory. Broadly defined as task specific self-confidence, goal setting theory incorporates self-efficacy in the following ways:

  1. People with higher self-efficacy set harder goals which as per the terms of the theory lead to higher motivation and task performance
  2. People with higher self-efficacy are more committed to the goals they have set and in turn more likely to achieve them.
  3. People with higher self-efficacy are more likely to respond positively to negative feedback and use it productively rather than be discouraged.
  4. Appropriately challenging leader assigned goals and communicating these powerfully can increase follower self-efficacy as they directly imply that the leader has confidence in the employees' ability to achieve them.
  5. Leaders can also increase follower self-efficacy, and in turn goal commitment and task performance, by providing quality training and either modeling task performance themselves or providing appropriate performance models. [68]

Self-efficacy levels can also influence how people react to not meeting specific challenging goals. People with high self-efficacy redouble their efforts whereas people with low self-efficacy expend less effort and coast along. Goal achievement also interacts with self-efficacy and goal achievement does not necessarily lead to increased efforts as after meeting challenging goals some can be reluctant to expend a similar level of effort again and will settle for the goal they have. [69]

Goal choice

Self-efficacy, past experiences, and various other social factors influence goal setting. [6] Failure to achieve previous goals often leads to setting more achievable goals.

Learning goals

There are times when having specific performance goals is not a best option; this is the case when the goal requires skills or knowledge that have not yet been acquired. Tunnel vision can be a consequence of specific performance goals; if a person is too focused on attaining a specific goal, they may ignore the need to learn new skills or acquire new information. This concept is illustrated well by the "basketball game task" study in which observers watched a video of a group of people wearing white shirts and black shirts who are passing a basketball back and forth, and the observers were instructed to count the number of times a basketball is passed between only the players wearing white shirts. During the video, a woman carrying an open umbrella walks across the screen. Of 28 observers who were focused on counting the number of passes between only the players wearing white shirts, only 6 reported noticing the woman carrying the umbrella. When observers watched the video without focusing on a specific task, all of the observers noticed the umbrella woman. [70] In situations where the risk of tunnel vision is high, the best option is to set a learning goal. A learning goal is a generalized goal to achieve knowledge in a certain topic or field, but it can ultimately lead to better performance in more complex tasks related to the learning goals. [64] [65]

Further to the above, learning goals can be more specifically operationalized as "a desired number of strategies, processes, or procedures to be developed in order to master a task'" [71] . Some specific examples of learning goals from the literature are below:

Locke and Latham (2006) attribute this response to metacognition. They believe that "a learning goal facilitates or enhances metacognition—namely, planning, monitoring, and evaluating progress toward goal attainment". [6] This is necessary in environments with little or no guidance and structure. Although jobs typically have set goals, individual goals and achievement can benefit from metacognition. Some possible uses of learning goals follow:

Framing

Framing, or how goals are viewed, influences performance. When one feels threatened and or intimidated by a high goal they perform poorer than those who view the goal as a challenge. [6] Individuals who identify situations as challenge perform better under difficult performance goal conditions. Individuals who view situations as threats get better results using learning goals focused on developing strategy to achieve the task. [75] These results connect goal setting theory to Folkman and Lazurus' Transactional Model of Stress and Coping which focused on the subjective appraisal of stress as being crucial to performance under challenging conditions.

Habits

Habits, defined as "behavioral tendencies tied to specific contexts, such as time of day, location, the presence of particular people, preceding actions, or even one's mood", habits develop through context, repetition, and reward and interact closely with goals to impact (often negatively) goal attainment. While goal setting can initiate behaviour change, it does appear likely that interventions combining goal setting with habit management strategies through disrupting bad habits by making them harder to fall into are more likely to be successful. Habits also reduce cognitive load and therefore good habit formation may be of benefit in particular to learning goal achievement which is often associated with more complex tasks by freeing up the cognitive resources needed to work towards the learning goal. [76]

Affect

Realization of goals has an effect on affect—that is, feelings of success and satisfaction. Achieving goals has a positive effect, and failing to meet goals has negative consequences. [6] However, the effect of goals is not exclusive to one realm. Success in one's job can compensate for feelings of failure in one's personal life. [6]

Group goals

The relationship between group goals and individual goals influences group performance; when goals are compatible there is a positive effect, but when goals are incompatible the effects can be detrimental to the group's performance. [6] There is another factor at work in groups, and that is the sharing factor; a positive correlation exists between sharing information within the group and group performance. [6] In the case of group goals, feedback needs to be related to the group, not individuals, in order for it to improve the group's performance. [6]

Goal concordance (agreement) among members of groups as well as concordance across hierarchies in organizations has positive performance impacts. [77] Research evaluating effects of goals on employee commitment found an indirect relationship mediated by employee perception of organizational support, suggesting that leaders directly support goal setting by individual employees. [78]

Overall, the available evidence suggests that group goals can have a robust effect on group performance. Less clearly, individual goals may promote group performance if used cautiously as in interdependent groups there is a potential for goal conflict between individual and group goals which could hinder group performance. There does appear to be a need for more work in this area. [79]

Values

In goal setting terms, values can be defined as trans-situational goals with goals being more specific than values which are higher order and more general. In this sense goals can be defined further as the mechanism by which values lead to action. [63] Goals can provide a vehicle for closing the value-action gap.

Sub-goals or proximal goals

Goal setting theory generally, but not always, supports the use of sub-goals (also known as proximal goals) which are intermediate/stepping stone goals on the way to goals (also known as distal goals). Proximal goals work by providing immediate incentives to maintain current performance, whereas distal goals are too far removed to have the same effect. In complex problem-solving tasks, setting subgoals increased initial self-efficacy and attaining proximal goals increased self-efficacy, performance satisfaction, and task persistence. [80] One of the ways to reduce self-defeating while accomplishing sub-goals is to make sure to have deadlines for each sub-goal. Setting these deadlines adds a factor of accountability and helps to check on ourselves. The main reason why we don't usually accomplish sub-goals is because we don't put a timeframe to them. [81]

While generally positive, setting too many sub-goals can have negative impacts such as reduced satisfaction (it's not an achievement to complete a goal that is too easy) and send the signal that managers do not have faith in employee ability to achieve challenging goals. [80]

Goals and traits

On a basic level, the two types of goals are learning goals and performance goals; each possesses different traits associated with the selected goal. [6] [64]

Learning goals involve tasks where skills and knowledge can be acquired, whereas performance goals involve easy-to-accomplish tasks that will make one appear successful (thus tasks where error and judgment may be possible are avoided).

A more complex trait-mediation study is the one conducted by Lee, Sheldon, and Turban (2003), [82] which yielded the following results:

Goal orientation

Whereas goal setting theory was developed in the sub-domain organizational psychology and primarily focuses on motivation and measuring task performance, the related but distinct literature around goal orientation was developed in the sub-domain of educational psychology and tends to focus on ability and trait measurement, this division has led to attempts to integrate the two literatures which in turn has led to the following conclusions:

These conclusions have led to the following inferences:

Macro-level goals

Macro-level goals refer to goal setting that is applied to the company as a whole. Cooperative goals reduce the negative feelings that occur as a result of alliances and the formation of groups. [6] The most common parties involved are the company and its suppliers. The three motivators for macro-level goals are: self-efficacy, growth goals, and organizational vision. [6]

Sub-conscious goals

Recent reviews of the available evidence suggests that goal setting theory applies to subconscious goals as well as consciously set goals. Subconsciously priming achievement goals through achievement related words and/or suitably triumphal photographs can significantly improve task and therefore job performance. Further enhancing this effect, context specific primes appear to induce substantially stronger goal effects. Furthermore, primed goals and consciously set goals work better together in improving task/job performance. [83] Inevitably the use of sub-conscious goal with employees to improve work performance carries with it many potential ethical issues and concerns. [63]

General action and inaction goals

Action goals encourage people to engage in more active behaviors, whereas inactive goals tend to result as inactive behaviors. [84] [85] Common action goals can be to do something, perform a certain act, or to go someplace, whereas typical inaction goals can take the form of having a rest or to stop doing something.

Goal-regulated overall activity and inactivity tendency result from both biological conditions and social-cultural environment. [86] [ page needed ] Recent research revealed that most nations hold more favorable attitude towards action rather than inaction, even though some countries value action and inaction slightly differently than others. [87]

Recent research suggested that people tend to choose inaction goals when they are making decisions among choices where uncertainty could result in negative outcomes, but they prefer action over inaction in their daily behaviors when no deliberation is needed. [88] [89] Timothy D. Wilson and colleagues found that many people "preferred to administer electric shocks to themselves instead of being left alone with their thoughts". [90]

Combining learning and performance goals

In workplace settings employees are often expected to achieve performance outcomes at tasks that are moderately complex and require learning new things. As noted above, setting performance goals can cause difficulties and lower performance compared to a "do your best" condition when prerequisite skills, strategies and knowledge are not in place which may be due to a cognitive load effect arising from the demands of complex tasks for relative novices. For these sorts of complex task situations learning and performance goals can be used effectively in combination if logically connected. [91] Furthermore, while learning goals do tend to be most effective for new and complex tasks requiring complex application of strategy to achieve the task this does not mean that learning goals will be motivational enough on their own to ensure that the new strategies are used and an additional performance goal could motivate employees to actually use the discovered or acquired strategies to attain the desired outcome. [73]

Stretch goals

Stretch or extremely hard to reach goals remain a subject of considerable debate with arguments both for and against their use. Among the potential negative side effects of stretch goals include them being dismissed as absurd or ignored by employees. Even if taken seriously, stretch goals can lead to employee burn out attempting to achieve them. Caveats aside, there are some ways that stretch goals can be valuable in spurring creative solutions to problems and new directions especially if used alongside more normal goals and without the need to achieve them but instead measure how much progress was made towards them. [72]

Controversies, responses and resolutions

As a theory developed through induction there have been, and continue to be, circumstances where goal setting theory has been challenged and/or conflicting results have been reported. Specific examples of these controversies and resolutions to them are discussed below.

Assigned goals versus participatively set goals

The question of whether or not participatively set goals are more motivating than supervisor set goals arose due to differences in findings between Erez and her colleagues and those of Latham and colleagues. Erez and her colleagues found evidence that under certain circumstances Latham's earlier conclusion that performance was the same regardless of whether or not goals were set by supervisors or participatively, was wrong. This disagreement was resolved through a process beginning with a conversation between Erez and Latham with Locke as the neutral interlocutor which in turn led to both Erez and Latham jointly designing an experiment to explore the reasons for their disagreement. [92] This collaboration of two researchers with differing views is an early example of adversarial collaboration and led to the following published findings that resolved the disagreement:

  1. Supervisor set goals are equally as motivating if they are accompanied by a reason for the goal.
  2. Participation in goal setting and decision making improves employee performance through increasing self-efficacy and aiding the discovery of suitable task strategies to achieve goals. [63] [93]

The Dark Side of Goal Setting

A 2009 article, "Goals Gone Wild" by Ordonez et al., sparked controversy by suggesting goal setting might lead to unethical behavior. [61] The authors argued that the benefits of goal setting are often overstated, while its downsides are underreported. [94]

One concern is that specific, challenging goals can lead to narrow thinking. Employees become laser-focused on achieving the goal, potentially neglecting other important aspects of their job. For instance, the article cites the case of Ford Motor Company. Under pressure to build a lighter car, safety measures were overlooked. [95] This exemplifies how a singular focus on achieving a goal can have negative consequences.

Ordonez et al. further argue that setting too many goals or offering excessive rewards for quick results can pressure employees to prioritize quantity over quality and even resort to unethical shortcuts. Additionally, the authors suggest that goal setting might decrease intrinsic motivation by emphasizing extrinsic rewards. [61]

This perspective challenges the traditional view of goal setting as a universally positive tool. Locke and Latham countered these arguments, while leveling accusations of Ordonez et al. having violated principles of good scholarship. [96] Locke and Latham emphasized the importance of goals in organizational behavior and for individual purpose setting. [96] A further reply from Ordonez et al. disputed Locke and Latham's points. [97] The debate continues, with some scholars proposing learning goals as a potential solution to ethical concerns arising from performance goals. [98]

While goal setting is a powerful tool for motivation and performance, it requires careful handling. Overemphasizing specific, challenging goals without considering potential downsides can lead to ethical lapses and counterproductive behavior. A more balanced approach is necessary, involving thoughtful goal design, awareness of potential side effects, and ongoing monitoring. By taking a critical look at goal setting, organizations can ensure it fosters both ethical conduct and successful outcomes. [99]


See also

Related Research Articles

Work design is an area of research and practice within industrial and organizational psychology, and is concerned with the "content and organization of one's work tasks, activities, relationships, and responsibilities" (p. 662). Research has demonstrated that work design has important implications for individual employees, teams, organisations, and society.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Job satisfaction</span> Attitude of a person towards work

Job satisfaction, employee satisfaction or work satisfaction is a measure of workers' contentment with their job, whether they like the job or individual aspects or facets of jobs, such as nature of work or supervision. Job satisfaction can be measured in cognitive (evaluative), affective, and behavioral components. Researchers have also noted that job satisfaction measures vary in the extent to which they measure feelings about the job. or cognitions about the job.

Expectancy theory proposes that an individual will behave or act in a certain way because they are motivated to select a specific behavior over others due to what they expect the result of that selected behavior will be. In essence, the motivation of the behavior selection is determined by the desirability of the outcome. However, at the core of the theory is the cognitive process of how an individual processes the different motivational elements. This is done before making the ultimate choice. The outcome is not the sole determining factor in making the decision of how to behave.

Content theory is a subset of motivational theories that try to define what motivates people. Content theories of motivation often describe a system of needs that motivate peoples' actions. While process theories of motivation attempt to explain how and why our motivations affect our behaviors, content theories of motivation attempt to define what those motives or needs are. Content theory includes the work of David McClelland, Abraham Maslow and other psychologists.

In social psychology, social loafing is the phenomenon of a person exerting less effort to achieve a goal when they work in a group than when working alone. It is seen as one of the main reasons groups are sometimes less productive than the combined performance of their members working as individuals.

In psychology, self-efficacy is an individual's belief in their capacity to act in the ways necessary to reach specific goals. The concept was originally proposed by the psychologist Albert Bandura in 1977.

Self-determination theory (SDT) is a macro theory of human motivation and personality that concerns people's innate growth tendencies and innate psychological needs. It pertains to the motivation behind people's choices in the absence of external influences and distractions. SDT focuses on the degree to which human behavior is self-motivated and self-determined.

Goal theory is the label used in educational psychology to discuss research into motivation to learn. Goals of learning are thought to be a key factor influencing the level of a student's intrinsic motivation.

Confidence is the feeling of belief or trust that a person or thing is reliable. Self-confidence is trust in oneself. Self-confidence involves a positive belief that one can generally accomplish what one wishes to do in the future. Self-confidence is not the same as self-esteem, which is an evaluation of one's worth. Self-confidence is related to self-efficacy—belief in one's ability to accomplish a specific task or goal. Confidence can be a self-fulfilling prophecy, as those without it may fail because they lack it, and those with it may succeed because they have it rather than because of an innate ability or skill.

Social cognitive theory (SCT), used in psychology, education, and communication, holds that portions of an individual's knowledge acquisition can be directly related to observing others within the context of social interactions, experiences, and outside media influences. This theory was advanced by Albert Bandura as an extension of his social learning theory. The theory states that when people observe a model performing a behavior and the consequences of that behavior, they remember the sequence of events and use this information to guide subsequent behaviors. Observing a model can also prompt the viewer to engage in behavior they already learned. Depending on whether people are rewarded or punished for their behavior and the outcome of the behavior, the observer may choose to replicate behavior modeled. Media provides models for a vast array of people in many different environmental settings.

Behavioural change theories are attempts to explain why human behaviours change. These theories cite environmental, personal, and behavioural characteristics as the major factors in behavioural determination. In recent years, there has been increased interest in the application of these theories in the areas of health, education, criminology, energy and international development with the hope that understanding behavioural change will improve the services offered in these areas. Some scholars have recently introduced a distinction between models of behavior and theories of change. Whereas models of behavior are more diagnostic and geared towards understanding the psychological factors that explain or predict a specific behavior, theories of change are more process-oriented and generally aimed at changing a given behavior. Thus, from this perspective, understanding and changing behavior are two separate but complementary lines of scientific investigation.

A goal or objective is an idea of the future or desired result that a person or a group of people envision, plan, and commit to achieve. People endeavour to reach goals within a finite time by setting deadlines.

Organizational behavior management (OBM) is a subdiscipline of applied behavior analysis (ABA), which is the application of behavior analytic principles and contingency management techniques to change behavior in organizational settings. Through these principles and assessment of behavior, OBM seeks to analyze and employ antecedent, influencing actions of an individual before the action occurs, and consequence, what happens as a result of someone's actions, interventions which influence behaviors linked to the mission and key objectives of the organization and its workers. Such interventions have proven effective through research in improving common organizational areas including employee productivity, delivery of feedback, safety, and overall morale of said organization.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Edwin Locke</span> American psychologist (b.1938)

Edwin A. Locke is an American psychologist and a pioneer in goal-setting theory. He is a retired Dean's Professor of Motivation and Leadership at the Robert H. Smith School of Business at the University of Maryland, College Park. He was also affiliated with the Department of Psychology. As stated by the Association for Psychological Science, "Locke is the most published organizational psychologist in the history of the field. His pioneering research has advanced and enriched our understanding of work motivation and job satisfaction. The theory that is synonymous with his name—goal-setting theory—is perhaps the most widely-respected theory in industrial-organizational psychology. His 1976 chapter on job satisfaction continues to be one of the most highly-cited pieces of work in the field."

Job performance assesses whether a person performs a job well. Job performance, studied academically as part of industrial and organizational psychology, also forms a part of human resources management. Performance is an important criterion for organizational outcomes and success. John P. Campbell describes job performance as an individual-level variable, or something a single person does. This differentiates it from more encompassing constructs such as organizational performance or national performance, which are higher-level variables.

Goal orientation, or achievement orientation, is an "individual disposition towards developing or validating one's ability in achievement settings". In general, an individual can be said to be mastery or performance oriented, based on whether one's goal is to develop one's ability or to demonstrate one's ability, respectively. A mastery orientation is also sometimes referred to as a learning orientation.

Core self-evaluations (CSE) represent a stable personality trait which encompasses an individual's subconscious, fundamental evaluations about themselves, their own abilities and their own control. People who have high core self-evaluations will think positively of themselves and be confident in their own abilities. Conversely, people with low core self-evaluations will have a negative appraisal of themselves and will lack confidence. The concept of core self-evaluations was first examined by Judge, Locke, and Durham (1997) and involves four personality dimensions: locus of control, neuroticism, generalized self-efficacy, and self-esteem. The trait developed as a dispositional predictor of job satisfaction, but has expanded to predict a variety of other outcomes. Core self-evaluations are particularly important because they represent a personality trait which will remain consistent over time. Furthermore, the way in which people appraise themselves using core self-evaluations has the ability to predict positive work outcomes, specifically, job satisfaction and job performance. These relationships have inspired increasing amounts of research on core self-evaluations and suggest valuable implications about the importance this trait may have for organizations.

Work motivation is a person's internal disposition toward work. To further this, an incentive is the anticipated reward or aversive event available in the environment. While motivation can often be used as a tool to help predict behavior, it varies greatly among individuals and must often be combined with ability and environmental factors to actually influence behavior and performance. Results from a 2012 study, which examined age-related differences in work motivation, suggest a "shift in people's motives" rather than a general decline in motivation with age. That is, it seemed that older employees were less motivated by extrinsically related features of a job, but more by intrinsically rewarding job features. Work motivation is strongly influenced by certain cultural characteristics. Between countries with comparable levels of economic development, collectivist countries tend to have higher levels of work motivation than do countries that tend toward individualism. Similarly measured, higher levels of work motivation can be found in countries that exhibit a long versus a short-term orientation. Also, while national income is not itself a strong predictor of work motivation, indicators that describe a nation's economic strength and stability, such as life expectancy, are. Work motivation decreases as a nation's long-term economic strength increases. Currently work motivation research has explored motivation that may not be consciously driven. This method goal setting is referred to as goal priming.

Employee motivation is an intrinsic and internal drive to put forth the necessary effort and action towards work-related activities. It has been broadly defined as the "psychological forces that determine the direction of a person's behavior in an organisation, a person's level of effort and a person's level of persistence". Also, "Motivation can be thought of as the willingness to expend energy to achieve a goal or a reward. Motivation at work has been defined as 'the sum of the processes that influence the arousal, direction, and maintenance of behaviors relevant to work settings'." Motivated employees are essential to the success of an organization as motivated employees are generally more productive at the work place.

Alex Stajkovic is an Organizational Behavior (OB) professor who has conducted research on confidence and goal priming. He holds the Dean's Professorship in Business at the Wisconsin School of Business at the University of Wisconsin - Madison. His research bears on self-efficacy, confidence, and primed goals. Stajkovic co-authored papers with Albert Bandura, Edwin Locke, and Fred Luthans. Stajkovic is a contributing editor to the Journal of Applied Psychology, as well as a member of the Midwestern Psychological Association and Society for Science of Motivation.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Grant, Anthony M. (September 2012). "An integrated model of goal-focused coaching: an evidence-based framework for teaching and practice" (PDF). International Coaching Psychology Review. 7 (2): 146–165 (149). doi:10.53841/bpsicpr.2012.7.2.146. S2CID   255938190. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-11-29. Goal setting should be done in such a way as to facilitate the development and implementation of an action plan. The action plan should be designed to motivate the individual into action, and should also incorporate means of monitoring and evaluating performance, thus providing information on which to base follow-up coaching sessions.
  2. Inzlicht, Michael; Legault, Lisa; Teper, Rimma (2014-08-01). "Exploring the Mechanisms of Self-Control Improvement". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 23 (4): 302–307. doi:10.1177/0963721414534256. ISSN   0963-7214. S2CID   3909434.
  3. Grant, Anthony M. (September 2012). "An integrated model of goal-focused coaching: an evidence-based framework for teaching and practice" (PDF). International Coaching Psychology Review. 7 (2): 146–165 (147). doi:10.53841/bpsicpr.2012.7.2.146. S2CID   255938190. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-11-29. Whilst the ideas represented by the acronym SMART are indeed broadly supported by goal theory (e.g. Locke, 1996), and the acronym SMART may well be useful in some instances in coaching practice, I think that the widespread belief that goals are synonymous with SMART action plans has done much to stifle the development of a more sophisticated understanding and use of goal theory in the coaching community, and this point has important implications for coaching research, teaching and practice.
  4. "Gary Latham".
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P. (2002). "Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey". American Psychologist. 57 (9): 705–717. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.126.9922 . doi:10.1037/0003-066x.57.9.705. ISSN   0003-066X. PMID   12237980. S2CID   17534210.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P. (October 2006). "New directions in goal-setting theory". Current Directions in Psychological Science . 15 (5): 265–268. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.553.1396 . doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00449.x. hdl:10818/8239. S2CID   17856375. Goal-setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2002) was developed inductively within industrial/organizational (I/O) psychology over a 25-year period, based on some 400 laboratory and field studies. These studies showed that specific, high (hard) goals lead to a higher level of task performance than do easy goals or vague, abstract goals such as the exhortation to 'do one's best'. So long as a person is committed to the goal, has the requisite ability to attain it, and does not have conflicting goals, there is a positive, linear relationship between goal difficulty and task performance. Because goals refer to future valued outcomes, the setting of goals is first and foremost a discrepancy-creating process. It implies discontent with one's present condition and the desire to attain an object or outcome.
  7. Latham, Gary P. (2009). "Motivate employee performance through goal-setting". In Locke, Edwin A. (ed.). Handbook of principles of organizational behavior: indispensable knowledge for evidence-based management (2nd ed.). Chichester, UK; Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 161–178. doi:10.1002/9781119206422.ch9. ISBN   978-0-470-74095-8. OCLC   317456331.
  8. Carson, Paula Phillips; Carson, Kerry D.; Heady, Ronald B. (1994). "Cecil Alec Mace: the man who discovered goal-setting". International Journal of Public Administration. 17 (9): 1679–1708. doi:10.1080/01900699408524960.
  9. Locke, Edwin A. (Spring 1996). "Motivation through conscious goal setting". Applied and Preventive Psychology. 5 (2): 117–124. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(96)80005-9.
  10. 1 2 Locke, Edwin A. (2001). "Motivation by goal setting". In Golembiewski, Robert T. (ed.). Handbook of organizational behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Marcel Dekker. pp. 43–56. ISBN   978-0-8247-0393-6. OCLC   44681839.
  11. Locke, Edwin A. (May 1968). "Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 3 (2): 157–189. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(68)90004-4.
  12. Swezey, Robert W.; Meltzer, Andrew L.; Salas, Eduardo (1994). "Some issues involved in motivating teams". In O'Neil, Harold F.; Drillings, Michael (eds.). Motivation: theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p.  146. ISBN   978-0-8058-1287-9. OCLC   29952231.
  13. Latham, Gary P.; Budworth, Marie-Hélène (2007). "The study of work motivation in the 20th century". In Koppes, Laura L.; Thayer, Paul W.; Vinchur, Andrew J.; Salas, Eduardo (eds.). Historical perspectives in industrial and organizational psychology. Series in applied psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp.  353–382 (366). ISBN   978-0-8058-4440-5. OCLC   71725282.
  14. Hinsz, Verlin B. (2005). "The influences of social aspects of competition in goal-setting situations". Current Psychology. 24 (4): 258–273. doi:10.1007/s12144-005-1027-4. ISSN   0737-8262. S2CID   145134669.
  15. Locke, Edwin A.; Shaw, Karyll N.; Saari, Lise M.; Latham, Gary P. (1981). "Goal setting and task performance: 1969–1980". Psychological Bulletin . 90 (1): 125–152. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.1.125.
  16. Winters, Dawn; Latham, Gary P. (1996-06-01). "The effect of learning versus outcome goals on a simple versus a complex task". Group & Organization Management. 21 (2): 236–250. doi:10.1177/1059601196212007. ISSN   1059-6011. S2CID   144851262.
  17. Keith, Nina; Frese, Michael (July 2005). "Self-regulation in error management training: emotion control and metacognition as mediators of performance effects". Journal of Applied Psychology . 90 (4): 677–691. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.4.677. PMID   16060786.
  18. Cianci, A. M.; Klein, H. J.; Seijts, G. H. (2010). "The effect of negative feedback on tension and subsequent performance: The main and interactive effects of goal content and conscientiousness". Journal of Applied Psychology. 95 (4): 618–630. doi:10.1037/a0019130. PMID   20604585.
  19. Skinner, Natalie; Roche, Ann M.; O'Connor, John; Pollard, Yvette; Todd, Chelsea, eds. (2005). "Goal setting". Workforce development TIPS (theory into practice strategies): a resource kit for the alcohol and other drugs field. Adelaide: Alcohol Education and Rehabilitation Foundation (AER); National Centre for Education and Training on Addiction (Australia). pp. 8–9. ISBN   978-1-876897-06-2. OCLC   156766716.
  20. Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P. (July 2004). "What should we do about motivation theory? Six recommendations for the twenty-first century" (PDF). Academy of Management Review . 29 (3): 388–403. doi:10.5465/amr.2004.13670974.
  21. 1 2 3 Steel, Piers; König, Cornelius J. (October 2006). "Integrating theories of motivation" (PDF). Academy of Management Review . 31 (4): 889–913. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.196.3227 . doi:10.5465/AMR.2006.22527462. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-10-27. Retrieved 2015-02-06.
  22. Höchli, Bettina; Brügger, Adrian; Messner, Claude (2018). "How Focusing on Superordinate Goals Motivates Broad, Long-Term Goal Pursuit: A Theoretical Perspective". Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 1879. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01879 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   6176065 . PMID   30333781.
  23. Sitzmann, Traci; Ely, Katherine (2011). "A meta-analysis of self-regulated learning in work-related training and educational attainment: What we know and where we need to go". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (3): 421–442. doi:10.1037/a0022777. ISSN   1939-1455. PMID   21401218.
  24. "The OKR Origin Story: A closer look at the man who invented OKRs". What Matters. Retrieved 2021-04-03.
  25. "What is an OKR? Definition and examples". What Matters. Retrieved 2021-04-03.
  26. "re:Work - Guide: Set goals with OKRs". rework.withgoogle.com. Retrieved 2021-04-03.
  27. Doerr, John E. (2018). Measure what matters: how Google, Bono, and the Gates Foundation rock the world with OKRs. Larry Page. New York, New York. ISBN   978-0-525-53622-2. OCLC   1016349101.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  28. Bar-Eli, M.; Tenenbaum, G.; Pie, J. S.; Btesh, Y.; Almog, A. (April 1997). "Effect of goal difficulty, goal specificity and duration of practice time intervals on muscular endurance performance". Journal of Sports Sciences. 15 (2): 125–135. doi:10.1080/026404197367407. ISSN   0264-0414. PMID   9258843.
  29. Zhang, S (2021). "Foresee the glory and train better: Narcissism, goal-setting, and athlete training". Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology. 10 (3): 381–393. doi:10.1037/spy0000264. hdl: 10545/625686 . S2CID   233322951 . Retrieved 2021-04-08.
  30. "re:Work - Goal Setting". rework.withgoogle.com. Retrieved 2021-04-03.
  31. Bandura, Albert (March 1993). "Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning" (PDF). Educational Psychologist. 28 (2): 117–148. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3. S2CID   52256247.[ permanent dead link ]
  32. Goal-setting theory might define self-efficacy as an impression that one has the capability of performing in a certain manner or of attaining certain goals. Or one could define self-efficacy as a belief that one has the capabilities to execute the courses of actions required to manage prospective situations. Unlike efficacy (defined as the power to produce an effect—in essence, competence), self-efficacy consists of the belief (whether or not accurate) that one has the power to produce that effect. For example, a person with high self-efficacy may engage in more health-related activity when an illness occurs, whereas a person with low self efficacy may succumb to feelings of hopelessness. (Compare: Sue, David; Sue, Derald Wing; Sue, Stanley; Sue, Diane (2015). Understanding abnormal behavior (11th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. p. 194. ISBN   978-1-305-53760-6.) Note the distinction between self-esteem and self-efficacy. Self-esteem in this context relates to a person's sense of self-worth, whereas self-efficacy relates to a person's perception of their ability to reach a goal. For example, take the case of an incompetent rock-climber. Though probably afflicted with poor self-efficacy in regard to rock climbing, this hypothetical person could retain their self-esteem unaffected.
  33. Kishiki, Tomokazu; Lapin, Brittany; Tanaka, Ryota; Francis, Taylor; Hughes, Kathryn; Carbray, JoAnn; Ujiki, Michael B. (October 2016). "Goal setting results in improvement in surgical skills: a randomized controlled trial". Surgery . 160 (4): 1028–1037. doi:10.1016/j.surg.2016.07.022. PMID   27531316.
  34. Latham, Gary P. (November 2004). "The motivational benefits of goal-setting". Academy of Management Perspectives . 18 (4): 126–129. doi:10.5465/ame.2004.15268727.
  35. Copeland, Susan R.; Hughes, Carolyn (March 2002). "Effects of goal setting on task performance of persons with mental retardation". Education & Training in Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities. 37 (1): 40–54. JSTOR   23879582.
  36. "re:Work - Guide: Care professionally and personally for your team". rework.withgoogle.com. Retrieved 2021-03-31.
  37. Shalley, Christina E. (April 1995). "Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on creativity and productivity". Academy of Management Journal . 38 (2): 483–503 (501). JSTOR   256689.
  38. Morisano, Dominique; Hirsh, Jacob B.; Peterson, Jordan B.; Pihl, Robert O.; Shore, Bruce M. (2010). "Setting, elaborating, and reflecting on personal goals improves academic performance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 95 (2): 255–264. doi:10.1037/a0018478. ISSN   1939-1854. PMID   20230067.
  39. Travers, Cheryl J.; Morisano, Dominique; Locke, Edwin A. (June 2015). "Self-reflection, growth goals, and academic outcomes: A qualitative study". The British Journal of Educational Psychology. 85 (2): 224–241. doi:10.1111/bjep.12059. ISSN   2044-8279. PMID   25546509.
  40. Schippers, Michaéla C.; Morisano, Dominique; Locke, Edwin A.; Scheepers, Ad W.A.; Latham, Gary P.; De Jong, Elisabeth M. (2020-01-01). "Writing about personal goals and plans regardless of goal type boosts academic performance". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 60: 101823. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101823 . hdl: 1765/123006 . ISSN   0361-476X.
  41. Schippers, Michaéla C.; Scheepers, Ad W. A.; Peterson, Jordan B. (2015-06-09). "A scalable goal-setting intervention closes both the gender and ethnic minority achievement gap". Palgrave Communications. 1 (1): 1–12. doi: 10.1057/palcomms.2015.14 . ISSN   2055-1045.
  42. Travers, Cheryl J.; Morisano, Dominique; Locke, Edwin A. (June 2015). "Self-reflection, growth goals, and academic outcomes: A qualitative study". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 85 (2): 224–241. doi:10.1111/bjep.12059. PMID   25546509.
  43. Li, Kun; Johnsen, Justin; Canelas, Dorian A. (2021). "Persistence, performance, and goal setting in massive open online courses". British Journal of Educational Technology. 52 (3): 1215–1229. doi:10.1111/bjet.13068. ISSN   1467-8535. S2CID   233636341.
  44. Beckman, Karley; Apps, Tiffani; Bennett, Sue; Dalgarno, Barney; Kennedy, Gregor; Lockyer, Lori (2021-04-03). "Self-regulation in open-ended online assignment tasks: the importance of initial task interpretation and goal setting". Studies in Higher Education. 46 (4): 821–835. doi:10.1080/03075079.2019.1654450. ISSN   0307-5079. S2CID   202255323.
  45. Epton, T (2017). "Unique effects of setting goals on behavior change: Systematic review and meta-analysis". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 85 (12): 1182–1198. doi:10.1037/ccp0000260. hdl: 1893/25978 . PMID   29189034. S2CID   22124233 . Retrieved 2021-04-15.
  46. Bruhn, Allison L.; Mcdaniel, Sara C.; Fernando, Josephine; Troughton, Leonard (2016-02-01). "Goal-Setting Interventions for Students with Behavior Problems: A Systematic Review". Behavioral Disorders. 41 (2): 107–121. doi: 10.17988/0198-7429-41.2.107 . ISSN   0198-7429. S2CID   148116102.
  47. Kumm, Skip; Maggin, Daniel (2021-03-16). "Intensifying Goal-Setting Interventions for Students With Emotional and Behavioral Disorders". Beyond Behavior. 30: 14–23. doi:10.1177/1074295621996616. ISSN   1074-2956. S2CID   233279544.
  48. Bruhn, Allison Leigh; Fernando, Josephine; McDaniel, Sara; Troughton, Leonard (2017-08-01). "Putting Behavioral Goal-Setting Research Into Practice". Beyond Behavior. 26 (2): 66–73. doi:10.1177/1074295617711208. ISSN   1074-2956. S2CID   149195514.
  49. Davis, William E.; Kelley, Nicholas J.; Kim, Jinhyung; Tang, David; Hicks, Joshua A. (2015-12-10). "Motivating the academic mind: high-level construal of academic goals enhances goal meaningfulness, motivation, and self-concordance" (PDF). Motivation and Emotion. 40 (2): 193–202. doi:10.1007/s11031-015-9522-x. ISSN   0146-7239. S2CID   39475261. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-02-02. Retrieved 2016-05-13.
  50. Mouratidis, Athanasios; Lens, Willy (2015-09-29). "Adolescents' psychological functioning at school and in sports: the role of future time perspective and domain-specific and situation-specific self-determined motivation". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 34 (8): 643–673. doi:10.1521/jscp.2015.34.8.643. ISSN   0736-7236.
  51. Lens, Willy; Paixao, Maria Paula; Herrera, Dora; Grobler, Adelene (2012-05-12). "Future time perspective as a motivational variable: content and extension of future goals affect the quantity and quality of motivation". Japanese Psychological Research. 54 (3): 321–333. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00520.x . Retrieved 2016-05-13.
  52. Kaliski, Burton S, ed. (2007). Encyclopedia of business and finance (2nd ed.). Detroit: Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 733–735. ISBN   978-0-02-866061-5. OCLC   64084686.
  53. Corpuz, John (2016-05-04). "20 best productivity apps". Tom's Guide . Retrieved 2016-05-13.
  54. Duffy, Jill (2016-01-06). "The 25 best fitness apps for 2016". PC Magazine . Retrieved 2016-05-13.
  55. 1 2 "Study focuses on strategies for achieving goals, resolutions — Dominican University of California". www.dominican.edu. Archived from the original on 2020-01-15. Retrieved 2016-09-30.
  56. Schippers, Michaéla. "Ikigai: Reflection on life goals optimizes human performance and happiness" (PDF). Retrieved 3 April 2021.
  57. Nelson, S. Katherine; Layous, Kristin; Cole, Steven W.; Lyubomirsky, Sonja (September 2016). "Do unto others or treat yourself? The effects of prosocial and self-focused behavior on psychological flourishing". Emotion . 16 (6): 850–861. doi:10.1037/emo0000178. ISSN   1931-1516. PMID   27100366. S2CID   3964903.
  58. Fishman Keera N.; Ashbaugh Andrea R.; Swartz Richard H. (2021-02-01). "Goal Setting Improves Cognitive Performance in a Randomized Trial of Chronic Stroke Survivors". Stroke. 52 (2): 458–470. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.120.032131 . ISSN   0039-2499. PMID   33467876.
  59. Schweitzer, Maurice E.; Ordóñez, Lisa; Douma, Bambi (2004-06-01). "Goal setting as a motivator of unethical behavior". Academy of Management Journal . 47 (3): 422–432. ISSN   1948-0989. JSTOR   20159591. Archived from the original on 2015-10-09. Retrieved 2013-01-23. [...] people with unmet goals were more likely to engage in unethical behavior than people attempting to do their best. This relationship held for goals both with and without economic incentives. We also found that the relationship between goal setting and unethical behavior was particularly strong when people fell just short of reaching their goals.
  60. Niven, Karen; Healy, Colm (2015-01-21). "Susceptibility to the 'Dark Side' of Goal-Setting: Does Moral Justification Influence the Effect of Goals on Unethical Behavior?". Journal of Business Ethics . 127: 115–127.
  61. 1 2 3 Ordóñez, Lisa D.; Schweitzer, Maurice E.; Galinsky, Adam D.; Bazerman, Max H. (2009-02-01). "Goals Gone Wild: The Systematic Side Effects of Overprescribing Goal Setting". Academy of Management Perspectives. 23 (1): 6–16. doi:10.5465/amp.2009.37007999. ISSN   1558-9080.
  62. Welsh, David T.; Baer, Michael D.; Sessions, Hudson; Garud, Niharika (2020). "Motivated to disengage: The ethical consequences of goal commitment and moral disengagement in goal setting". Journal of Organizational Behavior. 41 (7): 663–677. doi:10.1002/job.2467. hdl: 11343/241454 . ISSN   1099-1379. S2CID   225646227.
  63. 1 2 3 4 5 Latham, Gary P. (2007). Work motivation: history, theory, research, and practice. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage Publications. pp. 149–150. ISBN   978-1-4416-5408-3. OCLC   639015669.
  64. 1 2 3 Grant, Anthony M. (September 2012). "An integrated model of goal-focused coaching: an evidence-based framework for teaching and practice" (PDF). International Coaching Psychology Review. 7 (2): 146–165 (151). doi:10.53841/bpsicpr.2012.7.2.146. S2CID   255938190. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-11-29. Learning goals (sometimes referred to as mastery goals) focus the coachee's attention on the learning associated with task mastery, rather than on the performance of the task itself. An example of a learning goal in executive or workplace coaching might be 'learn how to be the best lawyer in my area of practice'. Learning goals tend to be associated with a range of positive cognitive and emotional processes including perception of a complex task as a positive challenge rather than a threat, greater absorption in the actual task performance (Deci & Ryan, 2002), and enhanced memory and well-being (Linnenbrink, Ryan & Pintrich, 1999). Furthermore, individual performance can be enhanced in highly complex or challenging situations when team goals are primarily framed as being learning goals, and the use of team-level learning goals can foster enhanced co-operation between team members (Kristof-Brown & Stevens, 2001). One benefit of setting learning goals is that they tend to be associated with higher levels of intrinsic motivation which in turn is associated with performance (Sarrazin et al., 2002).
  65. 1 2 Kegan, Robert; Congleton, Christina; David, Susan A (2013). "The goals behind the goals: pursuing adult development in the coaching enterprise". In David, Susan A; Clutterbuck, David; Megginson, David (eds.). Beyond goals: effective strategies for coaching and mentoring. Farnham, Surrey: Gower Publishing Limited. pp. 229–244. ISBN   978-1-4094-1851-1. OCLC   828416668.
  66. Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly (1997). Finding flow: the psychology of engagement with everyday life . New York: Basic Books. ISBN   978-0-465-04513-6. OCLC   36315862.
  67. Kehr, H. M. (2019). "Goal setting theory—Firmly entrenched, but narrow in its focus". Motivation Science. 5 (2): 110–111. doi:10.1037/mot0000132. S2CID   191726678 . Retrieved 2021-06-09.
  68. Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P. (September 2002). "Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey". American Psychologist. 57 (9): 708. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.705. ISSN   1935-990X. PMID   12237980. S2CID   17534210.
  69. Bandura, Albert; Cervone, Daniel (1986-08-01). "Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 38 (1): 92–113. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(86)90028-2. ISSN   0749-5978. S2CID   3775458.
  70. Simons & Chabris (1999). "Gorillas in our Midst" (PDF). Retrieved 2018-09-26.
  71. Advances in motivation science. Volume 2. Andrew J. Elliot. Waltham, MA. 2015. ISBN   978-0-12-802469-0. OCLC   918944235.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) CS1 maint: others (link)
  72. 1 2 Locke, Edwin A. (2013-01-03). New Developments in Goal Setting and Task Performance (1 ed.). Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203082744. ISBN   978-0-203-08274-4.
  73. 1 2 Seijts, Gerard H.; Latham, Gary P. (February 2005). "Learning versus performance goals: When should each be used?". Academy of Management Perspectives. 19 (1): 124–131. doi:10.5465/ame.2005.15841964. ISSN   1558-9080.
  74. 1 2 Seijts, G. H.; Latham, G. P.; Tasa, K.; Latham, B. W. (2004-04-01). "Goal setting and goal orientation: an integration of two different yet related literatures". Academy of Management Journal. 47 (2): 227–239. ISSN   0001-4273. JSTOR   20159574.
  75. Drach-Zahavy, Anat; Erez, Miriam (2002-07-01). "Challenge versus threat effects on the goal–performance relationship". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 88 (2): 667–682. doi:10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00004-3. ISSN   0749-5978.
  76. Fiorella, Logan (2020-09-01). "The Science of Habit and Its Implications for Student Learning and Well-being". Educational Psychology Review. 32 (3): 603–625. doi:10.1007/s10648-020-09525-1. ISSN   1573-336X. S2CID   216460117.
  77. Vancouver, Jeffrey B.; Schmitt, Neal W. (2006-12-07). "An exploratory examination of person–organization fit: organizational goal congruence". Personnel Psychology. 44 (2): 333–352. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00962.x. ISSN   0031-5826.
  78. Hutchison, Steven; Garstka, Mary L. (August 1996). "Sources of perceived organizational support: goal setting and feedback". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 26 (15): 1351–1366. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1996.tb00075.x. ISSN   0021-9029.
  79. Kleingeld, Ad; van Mierlo, Heleen; Arends, Lidia (November 2011). "The effect of goal setting on group performance: a meta-analysis". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 96 (6): 1289–1304. doi:10.1037/a0024315. ISSN   1939-1854. PMID   21744940. S2CID   22191705.
  80. 1 2 Stock, Jennifer; Cervone, Daniel (1990-10-01). "Proximal goal-setting and self-regulatory processes". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 14 (5): 483–498. doi:10.1007/BF01172969. ISSN   1573-2819. S2CID   30258565.
  81. "What are Sub-Goals - How to Use Them for Your Advantage | BLEND". Localization Services by BLEND. 2020-01-11. Retrieved 2021-10-24.
  82. Lee, Felissa K.; Sheldon, Kennon M.; Turban, Daniel B. (April 2003). "Personality and the goal-striving process: the influence of achievement goal patterns, goal level, and mental focus on performance and enjoyment". Journal of Applied Psychology . 88 (2): 256–265. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.2.256. PMID   12731709. S2CID   7832798.
  83. Chen, Xiao; Latham, Gary P.; Piccolo, Ronald F.; Itzchakov, Guy (2021). "An Enumerative Review and a Meta-Analysis of Primed Goal Effects on Organizational Behavior". Applied Psychology. 70 (1): 216–253. doi:10.1111/apps.12239. ISSN   1464-0597. S2CID   214187073.
  84. Albarracin, Dolores; Hepler, Justin; Tannenbaum, Melanie (2011-04-01). "General Action and Inaction Goals: Their Behavioral, Cognitive, and Affective Origins and Influences". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20 (2): 119–123. doi:10.1177/0963721411402666. ISSN   0963-7214. PMC   3678837 . PMID   23766569.
  85. Gupta, Rakesh (May 19, 2021). Education Technology in Physical Education and Sports. India: Friends Publications (India). p. 31. ISBN   978-93-90649-80-8.
  86. Hepler, Justin; Albarracin, Dolores; McCulloch, Kathleen C.; Noguchi, Kenji (December 2012). "Being active and impulsive: the role of goals for action and inaction in self-control". Motivation and Emotion. 36 (4): 416–424. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9263-4. PMC   3678776 . PMID   23766548.
  87. Zell, Ethan; Su, Rong; Li, Hong; Ho, Moon-Ho Ringo; Hong, Sungjin; Kumkale, Tarcan; Stauffer, Sarah D.; Zecca, Gregory; Cai, Huajian; Roccas, Sonia (September 2013). "Cultural differences in attitudes toward action and inaction: the role of dialecticism". Social Psychological and Personality Science . 4 (5): 521–528. doi:10.1177/1948550612468774. PMC   6103533 . PMID   30147848.
  88. Byrne, Ruth M. J.; McEleney, Alice (September 2000). "Counterfactual thinking about actions and failures to act" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition . 26 (5): 1318–1331. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1318. PMID   11009260.
  89. Roese, Neal J.; Hur, Taekyun; Pennington, Ginger L. (December 1999). "Counterfactual thinking and regulatory focus: implications for action versus inaction and sufficiency versus necessity". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 77 (6): 1109–1120. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1109. PMID   10626366.
  90. Wilson, Timothy D.; Reinhard, David A.; Westgate, Erin C.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Ellerbeck, Nicole; Hahn, Cheryl; Brown, Casey L.; Shaked, Adi (July 2014). "Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind". Science. 345 (6192): 75–77. Bibcode:2014Sci...345...75W. doi:10.1126/science.1250830. PMC   4330241 . PMID   24994650.
  91. Masuda, Aline D.; Locke, Edwin A.; Williams, Kevin J. (2015-01-02). "The effects of simultaneous learning and performance goals on performance: An inductive exploration". Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 27 (1): 37–52. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.982128. ISSN   2044-5911. S2CID   143855907.
  92. Latham, Gary P.; Erez, Miriam; Locke, Edwin A. (1988). "Resolving scientific disputes by the joint design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the Erez–Latham dispute regarding participation in goal setting". Journal of Applied Psychology. 73 (4): 753–772. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.73.4.753. ISSN   1939-1854.
  93. Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P.; Erez, Miriam (January 1988). "The Determinants of Goal Commitment". Academy of Management Review. 13 (1): 23–39. doi:10.5465/amr.1988.4306771. ISSN   0363-7425.
  94. Goals Gone Wild: The Systematic Side of Overprescribing Goal Setting
  95. Goals Gone Wild: The Systematic Side of Overprescribing Goal Setting Lisa D. Ordo ́n ̃ez, Maurice E. Schweitzer, Adam D. Galinsky, and Max H. Bazerman
  96. 1 2 Locke, Edwin A.; Latham, Gary P. (February 2009). "Has Goal Setting Gone Wild, or Have Its Attackers Abandoned Good Scholarship?". Academy of Management Perspectives. 23 (1): 17–23. doi:10.5465/amp.2009.37008000. ISSN   1558-9080.
  97. Ordonez, Lisa D.; Schweitzer, Maurice E.; Galinsky, Adam D.; Bazerman, Max H. (2009). "On Good Scholarship, Goal Setting, and Scholars Gone Wild". SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.1382000. ISSN   1556-5068. S2CID   233760637.
  98. Benzer, Justin K.; Creech, Suzannah K.; Mohr, David C.; Charns, Martin P. (December 2014). "Learning Goals May Prevent "Goals Gone Wild"". American Journal of Public Health. 104 (12): e1. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2014.302264. ISSN   0090-0036. PMC   4232136 . PMID   25320875.
  99. Lisa D. Ordóñez, Maurice E. Schweitzer, Adam D. Galinsky, and Max H. Bazerman (2009). "Goals Gone Wild: The Systematic Side Effects of Overprescribing Goal Setting." Academy of Management Perspectives. Retrieved from Goals gone wild.pdf.