Hawaiian grammar

Last updated

This article summarizes grammar in the Hawaiian language.

Contents

Syntax

Hawaiian is a predominantly verb–subject–object language. However, word order is flexible, and the emphatic word can be placed first in the sentence. [1] :p28 Hawaiian largely avoids subordinate clauses, [1] :p.27 and often uses a possessive construction instead. [1] :p.41 Hawaiian, unlike English, is a pro-drop language, meaning pronouns may be omitted when the meaning is clear from context. [2] :p.108

The typical detailed word order is given by the following, [1] :p.19 with most items optional:

  1. Tense/aspect signs: i, ua, e, etc.
  2. Verb
  3. Qualifying adverb: mau, wale, ole, pu, etc.
  4. Passive sign: ʻia
  5. Verbal directives: aku, mai, etc.
  6. Locatives nei or , or particles ana or ai
  7. Strengthening particle:
  8. Subject
  9. Object or predicate noun

Exceptions to VSO word order

If the sentence has a negative mood and the subject is a pronoun, word order is subject–verb–object following the negator ʻaʻole, as in:

ʻaʻole

not

ʻo ia

he

e

FUTURE

puka

graduate

ana

SINGLE.EVENT

ʻaʻole {ʻo ia} e puka ana

not he FUTURE graduate SINGLE.EVENT

"He won't graduate"

Another exception is when an emphatic adverbial phrase begins the sentence. In this case, a pronoun subject precedes the verb. [1] :p.29

Interrogatives

Yes–no questions can be unmarked and expressed by intonation, [3] :p.32 or they can be marked by placing anei after the leading word of the sentence. [1] :p.23 Examples of question-word questions include:

He

INDEFINITE

aha

What

kēia?

this?

He aha kēia?

INDEFINITE What this?

"What is this?"

ʻO

PROPER.SUBJECT

wai

who

kou

your

inoa?

name

ʻO wai kou inoa?

PROPER.SUBJECT who your name

"What is your name?"

See also Hawaiian Language: Syntax and other resources Archived 2010-05-22 at the Wayback Machine .

Nouns

As Hawaiian does not particularly discern between word types, any verb can be nominalized by preceding it with the definite article, however, some words that are used as nouns are rarely or never used as verbs. [1] :p.37 Within the noun phrase, adjectives follow the noun (e.g. ka hale liʻiliʻi "the house small", "the small house"), while possessors precede it (e.g. kou hale "your house"). Numerals precede the noun in the absence of the definite article, but follow the noun if the noun is preceded by the definite article. [1] :p.31

Articles

Every noun is preceded by an article (ka‘i). The three main ones are:

Number

In noun phrases, two numbers (singular and plural) are distinguished. The singular articles ke and ka and the plural article are the only articles that mark number:

In the absence of these articles, plurality is usually indicated by inserting the pluralizing particle mau immediately before the noun:

Most nouns do not change when pluralized; however, some nouns referring to people exhibit a lengthened vowel in the third syllable from the end in the plural:

Gender

In Hawaiian, there is no gender distinction in the third person. The word for third person (he, she, it) is ia. It is commonly preceded by ʻo as in ʻo ia and, following standard modern orthographical rules, is written as two words, but it can be seen as one when written by older speakers and in historical documents.

Hawaiian nouns belong to one of two genders, this gender system is not based on biological sex and is better analyzed as a distinction between alienable and inalienable possession, which is a common split in many of the world's languages. The two genders are known as the kino ʻō (o-class) and the kino ʻā (a-class). These classes are only taken into account when using the genitive case (see table of personal pronouns below).

Kino ʻō nouns, in general, are nouns whose creation cannot be controlled by the subject, such as inoa "name", puʻuwai "heart", and hale "house". Specific categories for o-class nouns include: modes of transportation (e.g. kaʻa "car" and lio "horse"), things that you can go into, sit on or wear (e.g., lumi "room", noho "chair", ʻeke "bag", and lole "clothes"), and people in your generation (e.g., siblings, cousins) and previous generations (e.g. makuahine "mother").

Kino ʻā nouns, in general, are those whose creation can be controlled, such as waihoʻoluʻu "color", as in kaʻu waihoʻoluʻu punahele "my favorite color". Specific categories include: your boyfriend or girlfriend (ipo), spouse, friends, and future generations in your line (all of your descendants).

The change of preposition of o "of" (kino ʻō) to a "of "(kino ʻā) is especially important for prepositional and subordinate phrases[ clarification needed ]:

Demonstrative determiners

Demonstrative determiners ProximalMedialDistal
Singularkēiakēnākēlā
Pluralkēia maukēnā maukēlā mau
Singular (aforementioned)ua ... neiua ... lā/alaua ... lā/ala
Plural (aforementioned)ua mau ... neiua mau ... lā/alaua mau ... lā/ala

Personal pronouns

Personal pronouns
Case
Nominative Genitive Accusative,
Dative
a-classo-classaffectionate
Singular
(1)
1staukaʻukoʻukuʻuiaʻu
2ndʻoekāukouʻoe
3rdiakānakonaOnly used
in 1st and
2nd person
singular.
iā ia
Dual
(2)
1st incl. kāuakā kāuako kāuaiā kāua
excl. māuakā māuako māuaiā māua
2ndʻoluaʻoluako ʻoluaʻolua
3rdlāuakā lāuakō lāuaiā lāua
Plural
(3+)
1stincl.kākoukā kākouko kākouiā kākou
excl.mākoukā mākouko mākouiā mākou
2ndʻoukouʻoukouko ʻoukouʻoukou
3rdlākoukā lākoukō lākouiā lākou

Verbs

Tense, aspect, and mood

Verbs can be analytically marked with particles to indicate tense, aspect and mood. Separate verb markers are used in relative clauses, after the negation word ʻaʻole, and in some other situations. [2]

Common Tense, Aspect, and Mood Markers
Tense/Aspect/MoodDefaultAfter ʻaʻole or in type I relative clauseType II relative clause
Tenseless[verb](e) [verb]e [verb] ai
Perfectiveua [verb]i [verb]i [verb] ai
Progressivee [verb] ana/ala/lāe [verb] ana/ala/lāe [verb] ana/ala/lā
Presentke [verb] nei/ala/lāe [verb] nei/ala/lāe [verb] nei/ala/lā
Intentive/infinitivee [verb]e [verb]e [verb] ai
Imperative(e) [verb]
Prohibitivemai [verb]

The marker ala/lā implies greater spatial or temporal distance from the speaker than nei or ana.

In his "Introduction to Hawaiian Grammar," W.D. Alexander [1] proposed that Hawaiian has a pluperfect tense as follows:

However, this is debatable since ʻē simply means "beforehand, in advance, already". [4] Andrews [Gram. 1.4] suggested the same thing that Alexander forwards. However, Ua hana ʻē au could mean both "I have already worked", "I already worked", and (depending on the temporal context) "I had worked previous to that moment." "Already" is the operative unifier for these constructions as well as the perfective quality denoted by ua. ʻĒ therefore is acting like a regular Hawaiian adverb, following the verb it modifies:

Passive Voice

Transitive verbs can be passivized with the particle ʻia, which follows the verb but precedes tense/aspect/mood markers. The agent, if specified, is marked with the preposition e, usually translated as "by" in English:

Equative sentences

Hawaiian does not have a copula verb meaning "to be" nor does it have a verb meaning "to have". Equative sentences are used to convey this group of ideas. All equative sentences in Hawaiian are zero-tense/mood (i.e., they cannot be modified by verbal markers, particles or adverbs).

Pepeke ʻAike He "A is a B"

Pepeke ʻAike He is the name for the simple equative sentence "A is a(n) B". The pattern is "He B (ʻo) A." ʻO marks the third person singular pronoun ia (which means "he/she/it") and all proper nouns.

  • He kaikamahine ʻo Mary. Mary is a girl.
  • He kaikamahine ʻo ia. She is a girl.
  • He Hawaiʻi kēlā kaikamahine. That girl is (a) Hawaiian.[ clarification needed ]
  • He haumana ke keiki. The child is a student.

Pepeke ʻAike ʻO

Pepeke ʻAike ʻO is the name for the simple equative sentence "A is B." The pattern is " ʻO A (ʻo) B," where the order of the nouns is interchangeable and where ʻo invariably marks the third person singular pronoun ia and all proper nouns (regardless of where it is in the utterance).

  • ʻO Mary ʻo ia. ʻO ia ʻo Mary. She is Mary.
  • ʻO Mary nō ia. ʻO ia nō ʻo Mary. It's Mary.
  • ʻO wau ʻo Mary. ʻO Mary wau. I'm Mary.
  • ʻO ʻoe ʻo Mary. ʻO Mary ʻoe. You are Mary.
  • ʻO Mary ke kaikamahine. ʻO ke kaikamahine ʻo Mary. Mary is the girl. The girl is Mary.
  • ʻO ka haumana ke keiki. ʻO ke keiki ka haumana. The student is the child. The child is the student.

Pepeke Henua (Locational equative)

Pepeke Henua is the name for the simple equative sentence "A is located (in/on/at/etc. B)." The pattern is "Aia (ʻo) A..."

  • Aia ʻo Mary ma Hilo. Mary is in Hilo.
  • Aia ʻo ia maloko o ka wai. He/she/it is inside (of) the water.
  • Aia ka haumana mahea? Aia mahea ka haumana? Where is the student?

Pepeke ʻAike Na

Pepeke ʻAike Na is the name of the simple equative sentence "A belongs to B." The pattern is "Na (B) A." The singular pronouns undergo predictable changes.

PronounNominativeGenitive "for" or "belonging to"
First person singular "I"(w)aunaʻu
Second person singular "You"ʻoenāu
Third person singular "he/she/it"(ʻo) ianāna
First person plural, dual inclusive "we; you and I"kāuana kāua

Pepeke ʻAike Na Examples:

Naʻu ke kaʻa. The car belongs to me. That's my car.

Na Mary ke keiki. The child is Mary's. It's Mary's child.

Nāna ka penikala. The pencil belongs to him/her/it.

Nāu nō au. I belong to you. I'm yours.

Note:

ʻO kēia ke kaʻa nāu. This is the car I'm giving to you.

He makana kēlā na ke aliʻi. This is a present for the chief.

Other verbal particles

Other post-verbal markers include [5] :pp.228–231

Causative verb creation

Causative verbs can be created from nouns and adjectives by using the prefix ho'o-, as illustrated in the following: [3] :p.24

Reduplication

Reduplication [3] :p.23 can emphasize or otherwise alter the meaning of a word. Examples are:

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fijian language</span> Austronesian language of Fiji

Fijian is an Austronesian language of the Malayo-Polynesian family spoken by some 350,000–450,000 ethnic Fijians as a native language. The 2013 Constitution established Fijian as an official language of Fiji, along with English and Fiji Hindi and there is discussion about establishing it as the "national language". Fijian is a VOS language.

Tahitian is a Polynesian language, spoken mainly on the Society Islands in French Polynesia. It belongs to the Eastern Polynesian group.

Niuean is a Polynesian language, belonging to the Malayo-Polynesian subgroup of the Austronesian languages. It is most closely related to Tongan and slightly more distantly to other Polynesian languages such as Māori, Samoan, and Hawaiian. Together, Tongan and Niuean form the Tongic subgroup of the Polynesian languages. Niuean also has a number of influences from Samoan and Eastern Polynesian languages.

Hurrian is an extinct Hurro-Urartian language spoken by the Hurrians (Khurrites), a people who entered northern Mesopotamia around 2300 BC and had mostly vanished by 1000 BC. Hurrian was the language of the Mitanni kingdom in northern Mesopotamia and was likely spoken at least initially in Hurrian settlements in modern-day Syria.

Tokelauan is a Polynesian language spoken in Tokelau and historically by the small population of Swains Island in American Samoa. It is closely related to Tuvaluan and is related to Samoan and other Polynesian languages. Tokelauan has a co-official status with English in Tokelau. There are approximately 4,260 speakers of Tokelauan, of whom 2,100 live in New Zealand, 1,400 in Tokelau, and 17 in Swains Island. "Tokelau" means "north-northeast".

Marquesan is a collection of East-Central Polynesian dialects, of the Marquesic group, spoken in the Marquesas Islands of French Polynesia. They are usually classified into two groups, North Marquesan and South Marquesan, roughly along geographic lines.

Vaeakau-Taumako is a Polynesian language spoken in some of the Reef Islands as well as in the Taumako Islands in the Temotu province of the Solomon Islands.

The Nafsan language, also known as South Efate or Erakor, is a Southern Oceanic language spoken on the island of Efate in central Vanuatu. As of 2005, there are approximately 6,000 speakers who live in coastal villages from Pango to Eton. The language's grammar has been studied by Nick Thieberger, who has produced a book of stories and a dictionary of the language.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Farasan Islands</span>

The Farasan Islands are a small group of coral islands approximately 40 km off the coast of Jizan in the Red Sea, belonging to Saudi Arabia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hindustani grammar</span> Grammatical features of the Hindustani lingua franca

Hindustani, the lingua franca of Northern India and Pakistan, has two standardised registers: Hindi and Urdu. Grammatical differences between the two standards are minor but each uses its own script: Hindi uses Devanagari while Urdu uses an extended form of the Perso-Arabic script, typically in the Nastaʿlīq style.

Oroha, categorized as an Austronesian language, is one of many languages spoken by Melanesian people in the Solomon Islands. It is also known as Maramasike, Mara Ma-Siki, Oraha, and Oloha, and is used primarily in the southern part of Malaita Island within the Malaita Province. Little Mala is composed of three indigenous languages of the 'Tolo' people which are Na’oni, Pau, and Oroha. They are all slightly different, yet come from the same origin. The three languages may be thought of as different dialects of the same language. The three Tolo villages now harbor schools under the Melanesian Mission.

Pohnpeian is a Micronesian language spoken as the indigenous language of the island of Pohnpei in the Caroline Islands. Pohnpeian has approximately 30,000 (estimated) native speakers living in Pohnpei and its outlying atolls and islands with another 10,000-15,000 (estimated) living off island in parts of the US mainland, Hawaii and Guam. It is the second-most widely spoken native language of the Federated States of Micronesia the first being Chuukese.

Maká is a Matacoan language spoken in Argentina and Paraguay by the Maká people. Its 1,500 speakers live primarily in Presidente Hayes Department near the Río Negro, as well as in and around Asunción.

The Owa language is one of the languages of Solomon Islands. It is part of the same dialect continuum as Kahua, and shares the various alternate names of that dialect.

The Anuta language is a Polynesian Outlier language from the island of Anuta in the Solomon Islands. It is closely related to the Tikopia language of the neighboring island of Tikopia, and it bears significant cultural influence from the island. The two languages have a high degree of mutual intelligibility, although Anutans can understand Tikopians better than the reverse.

Kokota is spoken on Santa Isabel Island, which is located in the Solomon Island chain in the Pacific Ocean. Santa Isabel is one of the larger islands in the chain, but it has a very low population density. Kokota is the main language of three villages: Goveo and Sisigā on the North coast, and Hurepelo on the South coast, though there are a few speakers who reside in the capital, Honiara, and elsewhere. The language is classified as a 6b (threatened) on the Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale (GIDS). To contextualize '6b', the language is not in immediate danger of extinction since children in the villages are still taught Kokota and speak it at home despite English being the language of the school system. However, Kokota is threatened by another language, Cheke Holo, as speakers of this language move from the west of the island closer to the Kokota-speaking villages. Kokota is one of 37 languages in the Northwestern Solomon Group, and as with other Oceanic languages, it has limited morphological complexity.

The morphology of the Welsh language shows many characteristics perhaps unfamiliar to speakers of English or continental European languages like French or German, but has much in common with the other modern Insular Celtic languages: Irish, Scottish Gaelic, Manx, Cornish, and Breton. Welsh is a moderately inflected language. Verbs conjugate for person, tense and mood with affirmative, interrogative and negative conjugations of some verbs. A majority of prepositions inflect for person and number. There are few case inflections in Literary Welsh, being confined to certain pronouns.

Zotung (Zobya) is a language spoken by the Zotung people, in Rezua Township, Chin State, Burma. It is a continuum of closely related dialects and accents. The language does not have a standard written form since it has dialects with multiple variations on its pronunciations. Instead, Zotung speakers use a widely accepted alphabet for writing with which they spell using their respective dialect. However, formal documents are written using the Lungngo dialect because it was the tongue of the first person to prescribe a standard writing, Sir Siabawi Khuamin.

LFN has an analytic grammar and resembles the grammars of languages such as the Haitian Creole, Papiamento, and Afrikaans. On the other hand, it uses a vocabulary drawn from several modern romance languages – Portuguese, Spanish, Catalan, French, and Italian.

This article describes the grammar of the Old Irish language. The grammar of the language has been described with exhaustive detail by various authors, including Thurneysen, Binchy and Bergin, McCone, O'Connell, Stifter, among many others.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Alexander, W. D., Introduction to Hawaiian Grammar, Dover, 2004.
  2. 1 2 Pukui, Mary; Elbert, Samuel (2001). Hawaiian Grammar. U. of Hawaii Press. ISBN   0-8248-0494-5.
  3. 1 2 3 Schütz, Albert J., All About Hawaiian, U. of Hawaii Press, 1995.
  4. "Nā Puke Wehewehe ʻŌlelo Hawaiʻi". wehewehe.org. Retrieved 2016-05-28.
  5. New Pocket Hawaiian Dictionary, grammar section (pp. 225–243), U. of Hawaii Press, 1992.