Nesting instinct

Last updated
Bird's nest in grass Bird nest in grass.jpg
Bird's nest in grass

Nesting behavior refers to an instinct in animals during reproduction to prepare a place with optimal conditions for offspring. [1] The nesting place provides protection against predators and competitors that mean to exploit or kill offspring. [2] It also provides protection against the physical environment. [1]

Contents

Nest building is important in family structure [3] and is therefore influenced by different mating behaviours and social settings. [4] It is found in a variety of animals such as birds, fish, mammals, amphibians, and reptiles. [1]

In mammals

Female dogs may show signs of nesting behaviour about one week before they are due [5] that include pacing and building a nest with items from around the house such as blankets, clothing, and stuffed animals. [5] (They also sometimes do this in cases of false pregnancy, or pseudocyesis). Domestic cats often make nests by bringing straw, cloth scraps, and other soft materials to a selected nook or box; they particularly are attracted to haylofts as nest sites. Commercial whelping and queening boxes are available; however, children's wading pools (dogs) and plastic dishpans (cats) work just as well. [5] In birds it is known as "going broody", and is characterized by the insistence to stay on the nest as much as possible, and by cessation of laying new eggs. Marsupials do not exhibit a nesting instinct per se, because the mother's pouch fulfills the function of housing the newborns. Nest building is performed in order to provide sufficient shelter and comfort to the arriving offspring. [6] Threats, such as predators, that decrease the chance of survival will increase care of offspring. [7]

Pigs

Under natural conditions, sows will leave the herd and travel up to 6.5 km (4.0 mi) [6] a day prior to parturition in order to find the appropriate spot for a nest. [8] The sows will use their forelimbs and snouts in order to create excavated depressions within the ground and to gather/transport nesting materials. [9] Although the nests vary in radius dependent on the age of the sow, the nests are generally a round to oval shape and are usually located near trees, uprooted stumps or logs. [9] The shelter provided by the nest built in sows is of utmost importance to thermoregulation. For the first two weeks of the piglets life their physiological thermoregulation is still developing, and due to a lack of amount of brown fat tissue, piglets require an increased surrounding temperature. Without the protection of the nest, the piglets will be subjected to climatic influences causing their internal temperature to drop to life-threatening levels. [6]

Farrowing crates have been widely implemented into modern pig husbandry in order to reduce piglet mortality via crushing. However, this type of housing disturbs the sows natural instinct to nest build due to lack of space. Thus, it is necessary for the sows to farrow without the performance of this natural pre-partum activity which results in high stress for the animal.

Rodents

Groundhog gathering nesting material for its warm burrow Marmota monax UL 07.jpg
Groundhog gathering nesting material for its warm burrow
Wood rat (Neotoma lepida) nest at Joshua Tree National Park Wood rat (Neotoma lepida) midden.jpg
Wood rat (Neotoma lepida) nest at Joshua Tree National Park

In rodents and lagomorphs, the nesting instinct is typically characterized by the urge to seek the lowest sheltered spot available; this is where these mammals give birth. Rats, for example, prefer to burrow amongst dense areas of vegetation or around human settlements which they come into contact with often. [10] Often some rodent species create burrows that develop microclimates. This is another way that nesting instinct aids in thermoregulation. [4] Alzheimer's disease in rats has been observed to impair the nesting ability, especially in females. These impairments become exaggerated with age and progression of disease. [3]

Particularly among burrowing animals, such as groundhogs and prairie dogs nesting is used all across the burrows for uses such as insulation, bedding, litter chambers, transportation, comfort and various other uses. [11] Marmot species such as groundhogs, and alpine marmots nest their borrows with thick grasses in advance of winter, this keeps a thermoregulated insulated comfortable environment for the marmots as they undergo hibernation. [12] [13] [14]

Hormones and nesting behavior

Maternal nest-building is regulated by the hormonal actions of estradiol, progesterone, and prolactin. Given the importance of shelter to offspring survival and reproductive success, it is no wonder that a set of common hormonal signals has evolved. However, the exact timing and features of nest building vary among species, depending on endocrine and external factors.

The initial drive to perform this behavior is stimulated internally via hormones, specifically a rise in prolactin levels. This increase is driven by an increase in prostaglandin and a decrease in progesterone. [15] The second phase of nest building is driven by external stimuli, this phase is also known as the material-oriented phase. In this stage it is said that external stimuli such as the proper nest building materials must be present. Both internal and external stimuli must exist in conjunction with one another for nest building to commence. The cessation of the nest building is correlated with a rise in oxytocin which is the hormone responsible for the contraction of the uterus. Shortly after this, parturition will commence. [6]

In rabbits, nest building occurs towards the last third of pregnancy. The mother digs and builds a nest of straw and grass, which she lines with hair plucked from her body. This sequential motor pattern is produced by changes in estradiol, progesterone, and prolactin levels. Six to eight days pre-partum, high levels of estradiol and progesterone lead to a peak in digging behavior. Both estradiol and progesterone are produced and released by the ovaries. One to three days pre-partum, straw-carrying behavior is expressed as a function of decreasing progesterone levels, maintenance of high estradiol levels, and increasing prolactin levels. This release of prolactin (from the anterior pituitary) is likely caused by the increase in estrogen-to-progesterone ratio. One day pre-partum to four days post-partum, hair loosening and plucking occur as a result of low progesterone and high prolactin levels, together with a decrease in testosterone. [16] In house mice and golden hamsters, nest-building takes place earlier, at the start or middle of pregnancy. For these species, nest-building coincides with high levels of estrogen and progestin. [17] [18]

External factors also interact with hormones to influence maternal nest-building behavior. Pregnant rabbits that have been shaved will line their straw nest with available alternatives, such as male rabbit hair or synthetic hair. If given both straw and hair, mothers prefer straw during the straw-carrying period, and prefer hair during the nest-lining period. If given hair as the only material, shaved mothers collect the hair even when it is the straw-carrying period. [19]

In birds

Research on avian paternal behavior shows that nest-building is triggered by different stimuli in the two sexes. Unlike the case for females, male nest-building among ring doves depends on the behavior of the prospective mate rather than on hormonal mechanisms. Males that are castrated and injected daily with testosterone either court females or build nests, depending purely on the behavior of the female. Hence, the male avian transition from courtship to nest-building is prompted by social cues and not by changes in hormone levels. [20]

In fish

In sand goby ( Pomatoschistus minutus) the males are the ones who build the nests. When males exhibit increased paternal care to eggs, they build nests with smaller entrances in comparison to males who provide less parental care. This helps prevent predators from entering the nest and consuming the offspring or developing eggs. [7]

In insects

Tawny mining bee (Andrena fulva) nest entrance Tawny Mining Bee nest - Andrena Fulva 2d.jpg
Tawny mining bee (Andrena fulva) nest entrance

Nesting behavior is also present in many invertebrates. The best known example of nesting behavior in insects is that of the domestic honey bee. Most bees build nests. Solitary bees, like honey bees, make nests. However, solitary bees make individual nests for larvae and are not always in colonies. [21] Solitary bees will burrow into the ground, dead wood and plants. [21] [22]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hypothalamus</span> Area of the brain below the thalamus

The hypothalamus is a small part of the brain that contains a number of nuclei with a variety of functions. One of the most important functions is to link the nervous system to the endocrine system via the pituitary gland. The hypothalamus is located below the thalamus and is part of the limbic system. It forms the ventral part of the diencephalon. All vertebrate brains contain a hypothalamus. In humans, it is the size of an almond.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prolactin</span> Protein family and hormone

Prolactin (PRL), also known as lactotropin and mammotropin, is a protein best known for its role in enabling mammals to produce milk. It is influential in over 300 separate processes in various vertebrates, including humans. Prolactin is secreted from the pituitary gland in response to eating, mating, estrogen treatment, ovulation and nursing. It is secreted heavily in pulses in between these events. Prolactin plays an essential role in metabolism, regulation of the immune system and pancreatic development.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Behavioral ecology</span> Study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures

Behavioral ecology, also spelled behavioural ecology, is the study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures. Behavioral ecology emerged from ethology after Niko Tinbergen outlined four questions to address when studying animal behaviors: What are the proximate causes, ontogeny, survival value, and phylogeny of a behavior?

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dominance hierarchy</span> Type of socially subordinate ranking

In the zoological field of ethology, a dominance hierarchy is a type of social hierarchy that arises when members of animal social groups interact, creating a ranking system. A dominant higher-ranking individual is sometimes called an alpha, and a submissive lower-ranking individual is called a beta. Different types of interactions can result in dominance depending on the species, including ritualized displays of aggression or direct physical violence. In social living groups, members are likely to compete for access to limited resources and mating opportunities. Rather than fighting each time they meet, individuals of the same sex establish a relative rank, with higher-ranking individuals often gaining more access to resources and mates. Based on repetitive interactions, a social order is created that is subject to change each time a dominant animal is challenged by a subordinate one.

In mammalian species, pseudopregnancy is a physical state whereby all the signs and symptoms of pregnancy are exhibited, with the exception of the presence of a fetus, creating a false pregnancy. The corpus luteum is responsible for the development of maternal behavior and lactation, which are mediated by the continued production of progesterone by the corpus luteum through some or all of pregnancy. In most species, the corpus luteum is degraded in the absence of a pregnancy. However, in some species, the corpus luteum may persist in the absence of pregnancy and cause "pseudopregnancy", in which the female will exhibit clinical signs of pregnancy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alloparenting</span> Parenting not done by the birth parents

Alloparenting is a term used to classify any form of parental care provided by an individual towards young that are not its own direct offspring. These are often referred to as "non-descendant" young, even though grandchildren can be among them. Among humans, alloparenting is often performed by a child's grandparents and older siblings. Individuals providing this care are referred to using the neutral term of alloparent.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pig</span> Domesticated omnivorous even-toed ungulate

The pig, also called swine or hog, is an omnivorous, domesticated, even-toed, hoofed mammal. It is named the domestic pig when distinguishing it from other members of the genus Sus. It is considered a subspecies of Sus scrofa by some authorities, but as a distinct species by others. Pigs were domesticated in the Neolithic, both in East Asia and in the Near East. When domesticated pigs arrived in Europe, they extensively interbred with wild boar but retained their domesticated features.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lactational amenorrhea</span> Post-partum infertility due to breast feeding

Lactational amenorrhea, also called postpartum infertility, is the temporary postnatal infertility that occurs when a woman is amenorrheic and fully breastfeeding.

Cross-fostering is a technique used in animal husbandry, animal science, genetic and nature versus nurture studies, and conservation, whereby offspring are removed from their biological parents at birth and raised by surrogates, typically of a different species, hence 'cross.' This can also occasionally occur in nature.

<i>Nomada</i> Genus of bees

With over 850 species, the genus Nomada is one of the largest genera in the family Apidae, and the largest genus of cuckoo bees. Cuckoo bees are so named because they enter the nests of a host and lay eggs there, stealing resources that the host has already collected. The name "Nomada" is derived from the Greek word nomas, meaning "roaming" or "wandering."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reproductive suppression</span>

Reproductive suppression is the prevention or inhibition of reproduction in otherwise healthy adult individuals. It occurs in birds, mammals, and social insects. It is sometimes accompanied by cooperative breeding. It is maintained by behavioral mechanisms such as aggression, and physiological mechanisms such as pheromone signalling. In evolutionary terms, it may be explained by the theory of inclusive fitness.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Savaging</span> In ethology, aggressive behaviour displayed by the mother towards the offspring

Savaging is a term used in the study of ethology that refers to aggressive behaviour displayed by the mother towards the offspring. Aggressive behaviour includes being rough with, injuring, biting, attacking, crushing and killing of the offspring. While savaging behaviour has been seen in multiple species, it is predominantly demonstrated in domestic pigs. As the definition of savaging is so broad, research on the prevalence of savaging behaviour varies with reports of little savaging of offspring to savaging of offspring up to the 20th percentile. Prevalence of aggressive, non-fatal savaging is greater in gilts, or females who have not yet previously farrowed, as piglet-focused aggression is more frequent in young animals than sows, adult females who have previously given birth. Occurrence of savaging demonstrated by sows is greater if the sow has previously savaged her offspring either as a gilt or sow. Savaging behaviour usually occurs during the first two days after parturition. Prevalence of savaging is similar among first and second farrowing cycles. Savaging behaviour has a significant impact on both agricultural economy and animal welfare which is why it is currently a subject of interest in the pig industry.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Infanticide in rodents</span> Termination or consumption of newborn rodents by the parent or another rodent

Infanticide is the termination of a neonate after it has been born, and in zoology this is often the termination or consumption of newborn animals by either a parent or an unrelated adult. In rodents, it is not uncommon for the mother to commit infanticide shortly after parturition under conditions of extreme stress, or for an unrelated male to kill neonates.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Structures built by animals</span>

Structures built by non-human animals, often called animal architecture, are common in many species. Examples of animal structures include termite mounds, ant hills, wasp and beehives, burrow complexes, beaver dams, elaborate nests of birds, and webs of spiders.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parental brain</span>

Parental experience, as well as changing hormone levels during pregnancy and postpartum, cause changes in the parental brain. Displaying maternal sensitivity towards infant cues, processing those cues and being motivated to engage socially with her infant and attend to the infant's needs in any context could be described as mothering behavior and is regulated by many systems in the maternal brain. Research has shown that hormones such as oxytocin, prolactin, estradiol and progesterone are essential for the onset and the maintenance of maternal behavior in rats, and other mammals as well. Mothering behavior has also been classified within the basic drives.

Endocrinology of parenting has been the subject of considerable study with focus both on human females and males and on females and males of other mammalian species. Parenting as an adaptive problem in mammals involves specific endocrine signals that were naturally selected to respond to infant cues and environmental inputs. Infants across species produce a number of cues to inform caregivers of their needs. These include visual cues, like facial characteristics, or in some species smiling, auditory cues, such as vocalizations, olfactory cues, and tactile stimulation. A commonly mentioned hormone in parenting is oxytocin, however many other hormones relay key information that results in variations in behavior. These include estrogen, progesterone, prolactin, cortisol, and testosterone. While hormones are not necessary for the expression of maternal behavior, they may influence it.

Syrian hamster behavior refers to the ethology of the Syrian hamster.

Behavioural responses to stress are evoked from underlying complex physiological changes that arise consequently from stress.

Vertebrate maternal behavior is a form of parental care that is specifically given to young animals by their mother in order to ensure the survival of the young. Parental care is a form of altruism, which means that the behaviors involved often require a sacrifice that could put their own survival at risk. This encompasses behaviors that aid in the evolutionary success of the offspring and parental investment, which is a measure of expenditure exerted by the parent in an attempt to provide evolutionary benefits to the offspring. Therefore, it is a measure of the benefits versus costs of engaging in the parental behaviors. Behaviors commonly exhibited by the maternal parent include feeding, either by lactating or gathering food, grooming young, and keeping the young warm. Another important aspect of parental care is whether the care is provided to the offspring by each parent in a relatively equal manner, or whether it is provided predominantly or entirely by one parent. There are several species that exhibit biparental care, where behaviors and/or investment in the offspring is divided equally amongst the parents. This parenting strategy is common in birds. However, even in species who exhibit biparental care, the maternal role is essential since the females are responsible for the incubation and/or delivery of the young.

The cognitive ecology of individual recognition has been studied in many species, especially in primates or other mammalian species that exhibit complex social behaviours, but comparatively little research has been done on colonial birds. Colonial birds live in dense colonies in which many individuals interact with each other daily. For colonial birds, being able to identify and recognize individuals can be a crucial skill.

References

  1. 1 2 3 Baden, Andrea L. (2019-01-01). "A description of nesting behaviors, including factors impacting nest site selection, in black‐and‐white ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata)". Ecology and Evolution. 9 (3): 1010–1028. doi:10.1002/ece3.4735. ISSN   2045-7758. PMC   6374655 . PMID   30805137.
  2. Lewarch, Caitlin L.; Hoekstra, Hopi E. (2018-05-01). "The evolution of nesting behaviour in Peromyscus mice". Animal Behaviour. 139: 103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.03.008 . ISSN   0003-3472.
  3. 1 2 Torres-Lista, Virginia (15 June 2013). "Impairment of nesting behaviour in 3xTg-AD mice". Behavioural Brain Research. 247: 153–157. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.03.021. PMID   23523959. S2CID   24496866.
  4. 1 2 Lewarch, Caitlin L.; Hoekstra, Hopi E. (2018-05-01). "The evolution of nesting behaviour in Peromyscus mice". Animal Behaviour. 139: 103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.03.008 . ISSN   0003-3472.
  5. 1 2 3 Kustritz, M (2005). "Reproductive behaviour of small animals". Theriogenology. 64 (3): 734–746. doi:10.1016/j.theriogenology.2005.05.022. PMID   15946732.
  6. 1 2 3 4 Wischner, D.; Kemper, N.; Krieter, J. (2009). "Nest-building behvaiour in sows and consequences for pig husbandry" (PDF). Livestock Science. 124 (1–3): 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.livsci.2009.01.015.
  7. 1 2 Deal, Nicholas D.S.; Lehtonen, Topi K.; Lindström, Kai; Wong, Bob B.M. (2017-10-01). "Paternal investment with an uncertain future: effects of predator exposure on filial cannibalism and nesting behaviour". Animal Behaviour. 132: 81–90. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.07.024. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   53155468.
  8. Yun, K.; Valros, A. (2015). "Benefits of prepartum nest-building behaviour on parturition and lactating sows". Asian-Australasian Journal of Animal Sciences. 28 (11): 1519–1524. doi:10.5713/ajas.15.0174. PMC   4647089 . PMID   26333669.
  9. 1 2 Mayer, J.; Martin, D.; Brisbin, I. Jr. (2002). "Characteristics of wild pig farrowing nests and bed in the upper Coastal Plain of South Carolina". Applied Animal Behaviour. 78: 1–17. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(02)00114-4 .
  10. Triunveri, Alfeo (24 May 2012). "Rodents: Habitat, Pathology and Environmental Impact". Animal Science, Issues and Professions: 1–194.
  11. The Ecological Impact of Animal Nests and Burrows, by M. H. Hansell. Functional Ecology, 1993, Vol. 7, No. 1 (1993), pp. 5–12. Published by The British Ecological Society.
  12. Friedman, M. H. F.; Armour, John C. (August 14, 1936). "Gastric secretion in the groundhog (Marmota monax) during hibernation". Journal of Cellular and Comparative Physiology. 8 (2): 201–211. doi:10.1002/jcp.1030080207 via Wiley Online Library.
  13. "CAB Direct". www.cabdirect.org.
  14. The Evolution of Marmot Sociality: II. Costs and Benefits of Joint Hibernation, by Walter Arnold. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 1990, Vol. 27, No. 4 (1990), pp. 239–246. Published by Springer.
  15. Algers, B.; Uvnas-Moberg, K. (2007). "Maternal behaviour in pigs" (PDF). Hormones and Behavior. 52 (1): 78–85. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.022. PMID   17482189. S2CID   9742677.
  16. González-Mariscal, G.; Melo, A. I.; Jiménez, P.; Beyer, C.; Rosenblatt, J. S. (1996). "Estradiol, Progesterone, and Prolactin Regulate Maternal Nest-Building in Rabbits". Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 8 (12): 901–907. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.1996.tb00818.x. PMID   8953467. S2CID   40974635.
  17. Lisk, Robert D. (1971). "Oestrogen and progesterone synergism and elicitation of maternal nest-building in the mouse (Mus musculus)". Animal Behaviour. 19 (3): 606–610. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(71)80118-5. PMID   5156617.
  18. Richards, M.P.M. (1969). "Effects of oestrogen and progesterone on nest building in the golden hamster". Animal Behaviour. 17 (2): 356–361. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(69)90022-0. PMID   5388889.
  19. González-Mariscal, G.; Cuamatzi, E.; Rosenblatt, J.S. (1998). "Hormones and External Factors: Are They "On/Off" Signals for Maternal Nest-Building in Rabbits?". Hormones and Behavior. 33 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1997.1425. PMID   9571007. S2CID   25558539.
  20. Silver, Rae (1978). "The Parental Behavior of Ring Doves: The intricately coordinated behavior of the male and female is based on distinct physiological mechanisms in the sexes". American Scientist. 66 (2). Sigma Xi: 209–215. Bibcode:1978AmSci..66..209S. JSTOR   27848517.
  21. 1 2 "Make a Bee Hotel – The Pollinator Garden". www.foxleas.com. Retrieved 2018-11-19.
  22. "Enhancing Habitat for Bees – The Xerces Society". The Xerces Society. Retrieved 2018-11-19.