Roland William Fleming

Last updated
Roland William Fleming
Roland William Fleming 2019.jpg
Roland William Fleming standing in his office in 2019
Born (1978-03-01) 1 March 1978 (age 46)
Nationality British and German
Education New College School
Magdalen College School
Alma mater University of Oxford (BA)
Massachusetts Institute of Technology (PhD)
Known forVisual perception of materials and objects
AwardsVSS Young Investigator Award (2013)
FRSB
Scientific career
Fields
Institutions Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics
Justus Liebig University of Giessen
Thesis Human Visual Perception under Real-world Illumination  (2004)
Doctoral advisor Edward H. Adelson
Website www.allpsych.uni-giessen.de/fleminglab/

Roland William Fleming, FRSB (born 1978 in Oxford, UK) is a British and German interdisciplinary researcher specializing in the visual perception of objects and materials. He is the Kurt Koffka Professor of Experimental Psychology at Justus Liebig University of Giessen. [1] and the Executive Director of the Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior of the Universities of Marburg and Giessen. [2] He is also co-Spokesperson for the Research Cluster “The Adaptive Mind”. [3]

Contents

Biography

Fleming was educated at New College School and Magdalen College School in Oxford. Thereafter, he was a student at New College, University of Oxford, where he studied for a Bachelor’s degree in Psychology, Philosophy and Physiology. He graduated with First Class Honours in 1999. [4] He then studied for a Doctorate at the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT, graduating in 2004. His doctoral thesis “Human Visual Perception under Real-World Illumination” was supervised by Edward H. Adelson. [5]

In 2003, he took up a post-doctoral research position at the Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, working in the department of Heinrich H. Bülthoff. [6] From 2009–2013 he served as co-Editor-In-Chief of the journal ACM Transactions on Applied Perception. [7] In 2010, moved to the Justus Liebig University of Giessen to become the Kurt Koffka Junior Professor of Experimental Psychology. [1] From 2013–2016, [8] Fleming coordinated the Marie Curie Initial Training Network “PRISM: Perceptual Representation of Illumination, Shape and Material”. [9] From 2016–2022 he ran the ERC Consolidator Grant “SHAPE: On the Perception of Growth, Form and Process”. [10] He received tenure in 2016 and was promoted to Full Professor in 2020. [4] In 2021 he became the Executive Director of the Centre for Mind, Brain and Behavior at the Universities of Marburg and Giessen. In 2022 he was elected Fellow of the Royal Society of Biology. [11] Fleming has served on the Expert Review Group and the Interview Panel for the Wellcome Trust. [12] In 2023 he was awarded the ERC Advanced Grant “STUFF: Perceiving Materials and their Properties”. [13]

Honors and awards

In 2012, Fleming was awarded the Faculty Research Prize of the Justus Liebig University of Giessen (“Preis der Justus-Liebig-Universität Gießen”) for his work on the visual estimation of 3D shape from image orientations. [14] In 2013, he was awarded the Elsevier/Vision Sciences Society Young Investigator Award. [15] In 2016 he was awarded an ERC Consolidator Grant “SHAPE: On the Perception of Growth, Form and Process” [10] and in 2023 an ERC Advanced Grant “STUFF: Perceiving Materials and their Properties”. [13] In 2021, he delivered the Vision Sciences Society annual Public Lecture. [16] In 2022 he was elected Fellow of the Royal Society of Biology. [11]

Research

Fleming specializes in the human visual perception of materials and objects, and their physical properties. [1] He is particularly known for his contributions to establishing material perception as a field of study in vision science. [17] He uses a combination of research methods from experimental psychology, computational neuroscience, computer graphics and machine learning. [4]

Material Perception

Fleming’s early works focused on the visual perception of the optical properties of surfaces and materials, such as gloss, [18] translucency [19] and transparency. [20] He helped determine the role of visual cues such as motion [21] and binocular stereopsis [22] [23] [24] in the perception of surface reflectance, especially gloss. A recurring theme within this work was the concept that specular reflections behave unlike surface markings—such as pigmentation patterns or scratches—leading to specific visual cues for identifying specular reflections and therefore glossy surfaces. He also investigated how multi-component patterns of specular reflection lead to hazy glossy appearances. [25] His more recent studies on surface appearance have tested whether artificial neural networks can reproduce the patterns of errors and successes that human observers make when judging material properties. [26] [27] [28]

In addition to studying how the visual system estimates optical properties of materials, he has also investigated the relationship between other material properties and material categories, and how these are affected by the viewing distance, as in the so-called ‘material-scale ambiguity’. [29]

Fleming also led a number of studies on how the visual system infers the mechanical properties of materials, such as compliance, [30] elasticity, [31] and viscosity [32] [33] [34] [35] from optical, shape and motion cues. [36] [37] Most of these studies used finite elements computer simulations of liquids or deformable solids interacting with their surroundings. A recurring theme within this body of work is the idea that the visual system represents stimuli in a multi-dimensional space of midlevel visual features, which statistically characterize how shape, motion and appearance evolve over time. He and his colleagues have claimed that such representations facilitate disentangling intrinsic material properties from other factors that also contribute to the proximal stimulus [38] [34] such as a flowing liquid’s speed, or the force deforming a compliant solid.

Early in his career, Fleming argued that the visual system infers material properties through heuristics, using simple image statistics that correlate with surface properties under typical viewing conditions. [5] [18] [19] Later however, he proposed that the visual system uses richer internal models of the appearance of objects and materials under typical viewing conditions—an idea he calls ‘Statistical Appearance Models'. [17] [39] Specifically, he has suggested that the visual system acquires the ability to infer material properties (or other distal stimulus properties) by learning generative models of proximal stimuli through unsupervised learning objectives, such as compressing or predictive coding of image content. [40] [41] A proof-of-concept of this theory was demonstrated by training an unsupervised artificial neural network model on a dataset of computer rendered images of bumpy, glossy surfaces. [42] Fleming and his colleagues found that the model spontaneously learned to disentangle scene variables—such as lighting and surface reflectance—even though it was given no explicit information about the true values of these variables. Moreover, the model correctly predicted both successes and failures (i.e., illusions) of human gloss perception.

Shape Perception

Fleming’s early works focused on the visual estimation of three-dimensional (3D) shape from specular reflections, [43] [44] shading [45] and texture. [46] He is particularly known as a proponent of the role of ‘orientation fields’ in shape perception. [47] [48] Orientation fields refer to spatially varying patterns of the dominant local orientation across the image of a surface, as measured by populations of orientation-selective neurons at each image location. [45] [43] [44] [49] [46] Fleming and his colleagues have shown that local image orientation signals tend to vary smoothly across curved surfaces in ways that are systematically related to 3D shape properties. [50] For textured surfaces, local image orientation is related to first-order shape properties, especially surface slant and tilt. [46] For shading patterns and specular reflections, local image orientation structure is related to second-order shape properties, especially the direction of minimum second derivative of surface depths, and the ratio of minimum and maximum second derivative magnitudes. [43] [49] He has argued that orientation fields provide a fundamental source of information about shape, [51] [52] [53] and that their use by the visual system predicts specific illusions of perceived shape, such as when illumination changes. [54]

In addition to the visual estimation of 3D shape, Fleming has also investigated the perceptual organization of shape [55] [56] and the use of shape to make additional inferences about objects and their properties, [30] [31] [32] [34] [57] —a process he calls ‘Shape Understanding’. [10] Fleming led a number of studies on how the visual system makes inferences about the processes and transformations that have formed objects or altered their shape. [58] [59] [60] [61] [62] [63] [64] A recurring theme within this body of work is that an object’s ‘causal history’ leaves traces in its shape, which can be used to identify which of its features are the result of shape-altering transformations. [59] [61] Such transformations include simple spatial distortions [58] [59] and more complex biological growth processes. [60] By analogy to the visual system’s ability to separate images depicting transparent surfaces into multiple distinct causes, Fleming and his colleagues refer to the separation of shape into distinct causes as ‘Shape Scission’. [64] An example of this is the ability to distinguish the causes of different shape features that occur when a face or object is fully covered with a cloth veil. [65] Some of the visible features of the surface of the cloth are caused by the textile draping of its own accord, while others are due to the protrusion of the underlying object. Observers can distinguish these causes, even when the hidden object is of unknown shape. [65]

Fleming has also investigated the role of shape in object categorization, especially in one-shot learning of novel object categories from a single (or small number of) exemplars. [66] [67] In this context, Fleming and colleagues developed a computational model for predicting the perceived similarity between pairs of two-dimensional (2D) shapes, called ‘ShapeComp’. [68] The model combines a large number of shape features to capture different aspects of shape. He and his colleagues have also studied how shape cues contribute to the visual perception of animacy, [69] and conversely how semantics alter the perceptual organization of shape. [70] Fleming and colleagues have argued that human visual one-shot categorization involves inferring a generative model from the exemplar object. [66] [67] They have proposed that this involves segmenting the object into parts, and representing their relations in a way that can be modified to synthesize novel variants belonging to the same category as the exemplar. They claim that this idea is supported by experiments in which participants are presented with a single exemplar and are asked to draw novel variants. [67]

Computer Graphics

Fleming’s work in computer graphics has mainly focused on perceptually-based approaches to representing and modifying photographic imagery. He contributed to the development of image-based algorithms for altering the material appearance [71] and shape [72] of objects in photographs. His work on orientation fields led to methods for synthesizing images of objects with particular 3D shape and material appearance based on purely 2D image operations. [73] He also contributed to work investigating perceptually-based methods for converting between and presenting conventional (low-dynamic range) and high-dynamic range images. [74] [75] He co-authored a text book entitled “Visual Perception from a Computer Graphics Perspective” [76]

Grasping and interacting with objects and materials

Fleming’s work on motor control has focused primarily on the effects of 3D shape [77] [78] [79] and material properties [78] [80] [81] —including mass, [80] [81] friction [80] [82] and rigidity [83] —on grasping. He and his colleagues have investigated various illusions related to grasping, [81] [84] including the ‘material-weight illusion, [81] a variant of the size-weight illusion, in which the expected weight of an object is manipulated through its surface material (instead of its volume as in the size-weight illusion). He and his colleagues developed a computational model for predicting human precision grip (thumb and forefinger) grasp locations on objects with varying 3D shape and materials properties. [78] The model combines multiple cost functions related to the properties of the object and the actor’s hand. The model predicted average human grasp locations approximately as well as different individuals’ grasps predict one another. His research group has developed methods for measuring the contact regions between hands and objects to capture unconstrained, whole-hand grasping behavior. [85]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Shape</span> Form of an object

A shape is a graphical representation of an object's form or its external boundary, outline, or external surface. It is distinct from other object properties, such as color, texture, or material type. In geometry, shape excludes information about the object's position, size, orientation and chirality. A figure is a representation including both shape and size.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Peripheral vision</span> Area of ones field of vision outside of the point of fixation

Peripheral vision, or indirect vision, is vision as it occurs outside the point of fixation, i.e. away from the center of gaze or, when viewed at large angles, in the "corner of one's eye". The vast majority of the area in the visual field is included in the notion of peripheral vision. "Far peripheral" vision refers to the area at the edges of the visual field, "mid-peripheral" vision refers to medium eccentricities, and "near-peripheral", sometimes referred to as "para-central" vision, exists adjacent to the center of gaze.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fovea centralis</span> Small pit in the retina of the eye responsible for all central vision

The fovea centralis is a small, central pit composed of closely packed cones in the eye. It is located in the center of the macula lutea of the retina.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Figure–ground (perception)</span> Humans ability to separate foreground from background in visual images

Figure–ground organization is a type of perceptual grouping that is a vital necessity for recognizing objects through vision. In Gestalt psychology it is known as identifying a figure from the background. For example, black words on a printed paper are seen as the "figure", and the white sheet as the "background".

Stereopsis is the component of depth perception retrieved through binocular vision. Stereopsis is not the only contributor to depth perception, but it is a major one. Binocular vision happens because each eye receives a different image because they are in slightly different positions in one's head. These positional differences are referred to as "horizontal disparities" or, more generally, "binocular disparities". Disparities are processed in the visual cortex of the brain to yield depth perception. While binocular disparities are naturally present when viewing a real three-dimensional scene with two eyes, they can also be simulated by artificially presenting two different images separately to each eye using a method called stereoscopy. The perception of depth in such cases is also referred to as "stereoscopic depth".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Chubb illusion</span> Optical illusion

The Chubb illusion is an optical illusion or error in visual perception in which the apparent contrast of an object varies substantially to most viewers depending on its relative contrast to the field on which it is displayed. These visual illusions are of particular interest to researchers because they may provide valuable insights in regard to the workings of human visual systems.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fixation (visual)</span> Maintaining ones gaze on a single location

Fixation or visual fixation is the maintaining of the gaze on a single location. An animal can exhibit visual fixation if it possess a fovea in the anatomy of their eye. The fovea is typically located at the center of the retina and is the point of clearest vision. The species in which fixational eye movement has been verified thus far include humans, primates, cats, rabbits, turtles, salamanders, and owls. Regular eye movement alternates between saccades and visual fixations, the notable exception being in smooth pursuit, controlled by a different neural substrate that appears to have developed for hunting prey. The term "fixation" can either be used to refer to the point in time and space of focus or the act of fixating. Fixation, in the act of fixating, is the point between any two saccades, during which the eyes are relatively stationary and virtually all visual input occurs. In the absence of retinal jitter, a laboratory condition known as retinal stabilization, perceptions tend to rapidly fade away. To maintain visibility, the nervous system carries out a procedure called fixational eye movement, which continuously stimulates neurons in the early visual areas of the brain responding to transient stimuli. There are three categories of fixational eye movement: microsaccades, ocular drifts, and ocular microtremor. At small amplitudes the boundaries between categories become unclear, particularly between drift and tremor.

Stuart M. Anstis is a professor emeritus of psychology at the University of California, San Diego, in the United States.

The visual appearance of objects is given by the way in which they reflect and transmit light. The color of objects is determined by the parts of the spectrum of light that are reflected or transmitted without being absorbed. Additional appearance attributes are based on the directional distribution of reflected (BRDF) or transmitted light (BTDF) described by attributes like glossy, shiny versus dull, matte, clear, turbid, distinct, etc. Since "visual appearance" is a general concept that includes also various other visual phenomena, such as color, visual texture, visual perception of shape, size, etc., the specific aspects related to how humans see different spatial distributions of light have been given the name cesia. It marks a difference with color, which could be defined as the sensation arising from different spectral compositions or distributions of light.

Form perception is the recognition of visual elements of objects, specifically those to do with shapes, patterns and previously identified important characteristics. An object is perceived by the retina as a two-dimensional image, but the image can vary for the same object in terms of the context with which it is viewed, the apparent size of the object, the angle from which it is viewed, how illuminated it is, as well as where it resides in the field of vision. Despite the fact that each instance of observing an object leads to a unique retinal response pattern, the visual processing in the brain is capable of recognizing these experiences as analogous, allowing invariant object recognition. Visual processing occurs in a hierarchy with the lowest levels recognizing lines and contours, and slightly higher levels performing tasks such as completing boundaries and recognizing contour combinations. The highest levels integrate the perceived information to recognize an entire object. Essentially object recognition is the ability to assign labels to objects in order to categorize and identify them, thus distinguishing one object from another. During visual processing information is not created, but rather reformatted in a way that draws out the most detailed information of the stimulus.

Laurence T. Maloney is an American psychology professor at New York University’s Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science. He is known for applying mathematical models to human behavior.

In visual perception, structure from motion (SFM) refers to how humans recover depth structure from object's motion. The human visual field has an important function: capturing the three-dimensional structures of an object using different kinds of visual cues.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Spatial ability</span> Capacity to understand 3D relationships

Spatial ability or visuo-spatial ability is the capacity to understand, reason, and remember the visual and spatial relations among objects or space.

Binocular switch suppression (BSS) is a technique to suppress usually salient images from an individual's awareness, a type of experimental manipulation used in visual perception and cognitive neuroscience. In BSS, two images of differing signal strengths are repetitively switched between the left and right eye at a constant rate of 1 Hertz. During this process of switching, the image of lower contrast and signal strength is perceptually suppressed for a period of time.

Visual crowding is the inability to view a target stimulus distinctly when presented in a clutter. Crowding impairs the ability to discriminate object features and contours among flankers, which in turn impairs people's ability to respond appropriately to the target stimulus.

In psychology and neuroscience, multiple object tracking (MOT) refers to the ability of humans and other animals to simultaneously monitor multiple objects as they move. It is also the term for certain laboratory techniques used to study this ability.

Ensemble coding, also known as ensemble perception or summary representation, is a theory in cognitive neuroscience about the internal representation of groups of objects in the human mind. Ensemble coding proposes that such information is recorded via summary statistics, particularly the average or variance. Experimental evidence tends to support the theory for low-level visual information, such as shapes and sizes, as well as some high-level features such as face gender. Nonetheless, it remains unclear the extent to which ensemble coding applies to high-level or non-visual stimuli, and the theory remains the subject of active research.

The V1 Saliency Hypothesis, or V1SH is a theory about V1, the primary visual cortex (V1). It proposes that the V1 in primates creates a saliency map of the visual field to guide visual attention or gaze shifts exogenously.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Vergence-accommodation conflict</span> Visual and perceptual phenomenon

Vergence-accommodation conflict (VAC), also known as accommodation-vergence conflict, is a visual phenomenon that occurs when the brain receives mismatching cues between vergence and accommodation of the eye. This commonly occurs in virtual reality devices, augmented reality devices, 3D movies, and other types of stereoscopic displays and autostereoscopic displays. The effect can be unpleasant and cause eye strain.

Mary Myleen Hayhoe is an Australian American psychologist who researches vision. She has developed virtual environments for the investigation of visually guided behaviour. In 2017, Hayhoe was awarded the Vision Sciences Society's Davida Teller Award. In 2002, she was awarded the Optica's Edgar D. Tillyer Award for her contributions to visual perception and cognition. In 2024, Hayhoe was awarded the Kurt Koffka Medal for "advancing the fields of perception or developmental psychology to an extraordinary extent".

References

  1. 1 2 3 Fleming Lab Webpage: https://www.allpsych.uni-giessen.de/fleminglab/
  2. Website of the Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior: https://www.cmbb-fcmh.de/de/cmbb-info/organisation
  3. Website of The Adaptive Mind: https://www.theadaptivemind.de/governance/board-of-directors.html
  4. 1 2 3 ORCID Webpage, Roland Fleming: https://orcid.org/my-orcid?orcid=0000-0001-5033-5069
  5. 1 2 Fleming’s PhD Thesis, MIT Online Repository of PhD Theses: https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/30112
  6. Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Website: https://www.kyb.tuebingen.mpg.de/84275/Alumni-AGBU
  7. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Masthead: https://dl.acm.org/action/showFmPdf?doi=10.1145%2F1462055
  8. Entry for Marie Curie ITN “PRISM” on Cordis Website: https://cordis.europa.eu/project/id/316746
  9. PRISM Website: https://www.allpsych.uni-giessen.de/prism/network/Admin.html
  10. 1 2 3 Entry for ERC Consolidator Grant “SHAPE” on Cordis Website: https://cordis.europa.eu/project/id/682859
  11. 1 2 Online Edition of The Biologist, magazine of the Royal Society of Biology: https://ocean.exacteditions.com/issues/99541/spread/39
  12. Website of the Wellcome Trust, List of Members of the Interview Panel: https://wellcome.org/grant-funding/guidance/funding-application-advisory-committees/science-interview-panel
  13. 1 2 "ERC Advanced Grants 2022 List of Principal Investigators selected for funding" (PDF).
  14. Justus Liebig University Giessen website: Table of winners of the faculty research prize:  https://www.uni-giessen.de/org/admin/stab/stf/dl/preise/tabelle_pjlu
  15. Vision Sciences Society Website, Young Investigator Award, 2013: https://www.visionsciences.org/2013-yia/
  16. Vision Sciences Society Website, Annual Public Lecture, 2021:  https://www.visionsciences.org/2021-public-lecture/
  17. 1 2 Fleming, Roland W. (2014). "Visual Perception of Materials and their Properties". Vision Research. 94: 62–75. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.11.004 . PMID   24291494. S2CID   18018810.
  18. 1 2 Fleming, RW; Dror, RO; Adelson, EH (2003). "Real world illumination and the perception of surface reflectance properties". Journal of Vision. 3 (5): 347–368. doi: 10.1167/3.5.3 . PMID   12875632.
  19. 1 2 Fleming, RW; Bülthoff, HH (2005). "Low-level image cues in the perception of translucent materials". ACM Transactions on Applied Perception. 2 (3): 346–382. doi:10.1145/1077399.1077409. S2CID   18765054.
  20. Fleming, RW; Jäkel, F; Maloney, LT (2011). "Visual Perception of Thick Transparent Materials". Psychological Science. 22 (6): 812–820. doi:10.1177/0956797611408734. PMID   21597102. S2CID   5934256.
  21. Doerschner, K; Fleming, RW; Yilmaz, O; Schrater, PR; Hartung, B; Kersten, D (2011). "Visual Motion and the Perception of Surface Material". Current Biology. 21 (23): 1–7. Bibcode:2011CBio...21.2010D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.036. PMC   3246380 . PMID   22119529.
  22. Murry, A; Welchman, AE; Blake, A; Fleming, RW (2013). "Specular reflections and the estimation of shape from binocular disparity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (6): 2413–2418. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110.2413M. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212417110 . PMC   3568321 . PMID   23341602.
  23. Murry, A; Fleming, RW; Welchman, AE (2014). "Key characteristics of specular stereo". Journal of Vision. 14 (14): 14. doi:10.1167/14.14.14. PMC   4278431 . PMID   25540263.
  24. Murry, A; Fleming, RW; Welchman, AE (2016). "'Proto-rivalry': how the binocular brain identifies gloss". Proc. R. Soc. B. 283 (1830). doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.0383. PMC   4874713 . PMID   27170713.
  25. Vangorp, P; Barla, P; Fleming, RW (2017). "The perception of hazy gloss". Journal of Vision. 17 (5): 19. doi: 10.1167/17.5.19 . PMID   28558395. S2CID   26091530.
  26. Storrs, KR; Anderson, BL; Fleming, RW (2021). "Unsupervised learning predicts human perception and misperception of gloss". Nature Human Behaviour. 5 (10): 1402–1417. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01097-6. PMC   8526360 . PMID   33958744.
  27. Prokott, KE; Tamura, H; Fleming, RW (2021). "Gloss perception: Searching for a deep neural network that behaves like humans". Journal of Vision. 21 (12): 14. doi:10.1167/jov.21.12.14. PMC   8626854 . PMID   34817568.
  28. Tamura, H; Prokott, KE; Fleming, RW (2021). "Distinguishing mirror from glass: A 'big data' approach to material perception". Journal of Vision. 22 (4): 4. doi:10.1167/jov.22.4.4. PMC   8934559 . PMID   35266961.
  29. Cheeseman, JR; Fleming, RW; Schmidt, F (2022). "Scale Ambiguities in Material Perception". iScience. 25 (3): 103970. doi:10.1016/j.isci.2022.103970. PMC   8914553 . PMID   35281732.
  30. 1 2 Paulun, VC; Schmidt, F; Van Assen, JJ; Fleming, RW (2017). "Shape, motion and optical cues to stiffness of elastic objects". Journal of Vision. 17 (1): 1–22. doi: 10.1167/17.1.20 . PMID   28114494.
  31. 1 2 Paulun, VC; Fleming, RW (2020). "Visually inferring elasticity from the motion trajectory of bouncing cubes". Journal of Vision. 20 (6): 6. doi:10.1167/jov.20.6.6. PMC   7416883 . PMID   32516356.
  32. 1 2 Paulun, VC; Kawabe, T; Nishida, S; Fleming, RW (2015). "Seeing liquids from static snapshots". Vision Research. 115 (Pt B): 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.023 . PMID   25676882. S2CID   8166492.
  33. Kawabe, T; Maruya, K; Fleming, RW; Nishida, S (2015). "Seeing liquids from visual motion". Vision Research. 109(B): 125–138. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.07.003 . PMID   25102388. S2CID   18228658.
  34. 1 2 3 Van Assen, JJ; Barla, P; Fleming, RW (2018). "Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids". Current Biology. 28 (3): 452–458. Bibcode:2018CBio...28E.452V. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.037. PMC   5807092 . PMID   29395924.
  35. Van Assen, JJ; Nishida, S; Fleming, RW (2020). "Visual perception of liquids: Insights from deep neural networks". PLOS Computational Biology. 16 (8): e1008018. Bibcode:2020PLSCB..16E8018V. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008018 . PMC   7437867 . PMID   32813688.
  36. Van Assen, JJ; Fleming, RW (2016). "Influence of optical material properties on the perception of liquids". Journal of Vision. 16 (12): 12. doi: 10.1167/16.15.12 . PMID   27973644.
  37. Schmidt, F; Paulun, VC; Van Assen, JJ; Fleming, RW (2017). "Inferring the stiffness of unfamiliar objects from optical, shape, and motion cues". Journal of Vision. 17 (18): 18. doi: 10.1167/17.3.18 . PMID   28355630.
  38. Fleming, RW (2017). "Material Perception". Annual Review of Vision Science. 3 (1): 365–388. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061429 . PMID   28697677.
  39. Roland Fleming’s lecture at the Shitsukan Symposium, Tokyo, July 2014: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UDLqxukmqkY
  40. Storrs, KR; Fleming, RW (2021). "Learning About the World by Learning About Images". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 30 (2): 120–128. doi:10.1177/0963721421990334. S2CID   233430117.
  41. Fleming, RW; Storrs, KR (2019). "Learning to See Stuff". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 30 (30): 100–108. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.07.004. PMC   6919301 . PMID   31886321.
  42. Storrs, KR; Anderson, BL; Fleming, RW (2021). "Unsupervised learning predicts human perception and misperception of gloss". Nature Human Behaviour. 5 (10): 1402–1417. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01097-6. PMC   8526360 . PMID   33958744.
  43. 1 2 3 Fleming, Roland W.; Torralba, Antonio; Adelson, Edward H. (2004-08-02). "Specular reflections and the perception of shape". Journal of Vision. 4 (9): 798–820. doi: 10.1167/4.9.10 . ISSN   1534-7362. PMID   15493971.
  44. 1 2 Weidenbacher, Ulrich; Bayerl, Pierre; Neumann, Heiko; Fleming, Roland (2006-07-01). "Sketching shiny surfaces: 3D shape extraction and depiction of specular surfaces". ACM Transactions on Applied Perception. 3 (3): 262–285. doi:10.1145/1166087.1166094. ISSN   1544-3558. S2CID   1492355.
  45. 1 2 Fleming, Roland W. (Roland William) (2004). Human visual perception under real-world illumination (Thesis thesis). Massachusetts Institute of Technology. hdl:1721.1/30112.
  46. 1 2 3 Fleming, Roland W.; Holtmann-Rice, Daniel; Bülthoff, Heinrich H. (2011-12-20). "Estimation of 3D shape from image orientations". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (51): 20438–20443. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10820438F. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114619109 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   3251077 . PMID   22147916.
  47. "Preisträgerinnen und Preisträger ab dem Jahr 1976". Justus-Liebig-Universität Gießen (in German).
  48. "VSS 2013 Young Investigator – Roland W. Fleming".
  49. 1 2 Adelson, Edward H.; Torralba, Antonio; Fleming, Roland W. (2009-10-22). "Shape from Sheen". hdl:1721.1/49511.{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  50. Cholewiak, Steven; Vergne, Romain; Kunsberg, Benjamin; Zucker, Steven; Fleming, Roland (2015-09-01). "Distinguishing between texture and shading flows for 3D shape estimation". Journal of Vision. 15 (12): 965. doi: 10.1167/15.12.965 . ISSN   1534-7362.
  51. Fleming, Roland W. (2010-08-01). "From local image measurements to 3D shape". Journal of Vision. 10 (7): 2. doi: 10.1167/10.7.2 . ISSN   1534-7362.
  52. Fleming, Roland; Li, Yuanzhen; Adelson, Edward (2008-05-02). "Image statistics for 3D shape estimation". Journal of Vision. 8 (6): 76. doi: 10.1167/8.6.76 . ISSN   1534-7362.
  53. Fleming, Roland W.; Bülthoff, Heinrich H. (2005-09-01). "Orientation fields in the perception of 3D shape". Journal of Vision. 5 (8): 525. doi: 10.1167/5.8.525 . ISSN   1534-7362.
  54. Fleming, Roland; Vergne, Romain; Zucker, Steven (2013-07-02). "Predicting the effects of illumination in shape from shading". Journal of Vision. 13 (9): 611. doi: 10.1167/13.9.611 . ISSN   1534-7362.
  55. Anderson, Barton L.; Singh, Manish; Fleming, Roland W. (2002-03-01). "The Interpolation of Object and Surface Structure". Cognitive Psychology. 44 (2): 148–190. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0765. ISSN   0010-0285. PMID   11863323. S2CID   13862953.
  56. Spröte, Patrick; Fleming, Roland W. (2013-12-01). "Concavities, negative parts, and the perception that shapes are complete". Journal of Vision. 13 (14): 3. doi: 10.1167/13.14.3 . ISSN   1534-7362. PMID   24306852.
  57. Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W.; Valsecchi, Matteo (2020-06-03). "Softness and weight from shape: Material properties inferred from local shape features". Journal of Vision. 20 (6): 2. doi:10.1167/jov.20.6.2. ISSN   1534-7362. PMC   7416911 . PMID   32492099.
  58. 1 2 Schmidt, Filipp; Spröte, Patrick; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-09-01). "Perception of shape and space across rigid transformations". Vision Research. Quantitative Approaches in Gestalt Perception. 126: 318–329. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.04.011 . ISSN   0042-6989. PMID   25937375. S2CID   205662957.
  59. 1 2 3 Spröte, Patrick; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-09-01). "Bent out of shape: The visual inference of non-rigid shape transformations applied to objects". Vision Research. Quantitative Approaches in Gestalt Perception. 126: 330–346. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.08.009 . ISSN   0042-6989. PMID   26386343. S2CID   3568661.
  60. 1 2 Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-11-01). "Visual perception of complex shape-transforming processes". Cognitive Psychology. 90: 48–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.08.002 . ISSN   0010-0285. PMID   27631704. S2CID   67271.
  61. 1 2 Spröte, Patrick; Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-11-08). "Visual perception of shape altered by inferred causal history". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 36245. Bibcode:2016NatSR...636245S. doi:10.1038/srep36245. ISSN   2045-2322. PMC   5099969 . PMID   27824094.
  62. Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W. (2018-08-16). "Identifying shape transformations from photographs of real objects". PLOS ONE. 13 (8): e0202115. Bibcode:2018PLoSO..1302115S. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202115 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   6095529 . PMID   30114202.
  63. Fleming, Roland W.; Schmidt, Filipp (2019-04-01). "Getting "fumpered": Classifying objects by what has been done to them". Journal of Vision. 19 (4): 15. doi: 10.1167/19.4.15 . ISSN   1534-7362. PMID   30952166. S2CID   96448278.
  64. 1 2 Schmidt, Filipp; Phillips, Flip; Fleming, Roland W. (2019-08-01). "Visual perception of shape-transforming processes: 'Shape Scission'". Cognition. 189: 167–180. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.006. ISSN   0010-0277. PMID   30986590. S2CID   109941237.
  65. 1 2 Phillips, Flip; Fleming, Roland W. (2020-05-26). "The Veiled Virgin illustrates visual segmentation of shape by cause". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (21): 11735–11743. Bibcode:2020PNAS..11711735P. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917565117 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   7260992 . PMID   32414926.
  66. 1 2 Morgenstern, Yaniv; Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W. (2019-12-01). "One-shot categorization of novel object classes in humans". Vision Research. 165: 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2019.09.005 . ISSN   0042-6989. PMID   31707254. S2CID   207943767.
  67. 1 2 3 Tiedemann, Henning; Morgenstern, Yaniv; Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W. (2021-05-31). "One shot generalization in humans revealed through a drawing task". bioRxiv: 2021.05.31.446461. doi: 10.1101/2021.05.31.446461 . S2CID   235305386.
  68. Morgenstern, Yaniv; Hartmann, Frieder; Schmidt, Filipp; Tiedemann, Henning; Prokott, Eugen; Maiello, Guido; Fleming, Roland W. (2021-06-01). "An image-computable model of human visual shape similarity". PLOS Computational Biology. 17 (6): e1008981. Bibcode:2021PLSCB..17E8981M. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008981 . ISSN   1553-7358. PMC   8195351 . PMID   34061825.
  69. Schmidt, Filipp; Hegele, Mathias; Fleming, Roland W. (2017-09-01). "Perceiving animacy from shape". Journal of Vision. 17 (11): 10. doi: 10.1167/17.11.10 . ISSN   1534-7362. PMID   28973562.
  70. Schmidt, Filipp; Kleis, Jasmin; Morgenstern, Yaniv; Fleming, Roland W. (2020-12-17). "The role of semantics in the perceptual organization of shape". Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 22141. Bibcode:2020NatSR..1022141S. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-79072-w. ISSN   2045-2322. PMC   7746709 . PMID   33335146.
  71. Khan, Erum Arif; Reinhard, Erik; Fleming, Roland W.; Bülthoff, Heinrich H. (2006-07-01). "Image-based material editing". ACM Transactions on Graphics. 25 (3): 654–663. doi:10.1145/1141911.1141937. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0013-D0DB-B . ISSN   0730-0301.
  72. Vergne, Romain; Barla, Pascal; Bonneau, Georges-Pierre; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-07-11). "Flow-guided warping for image-based shape manipulation". ACM Transactions on Graphics. 35 (4): 93:1–93:12. doi:10.1145/2897824.2925937. ISSN   0730-0301. S2CID   16148138.
  73. Vergne, Romain; Barla, Pascal; Fleming, Roland W.; Granier, Xavier (2012-07-01). "Surface flows for image-based shading design". ACM Transactions on Graphics. 31 (4): 94:1–94:9. doi:10.1145/2185520.2185590. ISSN   0730-0301. S2CID   13913684.
  74. Akyüz, Ahmet Oǧuz; Fleming, Roland; Riecke, Bernhard E.; Reinhard, Erik; Bülthoff, Heinrich H. (2007-07-29). "Do HDR displays support LDR content? a psychophysical evaluation". ACM Transactions on Graphics. 26 (3): 38–es. doi:10.1145/1276377.1276425. ISSN   0730-0301.
  75. Masia, Belen; Agustin, Sandra; Fleming, Roland W.; Sorkine, Olga; Gutierrez, Diego (2009-12-01). "Evaluation of reverse tone mapping through varying exposure conditions". ACM SIGGRAPH Asia 2009 papers. SIGGRAPH Asia '09. New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery. pp. 1–8. doi:10.1145/1661412.1618506. ISBN   978-1-60558-858-2. S2CID   17255756.
  76. Thompson, William; Fleming, Roland; Creem-Regehr, Sarah; Stefanucci, Jeanine Kelly (2011-06-20). Visual Perception from a Computer Graphics Perspective. New York: A K Peters/CRC Press. doi:10.1201/b10927. ISBN   978-0-429-10493-0.
  77. Maiello, Guido; Paulun, Vivian C.; Klein, Lina K.; Fleming, Roland W. (2019). "Object Visibility, Not Energy Expenditure, Accounts For Spatial Biases in Human Grasp Selection". i-Perception. 10 (1): 204166951982760. doi:10.1177/2041669519827608. ISSN   2041-6695. PMC   6390223 . PMID   30828416.
  78. 1 2 3 Klein, Lina K.; Maiello, Guido; Paulun, Vivian C.; Fleming, Roland W. (2020-08-04). "Predicting precision grip grasp locations on three-dimensional objects". PLOS Computational Biology. 16 (8): e1008081. Bibcode:2020PLSCB..16E8081K. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008081 . ISSN   1553-7358. PMC   7428291 . PMID   32750070.
  79. Maiello, Guido; Schepko, Marcel; Klein, Lina K.; Paulun, Vivian C.; Fleming, Roland W. (2021). "Humans Can Visually Judge Grasp Quality and Refine Their Judgments Through Visual and Haptic Feedback". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 14: 591898. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.591898 . ISSN   1662-453X. PMC   7835720 . PMID   33510608.
  80. 1 2 3 Paulun, Vivian C.; Gegenfurtner, Karl R.; Goodale, Melvyn A.; Fleming, Roland W. (2016-08-01). "Effects of material properties and object orientation on precision grip kinematics". Experimental Brain Research. 234 (8): 2253–2265. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4631-7. ISSN   1432-1106. PMC   4923101 . PMID   27016090.
  81. 1 2 3 4 Paulun, Vivian C.; Buckingham, Gavin; Goodale, Melvyn A.; Fleming, Roland W. (2019-03-01). "The material-weight illusion disappears or inverts in objects made of two materials". Journal of Neurophysiology. 121 (3): 996–1010. doi:10.1152/jn.00199.2018. ISSN   0022-3077. PMC   6520622 . PMID   30673359.
  82. Klein, Lina K.; Maiello, Guido; Fleming, Roland W.; Voudouris, Dimitris (2021-04-01). "Friction is preferred over grasp configuration in precision grip grasping". Journal of Neurophysiology. 125 (4): 1330–1338. doi:10.1152/jn.00021.2021. ISSN   0022-3077. PMID   33596725. S2CID   231954961.
  83. Preißler, Lucie; Jovanovic, Bianca; Munzert, Jörn; Schmidt, Filipp; Fleming, Roland W.; Schwarzer, Gudrun (2021-11-01). "Effects of visual and visual-haptic perception of material rigidity on reaching and grasping in the course of development". Acta Psychologica. 221: 103457. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103457 . ISSN   0001-6918. PMID   34883348. S2CID   244938623.
  84. Maiello, Guido; Paulun, Vivian C.; Klein, Lina K.; Fleming, Roland W. (2018). "The Sequential-Weight Illusion". i-Perception. 9 (4): 204166951879027. doi:10.1177/2041669518790275. ISSN   2041-6695. PMC   6077907 . PMID   30090321.
  85. Hartmann, Frieder; Maiello, Guido; Rothkopf, Constantin A.; Fleming, Roland W. (2023-04-21). "Estimation of Contact Regions Between Hands and Objects During Human Multi-Digit Grasping". JoVE (Journal of Visualized Experiments) (194): e64877. doi: 10.3791/64877 . ISSN   1940-087X. PMID   37154551.