Competition model

Last updated

The Competition Model is a psycholinguistic theory of language acquisition and sentence processing, developed by Elizabeth Bates and Brian MacWhinney (1982). [1] The claim in MacWhinney, Bates, and Kliegl (1984) [2] is that "the forms of natural languages are created, governed, constrained, acquired, and used in the service of communicative functions." Furthermore, the model holds that processing is based on an online competition between these communicative functions or motives. The model focuses on competition during sentence processing, crosslinguistic competition in bilingualism, and the role of competition in language acquisition. It is an emergentist theory of language acquisition and processing, serving as an alternative to strict innatist and empiricist theories. According to the Competition Model, patterns in language arise from Darwinian competition and selection on a variety of time/process scales including phylogenetic, ontogenetic, social diffusion, and synchronic scales.

Contents

The Classic Competition Model

The classic version of the model focused on competition during sentence processing, crosslinguistic competition in bilingualism, and the role of competition in language acquisition.

Sentence Processing

The Competition Model was initially proposed as a theory of cross-linguistic sentence processing. [3] The model suggests that people interpret the meaning of a sentence by taking into account various linguistic cues contained in the sentence context, such as word order, morphology, and semantic characteristics (e.g., animacy), to compute a probabilistic value for each interpretation, eventually choosing the interpretation with the highest likelihood. According to the model, cue weights are learned inductively on the basis of the extent to which the cues are available and reliable guides to meanings in comprehension and to forms in production.

Because different languages use different cues to signal meanings, the Competition Model maintains that cue weights will differ between languages, and users of a given language will use the cue weights associated with that language, to guide their interpretation of sentences. Thus, when people learn other languages, they must learn which cues are important in which languages, in order to successfully interpret sentences in any language. The model defines a cue as an information source present in the surface structure of utterances that allows the language user to link linguistic form with meaning or function. Cues vary in their type (morphological, syntactic, prosodic, semantic, and pragmatic), availability (how often they are present), and reliability (how often they lead to the correct interpretation). Each cue has a certain level of cue validity, the joint product of availability and reliability. Cues of the same basic type, such as case-marking, animacy, or word order may have markedly different levels of validity in different languages. For example, the cue of animacy plays a minimal role in English, but a major role in Italian. [2]

The model holds that cues both compete and cooperate during processing. Sometimes cues cooperate or converge by pointing to the same interpretation or production. Sometimes, cues compete by pointing to conflicting interpretations or productions.

Language Acquisition

The application of the model to child language acquisition focuses on the role that cue availability and reliability play in determining the order of acquisition of grammatical structures. The basic finding is that children first learn the most available cue(s) in their language. [4] If the most available cue is not also the most reliable, then children slowly shift from depending on the available cue to depending on the more reliable cue.

Methods

The Competition Model implies that language emergence on the developmental or ontogenetic timescale can be examined in at least two ways. One methodology uses neural network models [5] to simulate the acquisition of detailed grammatical structures. Competition model researchers have constructed connectionist models for the acquisition of morphology, syntax, and lexicon in several languages, including English, [6] German, [7] and Hungarian. [8] In addition, the ontogenetic emergence of language has been examined from a biological viewpoint, using data on language processing from children with early focal lesions. [9] The results of studies of these children using reaction time methodologies and neuropsychological tests indicate that, although they have completely normal functional use of language, detailed aspects of processing are slower in some cases. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging technology, areas of neurological activation involved in specific linguistic tasks have been pinpointed in these children. These results have allowed researchers to evaluate a series of hypotheses regarding sensitive periods for the emergence of language in the brain.

The Unified Competition Model

The classic Competition Model accounts well for many of the basic features of sentence processing and cue learning. It relies on a small set of assumptions regarding cues, validity, reliability, competition, transfer, and strength—each of which could be investigated directly.  However, the model is limited in several important ways.

·       Brain Structure: The classic model makes no contact with what we now know about the organization of language in the brain. As a result, it provides only incomplete understanding of patterns of language disorder and loss.

·       Critical Period: The classic model fails to come to grips with the idea that there is a biologically-determined critical period for language acquisition.

·       Motivation: The classic model provides no role for social and motivational factors governing language learning, preference, code-switching, and attrition.

·       Mental Models: The classic model fails to include a role for mental model construction during comprehension and formulation during production.

·       Microgenesis: The classic model does not provide a microgenetic account for the course of item acquisition, fluency development, and cue strength learning.

Extending the classic model to deal with these challenges involves borrowing insights from related theories. The resultant broader theory is called the Unified Competition Model or UCM, [10] because it seeks to unify a variety of independent theoretical frameworks into a single overall model. The transition from the classic version of the model to the unified version worked to bring the model into fuller accord with the theory of emergentism, [11] as developed in the biological (West-Eberhard, 2003), [12] social (Kontopoulos, 1993) [13] and physical sciences (von Bertalanffy, 1968). [14]

Unifying the L1 and L2 Learning Models

A major challenge facing an emergentist, functionalist, non-nativist model such as the UCM involves dealing with age-related changes in the outcome of second language (L2) acquisition.  It is widely accepted that children end up acquiring a second language more completely than adults. One account proposes that this "fundamental difference" (Bley-Vroman, 2009) [15] between child and adult L2 learning arises from the expiration of a biologically-based critical period for natural language learning. In contrast, the framework of the Competition Model emphasizes that all forms of language acquisition make use of the same set of cognitive and social processes, although they differ in the relative reliance on specific processes and the extent to which these processes interact with other learning.

Specifically, the UCM [10] holds that adults are more challenged than children by a set of four risk factors that can impede L2 acquisition.

  1. The entrenchment of first language (L1) patterns (Schmid, 2017) [16] leads to competition with L2 patterns. The role of entrenchment and competition as shaping adult L2 performance was already a major features of the classic Competition Model (Bates & MacWhinney, 1981; McDonald, 1989). [17] It is important to consider that entrenchment is fundamental property of neural network functioning (Zevin, 2012), [18] rather than a species-specific genetic mechanism of the type involved in critical periods.
  2. Adults rely heavily on transfer of patterns from L1 to L2. This leads to quick initial learning which then fails to accurately acquire L2 patterns. In addition to these negative effects of transfer, adult reliance on translation from L1 creates a parasitic relation of L2 on L1 (Kroll, Van Hell, Tokowicz, & Green, 2010). [19]
  3. When acquiring new words, adults tend to apply overanalysis by isolating content words in phrases without regard to inflections and function words.  Children, on the other hand, are more likely to learn language by acquiring words as parts of larger chunks.
  4. Adults may be subject to isolation from interactions with the L2 community.

Adults can counterbalance these four risk factors through an emphasis on four protective or preventive factors.

  1. Adults can learn and consolidate new forms through an emphasis on resonance – the process of creating meaningful links between L2 forms (Schlichting & Preston, 2015). [20]
  2. Adults can bring themselves to think in L2 (Vygotsky, 1934). [21]   This process of internalization leads to further strengthening of links between L2 patterns, producing greater fluency.
  3. When learning new forms and combinations, adults can emphasize chunking of large phrasal units.
  4. Adults can avoid social isolation and maximize participation by reading L2 materials, watching L2 programs, and socializing with L2 groups (Firth & Wagner, 2007). [22]

All of these processes can impact both children and adults. What differs across age is the relative social status of the person and the degree to which they have already consolidated L1.

Three Components of Emergentism

The account given above for the effects of age on L2 learning emphasizes the role of competition as formulated in the classic version of the model. However, we know that there are variations in the learning of different types of linguistic structures. For example, although adults outperform children in terms of learning of the L2 lexicon, [23] they encounter significantly more problems in acquiring a nativelike L2 pronunciation. [24] To understand these differences, we need to think about language in terms of its component structural levels. Emergentist theory emphasizes three major dimensions that control physical, biological, and social processes.  These are competition, structural levels, and time/process frames.  The classic version of the Competition Model describes and quantifies the role of competition in language. However, in a fuller emergentist account, this analysis of competition must be supplemented through an analysis of structural levels and time/process scales.

Structural Analysis

Structural linguistic analysis (Harris, 1951) [25] distinguishes the levels of input phonology, output phonology, lexicon, semantics, morphology, syntax, mental models, and interaction. Processing on these levels can be analyzed in terms of the related theories of statistical learning (input phonology), gating and fluency [26] (output phonology), embodied cognition and hub-and-spoke theory [27] (semantics), DevLex [5] (lexicon), item-based patterns [28] (syntax), perspective theory [29] [30] (mental models), and CA theory [31] (interaction). The theories for lexicon, syntax, and mental models have been elaborated in specific ways that help unify the approach. These elaborations include specifically the theory of item-based patterns and the theory of perspective shifting.

Item-based Patterns

The theory of item-based patterns [28] is based on ideas about positional patterns from Braine (1962, 1976) [32] [33] that were modified by MacWhinney to restrict early patterns to individual lexical items. The theory holds that children learn item-based patterns to combine words by focusing on individual operators, such as "my" or "more". They learn these patterns as a part of their learning about the function of these operator words. They then generalize groups of item-based patterns to abstract feature-based patterns such as the one that places adjectives before nouns in English. In this way, the theory of item-based patterns can be viewed as an early instantiation of Construction Grammar, [34] albeit one specifically designed to account for child language learning. Sentence processing involves the repeated filling (or merge [35] or unification [36] ) of roles specified by the item-based and feature-based patterns.

Mental Models and Perspective

The linked dependency structure provide by the operation of item-based and feature-based patterns serves as the input to mental model construction. On this level, sentences encode agency, causation, reference, and space-time through a process of perspective taking. [29] This process allows the human mind to construct an ongoing cognitive simulation of the meaning of an utterance coded in linguistic abstractions, through the use of perceptual realities derived from one's embodied experience. [30] The perspective taking approach views the forms of grammar as emerging from repeated acts of perspective taking and perspective switching during online language comprehension. Grammatical devices such as pronouns, case, voice, and attachment can all be seen as ways of expressing shifts in a basically ego-centered perspective. As noted by Chafe (1994) [37] and MacWhinney (1977, 2008), perspective-taking and perspective-shifting play a central role in linking together models of the actions of agents, referents, positions in space-time, and causation (Talmy, 2000). Articulation of the links between the theory of perspective and the classic Competition Model depends on examination of online cue processing effects, as illustrated in studies such as McDonald and MacWhinney (1995) [38] which examined how verb-based implicit causality established mental models that influence anaphoric binding. One major goal in this line of research is to better understand the brain mechanisms underlying perspective shifting during language comprehension.

Complexity

Complexity arises from the hierarchical recombination of small parts into larger structures (Simon 1962). [39] For language, the smallest parts are the articulatory commands of output phonology, the auditory features of input phonology, and the perceptual features underlying semantics. These articulatory, auditory, and perceptual patterns combine into words that combine into phrases that combine into mental models that compose interactions and narratives. Within each of these major structural levels, we can distinguish additional substructures. Within phonology, words are structured into tone groups composed of syllables that are composed of onsets, nuclei, and codas, which control clusters of articulatory gestures. Within the lexicon, morphemes can be combined into compounds, phrases, inflected forms, and derivations. Syntactic patterns can be coded at the most elementary level in terms of item-based patterns, [28] which are then grouped on the next level of abstraction into constructions, and eventually general syntactic patterns. Mental models are based on an interlocking system emerging from the levels of role assignment, space-time configuration, causal relations, and perspective taking. [30]

The levels distinguished by structural analysis are richly interconnected. This means that, although they are partially decomposable (Simon, 1962), [39] they are not modular in the sense of Fodor (1983), [40] but rather interactive in the sense of Rumelhart and McClelland (1987). [41] In order to achieve gating and activation, processing levels must be interconnected in a way that permits smooth coordination. The UCM assumes that these interconnections rely on methods for topological, i.e. tonotopic (Wessinger, Buonocore, Kussmaul, & Mangun, 1997) [42] or somatotopic (Hauk, Johnsrude, & Pulvermuller, 2004), [43] organization that are used throughout the cortex.

Structural analysis has many important consequences for our understanding of relations between first and second language learning. Age-related first language entrenchment operates in very different ways in different cortical areas (Werker & Hensch, 2014). [44] In second language production, contrasts and timing relations between the levels of conceptualization, formulation, and articulation (Levelt, 1989) [45] produce marked effects on language performance (Skehan, 2009), [46] although similar effects can be found also in first language acquisition (Snow, 1999). [47] The details of this analysis can be found in MacWhinney (2017). [10]

Time/Process Scales

The third component of emergentist analysis (after competition and structural levels) is the theory of time/process scales. Emergentist theory holds that structures emerge from the constraints arising within time/process frames. The operation of time/process frames and their constraints can be illustrated by looking at how a set of four structural levels determine the shape of proteins (Campbell, Reece, & Mitchell, 1999). [48] During protein folding, the primary structure of the protein is constrained by the sequence of amino acids in the chain of RNA used by the ribosome as the template for protein synthesis. This sequence conveys a code shaped by evolution; but the physical shape of a specific protein is determined by processes operating after initial RNA transcription. The first partially folded structure to emerge is the secondary structure of coils and folds created by the imposition of constraints from hydrogen bonding across the amino acid chain. These forces can only impact the geometry of the protein once the primary structure emerges from the ribosome and begins to contract. After these second structures have formed, a tertiary structure arises from constraints imposed by hydrophobic reactions and disulfide bridges across the folds and coils of the secondary structures. Finally, the quaternary structure derives from the aggregation of polypeptide subunits based on the ternary structures. It is this final structure that allows each protein to serve its unique role, be it oxygen transport for hemoglobin or antigen detection for antibodies. In this partially decomposable emergent system, each level involves a configuration of components from lower levels, but the biochemical constraints operative on each level are unique to that level and only operate once that level has emerged during the process of folding.

The fully emergentist version of the UCM emphasizes the ways in which language learning and processing is constrained by processes operating at divergent structural levels in divergent time/process frames (MacWhinney, 2015a). To account for patterns of disfluency, stuttering, code-switching, and conversational sequencing, we look at the constraints imposed by online lexical access, patterns of cortical activation, and social affiliation. Some of these constraints operate across a timeframe of milliseconds and others, like social affiliation, extend across decades. The UCM distinguishes five major groups of time/process frames: phylogenetic drift in the species, diachronic change through social diffusion in the language, developmental change across the lifespan, memory consolidation across days, and frames for online processing and conversation in the moment. Within each of these five major time/process frame groups there are dozens of component processes working to constrain the shape of language.

The importance of timeframes is illustrated by a particularly puzzling aspect of language learning, which is the way in which young immigrant children can completely forget their first language (Ventureyra, Pallier, & Yoo, 2004). [49] These cases of complete language attrition contrasts with the minimal level of language attrition experienced by older immigrants (MacWhinney, 2018). [50] It is difficult to account for this in terms of entrenchment alone, because the young learners have used their L1 continually for as much as 6 years. Instead one needs to look at the role of social support for young immigrants, pressures to adopt L2, and the role of literacy in helping older learners consolidate their access to L1 forms. Understanding the interactions of these processes requires a model that can bring all of these forces and timeframes together in terms of competition, motivation, and consolidation. Elaborating these effects is now a major emphasis for ongoing UCM research.

Further Development of the Competition Model

The classic version of the Competition Model emphasized the ways in which cue reliability shaped cue strength. These effects were measured in highly structured sentence processing experiments. To address certain limitations of this research, the Unified Competition Model sought to account in greater detail for age-related facts in the comparison between child and adult second language learning. Within the classic model, the only mechanism that could account for these effects was competition between L1 and L2 patterns, as expressed through negative transfer.  Although transfer plays a major role as a risk factor for difficulties in adult L2 learning, it is not the only risk factor.

Looking more closely at the variety of L2 learning outcomes across structural levels and timeframes, it became evident that we needed to construct a more complex account for variable outcomes in L2 learning. This account required a deeper integration of emergentist theory into the UCM framework. The resultant account is now able to address each of the limitations of the classic model mentioned earlier. Specifically,

·       by linking linguistic structures to particular brain regions, the model is increasingly grounded neurolinguistically (MacWhinney, 2019), [51]

·       by delineating a set of risk and protective factors, the model deals more accurately with age-related patterns in L2 learning,

·       by providing a time/process frames account of social and motivational factors, the model accounts better for variation in L2 outcome by social groups, work environments, as well as providing accounts for patterns of code-switching and language attrition,

·       by linking in the theory of perspective-switching, we have a fuller understanding of online sentence processing, and

·       by developing corpus (MacWhinney, 2019) [52] and online experimental (eCALL) methods (MacWhinney, 2017), [53] the model now provides a fuller microgenetic account of the growth of fluency .

By addressing each of these issues within the context of analyses of L2 learning, the current version of the UCM allows us to better understand not only L2 learning, but also language evolution (MacWhinney, 2005), [54] language change, child language development (MacWhinney, 2015), [55] language disorders (Presson & MacWhinney, 2011), [56] and language attrition (MacWhinney, 2018). [50]

See also

Related Research Articles

Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language, as well as to produce and use words and sentences to communicate.

Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are considered as psychologically real, and research in cognitive linguistics aims to help understand cognition in general and is seen as a road into the human mind.

A second language (L2) is a language spoken in addition to one's first language (L1). A second language may be a neighbouring language, another language of the speaker's home country, or a foreign language. A speaker's dominant language, which is the language a speaker uses most or is most comfortable with, is not necessarily the speaker's first language. For example, the Canadian census defines first language for its purposes as "the first language learned in childhood and still spoken", recognizing that for some, the earliest language may be lost, a process known as language attrition. This can happen when young children start school or move to a new language environment.

Elizabeth Ann Bates was a professor of cognitive science at the University of California, San Diego. She was an internationally renowned expert and leading researcher in child language acquisition, psycholinguistics, aphasia, and the neurological bases of language, and she authored 10 books and over 200 peer-reviewed articles and book chapters on these subjects. Bates was well known for her assertion that linguistic knowledge is distributed throughout the brain and is subserved by general cognitive and neurological processes.

Language transfer is the application of linguistic features from one language to another by a bilingual or multilingual speaker. Language transfer may occur across both languages in the acquisition of a simultaneous bilingual, from a mature speaker's first language (L1) to a second language (L2) they are acquiring, or from an L2 back to the L1. Language transfer is most commonly discussed in the context of English language learning and teaching, but it can occur in any situation when someone does not have a native-level command of a language, as when translating into a second language. Language transfer is also a common topic in bilingual child language acquisition as it occurs frequently in bilingual children especially when one language is dominant.

Second-language acquisition (SLA), sometimes called second-language learning — otherwise referred to as L2acquisition, is the process by which people learn a second language. Second-language acquisition is also the scientific discipline devoted to studying that process. The field of second-language acquisition is regarded by some but not everybody as a sub-discipline of applied linguistics but also receives research attention from a variety of other disciplines, such as psychology and education.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Vocabulary development</span> Process of learning words

Vocabulary development is a process by which people acquire words. Babbling shifts towards meaningful speech as infants grow and produce their first words around the age of one year. In early word learning, infants build their vocabulary slowly. By the age of 18 months, infants can typically produce about 50 words and begin to make word combinations.

The desire to learn is often related to the concept of ‘motivation’. Motivation is the most used concept for explaining the failure or success of a language learner. Second language (L2) refers to a language an individual learns that is not his/her mother tongue, but is of use in the area of the individual. It is not the same as a foreign language, which is a language learned that is not generally spoken in the individual's area. In research on motivation, it is considered to be an internal process that gives behavior energy, direction and persistence in research . Learning a new language takes time and dedication. Once you do, being fluent in a second language offers numerous benefits and opportunities. Learning a second language is exciting and beneficial at all ages. It offers practical, intellectual and many aspirational benefits. In learning a language, there can be one or more goals – such as mastery of the language or communicative competence – that vary from person to person. There are a number of language learner motivation models that were developed and postulated in fields such as linguistics and sociolinguistics, with relations to second-language acquisition in a classroom setting. The different perspectives on L2 motivation can be divided into three distinct phases: the social psychological period, the cognitive-situated period and the process-oriented period.

Brian James MacWhinney is a Professor of Psychology and Modern Languages at Carnegie Mellon University. He specializes in first and second language acquisition, psycholinguistics, and the neurological bases of language, and he has written and edited several books and over 100 peer-reviewed articles and book chapters on these subjects. MacWhinney is best known for his competition model of language acquisition and for creating the CHILDES and TalkBank corpora. He has also helped to develop a stream of pioneering software programs for creating and running psychological experiments, including PsyScope, an experimental control system for the Macintosh; E-Prime, an experimental control system for the Microsoft Windows platform; and System for Teaching Experimental Psychology (STEP), a database of scripts for facilitating and improving psychological and linguistic research.

Artificial grammar learning (AGL) is a paradigm of study within cognitive psychology and linguistics. Its goal is to investigate the processes that underlie human language learning by testing subjects' ability to learn a made-up grammar in a laboratory setting. It was developed to evaluate the processes of human language learning but has also been utilized to study implicit learning in a more general sense. The area of interest is typically the subjects' ability to detect patterns and statistical regularities during a training phase and then use their new knowledge of those patterns in a testing phase. The testing phase can either use the symbols or sounds used in the training phase or transfer the patterns to another set of symbols or sounds as surface structure.

The generative approach to second language (L2) acquisition (SLA) is a cognitive based theory of SLA that applies theoretical insights developed from within generative linguistics to investigate how second languages and dialects are acquired and lost by individuals learning naturalistically or with formal instruction in foreign, second language and lingua franca settings. Central to generative linguistics is the concept of Universal Grammar (UG), a part of an innate, biologically endowed language faculty which refers to knowledge alleged to be common to all human languages. UG includes both invariant principles as well as parameters that allow for variation which place limitations on the form and operations of grammar. Subsequently, research within the Generative Second-Language Acquisition (GenSLA) tradition describes and explains SLA by probing the interplay between Universal Grammar, knowledge of one's native language and input from the target language. Research is conducted in syntax, phonology, morphology, phonetics, semantics, and has some relevant applications to pragmatics.

Bootstrapping is a term used in language acquisition in the field of linguistics. It refers to the idea that humans are born innately equipped with a mental faculty that forms the basis of language. It is this language faculty that allows children to effortlessly acquire language. As a process, bootstrapping can be divided into different domains, according to whether it involves semantic bootstrapping, syntactic bootstrapping, prosodic bootstrapping, or pragmatic bootstrapping.

The critical period hypothesis or sensitive period hypothesis claims that there is an ideal time window of brain development to acquire language in a linguistically rich environment, after which further language acquisition becomes much more difficult and effortful. It is the subject of a long-standing debate in linguistics and language acquisition over the extent to which the ability to acquire language is biologically linked to age. The critical period hypothesis was first proposed by Montreal neurologist Wilder Penfield and co-author Lamar Roberts in their 1959 book Speech and Brain Mechanisms, and was popularized by Eric Lenneberg in 1967 with Biological Foundations of Language.

In linguistics, grammaticality is determined by the conformity to language usage as derived by the grammar of a particular speech variety. The notion of grammaticality rose alongside the theory of generative grammar, the goal of which is to formulate rules that define well-formed, grammatical, sentences. These rules of grammaticality also provide explanations of ill-formed, ungrammatical sentences.

Social interactionist theory (SIT) is an explanation of language development emphasizing the role of social interaction between the developing child and linguistically knowledgeable adults. It is based largely on the socio-cultural theories of Soviet psychologist, Lev Vygotsky.

The order of acquisition is a concept in language acquisition describing the specific order in which all language learners acquire the grammatical features of their first language. This concept is based on the observation that all children acquire their first language in a fixed, universal order, regardless of the specific grammatical structure of the language they learn. Linguistic research has largely confirmed that this phenomenon is true for first-language learners; order of acquisition for second-language learners is much less consistent. It is not clear why the order differs for second-language learners, though current research suggests this variability may stem from first-language interference or general cognitive interference from nonlinguistic mental faculties.

The main purpose of theories of second-language acquisition (SLA) is to shed light on how people who already know one language learn a second language. The field of second-language acquisition involves various contributions, such as linguistics, sociolinguistics, psychology, cognitive science, neuroscience, and education. These multiple fields in second-language acquisition can be grouped as four major research strands: (a) linguistic dimensions of SLA, (b) cognitive dimensions of SLA, (c) socio-cultural dimensions of SLA, and (d) instructional dimensions of SLA. While the orientation of each research strand is distinct, they are in common in that they can guide us to find helpful condition to facilitate successful language learning. Acknowledging the contributions of each perspective and the interdisciplinarity between each field, more and more second language researchers are now trying to have a bigger lens on examining the complexities of second language acquisition.

The mental lexicon is defined as a mental dictionary that contains information regarding the word store of a language user, such as their meanings, pronunciations, and syntactic characteristics. The mental lexicon is used in linguistics and psycholinguistics to refer to individual speakers' lexical, or word, representations. However, there is some disagreement as to the utility of the mental lexicon as a scientific construct.

Statistical language acquisition, a branch of developmental psycholinguistics, studies the process by which humans develop the ability to perceive, produce, comprehend, and communicate with natural language in all of its aspects through the use of general learning mechanisms operating on statistical patterns in the linguistic input. Statistical learning acquisition claims that infants' language-learning is based on pattern perception rather than an innate biological grammar. Several statistical elements such as frequency of words, frequent frames, phonotactic patterns and other regularities provide information on language structure and meaning for facilitation of language acquisition.

The following outline is provided as an overview of and topical guide to second-language acquisition:

References

  1. Bates, Elizabeth; MacWhinney, Brian (1982). "Functionalist approaches to grammar". Language Acquisition: The State of the Art.
  2. 1 2 MacWhinney, Brian; Bates, Elizabeth; Kliegl, Reinhold (1984). "Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 23 (2): 127–150. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90093-8.
  3. MacWhinney, Brian; Elizabeth Bates (1989). The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   0-521-26196-1.
  4. Bates, Elizabeth (1984). "A crosslinguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies". Child Development. 55 (2): 341–354. doi:10.2307/1129947. JSTOR   1129947. PMID   6723440.
  5. 1 2 Li, Ping; Zhao, X.; MacWhinney, Brian (2007). "Dynamic self-organization and early lexical development in children". Cognitive Science. 31 (4): 581–612. doi:10.1080/15326900701399905. PMC   9808815 . PMID   21635309.
  6. MacWhinney, Brian; Leinbach, Jared (1991). "Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model". Cognition. 29 (1–2): 121–157. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(91)90048-9. PMID   1786671. S2CID   16291062.
  7. Taraban, Roman; McDonald, Janet; MacWhinney, Brian (1989). "Category learning in a connectionist model: Learning to decline the German definite article". Linguistic Categorization. doi:10.1075/cilt.61.13tar.
  8. MacWhinney, Brian; Pléh, Csaba (1985). "The development of sentence interpretation in Hungarian". Cognitive Psychology. 17 (2): 178–209. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(85)90007-6. S2CID   16022134.
  9. MacWhinney, Brian; Feldman, Heidi; Sacco, Kelly; Valdez-Perez, Raul (2000). "Online measures of basic language skills in children with early focal brain lesions". Brain and Language. 71 (3): 400–431. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2273. PMID   10716870. S2CID   25602984.
  10. 1 2 3 MacWhinney, Brian (2017). "A unified model of first and second language learning". Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition.
  11. MacWhinney, Brian (2015). "Emergentism". Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics: 689–706. doi:10.1515/9783110292022-035. ISBN   978-3-11-029184-1.
  12. West-Eberhard, Mary Jane (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  13. Kontopoulos, Kyriakos. The logics of social structure. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  14. von Bertalanffy, Ludwig (1968). General systems theory: foundations, development, applications. New York, NY: George Braziller.
  15. Bley-Vroman, Robert (2009). "The evolving context of the fundamental difference hypothesis". Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 31 (2): 175–198. doi:10.1017/S0272263109090275. S2CID   145290162.
  16. Schmid, Hans-Joerg (2017). Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning: How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge. New York: American Psychological Association.
  17. Bates, Elizabeth; MacWhinney, Brian (1981). "Second language acquisition from a functionalist perspective". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: 190–214. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1981.tb42009.x. S2CID   86330898.
  18. Zevin, Jason (2012). "A sensitive period for shibboleths: The long tail and changing goals of speech perception over the course of development". Developmental Psychobiology. 54 (6): 632–642. doi:10.1002/dev.20611. PMID   22714710.
  19. Kroll, Judith; Van Hell, Janet; Tokowicz, Natasha; Green, David (2010). "The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment". Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 13 (3): 373–381. doi:10.1017/S136672891000009X. PMC   2910435 . PMID   20676387.
  20. Schlichting, Margaret; Preston, Allison (2015). "Memory integration: neural mechanisms and implications for behavior". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 1: 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.07.005. PMC   4346341 . PMID   25750931.
  21. Vygotsky, Lev (1934). Thought and language. MIT Press.
  22. Firth, Alan; Wagner, Johannes (2007). "Second/foreign language learning as a social accomplishment: Elaborations on a reconceptualized SLA". Modern Language Journal. 91: 800–819. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00670.x.
  23. Snow, Catherine (1978). "The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from language learning". Child Development. 49: 1114–1128. doi:10.2307/1128751. JSTOR   1128751.
  24. Flege, James; Monro, Murray; Mackay, Ian (1995). "Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 97 (5 Pt 1): 3125–3134. doi: 10.1121/1.413041 . PMID   7759652.
  25. Harris, Zellig (1951). Structural linguistics. Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago Press.
  26. MacWhinney, Brian (2019). Wen, Zhisheng; Ahmadian, Mohammed (eds.). "Task-based analysis and the Competition Model". Researching Second Language Task Performance and Pedagogy: Essays in Honor of Peter Skehan. New York: Benjamins. doi:10.1075/tblt.13.14mac. S2CID   201130027.
  27. Kemmerer, David (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of language. New York: Psychology Press.
  28. 1 2 3 MacWhinney, Brian (2014). "Item-based patterns in early syntactic development". Constructions, Collocations, Patterns: 33–70. doi:10.1515/9783110356854.33. ISBN   978-3-11-035610-6.
  29. 1 2 MacWhinney, Brian (1977). "Starting points". Language. 53 (1): 152–168. doi:10.2307/413059. JSTOR   413059. S2CID   33322208.
  30. 1 2 3 MacWhinney, Brian (2008). How mental models encode embodied linguistic perspectives. In Klatzkty, MacWhinney, & Behrmann Embodiment, Ego-Space and Action. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 368–410.
  31. Schegloff, Emmanuel (2007). Sequence organization in interaction: A primer in conversation analysis. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  32. Braine, Martin (1963). "On learning the grammatical order of words". Psychological Review. 70 (4): 323–348. doi:10.1037/h0047696. PMID   14014895.
  33. Braine, Martin (1976). "Children's first word combinations". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 41 (1). doi:10.2307/1165959. JSTOR   1165959.
  34. Goldberg, Adele (1995). Constructions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  35. Hauser, Mark; Chomsky, Noam; Fitch, Tecumseh (2002). "The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?". Science. 298 (5598): 1569–1579. doi:10.1126/science.298.5598.1569. PMID   12446899.
  36. Steedman, Mark (2000). The syntactic process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  37. Chafe, Wallace (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
  38. McDonald, Janet; MacWhinney, Brian (1995). "The time course of anaphor resolution: Effects of implicit verb causality and gender". Journal of Memory and Language. 34 (4): 543–566. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1025.
  39. 1 2 Simon, Herbert (1962). "The architecture of complexity". Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society. 106: 467–482.
  40. Fodor, Jerry (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  41. Rumelhart, David (1987). Parallel distributed processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  42. Wessinger, Mark (1997). "Tonotopy in human auditory cortex examined with functional magnetic resonance imaging". Human Brain Mapping. 5 (1): 18–25. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:1<18::AID-HBM3>3.0.CO;2-Q. PMID   20408207. S2CID   2426271.
  43. Hauk, Olaf (2004). "Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex". Neuron. 41 (2): 301–307. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 . PMID   14741110. S2CID   7423629.
  44. Werker, Janet; Hensch, Takeo (2014). "Critical periods in speech perception: New directions". Annual Review of Psychology. 66: 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104 . PMID   25251488. S2CID   27047922.
  45. Levelt, Willem (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  46. Skehan, Peter (2009). "Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis". Applied Linguistics. 30 (4): 510–532. doi:10.1093/applin/amp047.
  47. Snow, Catherine (1999). "Social perspectives on the emergence of language". The Emergence of Language.
  48. Campbell, N.A. (1999). Biology: Fifth Edition. Menlo Park: Addison Wesley.
  49. Ventureyra, Valerie; Pallier, Christophe; Yoo, Hi-Yon (2004). "The loss of first language phonetic perception in adopted Koreans". Journal of Neurolinguistics. 17: 79–91. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00053-8. S2CID   8236527.
  50. 1 2 MacWhinney, Brian (2018). "Language attrition and the Competition Model". Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition.
  51. MacWhinney, Brian (2019). "Neuroemergentism: Levels and constraints". Journal of Neurolinguistics. 49: 232–234. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.04.002. PMC   6329393 . PMID   30643353.
  52. MacWhinney, Brian (2019). "Understanding spoken language through TalkBank". Behavior Research Methods. 51 (4): 1919–1927. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-1174-9. PMC   6546550 . PMID   30511153.
  53. MacWhinney, Brian (2017). "A shared platform for studying second language acquisition". Language Learning. 67: 254–275. doi:10.1111/lang.12220.
  54. MacWhinney, Brian (2005). "Language evolution and human development". Origins of the Social Mind.
  55. MacWhinney, Brian (2015). "Language development". Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Volume 2 Cognitive Processes: 296–338.
  56. Presson, Nora (2011). "The Competition Model and language disorders". Handbook of Psycholinguistic and Cognitive Processes: 31–48.