Effects of sleep deprivation in space

Last updated

Studies, which include laboratory investigations (Category I) and field evaluations (Category II and Category III) of population groups that are analogous to astronauts (e.g., medical and aviation personnel), provide compelling evidence that working long shifts for extended periods of time contributes to sleep deprivation and can cause performance decrements, health problems, and other detrimental consequences, including accidents, that can affect both the worker and others.

Contents

Performance errors relative to sleep loss and extended wakefulness

A meta-analysis (Category I) that was conducted by Pilcher and Huffcutt [1] examined data that were drawn from 19 research studies to characterize the effects of sleep deprivation on specific types of human performance. Motor skills, cognitive skills, and mood were assessed in terms of: partial sleep derivation (also known as sleep deprivation), which is defined as fewer than 5 hours of sleep in a 24-hour period for 1 or more days; short-term total sleep deprivation (no sleep attained for fewer than 45 hours); and long-term sleep deprivation (no sleep attained for a period in excess of 45 hours). These researchers found that sleep-deprived subjects performed considerably worse on motor tasks, cognitive tasks, and measures of mood than did non-sleep-deprived subjects. The greatest impact on cognitive performance was seen from multiple days of partial sleep deprivation, although short- and long-term sleep deprivation also showed an effect. Meta-analyses of sleep deprivation effects in medical residents found deficits in both laboratory tasks and clinical tasks. [2]

The magnitude of the chronic partial sleep loss has been experienced by astronauts in flight [3] :90–91 [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] has been reported to negatively impact cognitive performance in multiple Category I, Category II and Category III laboratory and field studies. [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] Performance can be affected whether sleep loss is in the form of a night of substantially reduced sleep, a night of total sleep deprivation, or a series of less drastic, but more chronic, restricted sleep hours. A 1997 study by Dinges et al. [10] revealed that when sleep is restricted to the level that is commonly experienced by astronauts, a "sleep debt" accrues and, in less than 1 week, performance deficits during waking hours reach levels of serious impairment.

Chronic reduction of sleep can impact performance in a manner that is similar to that of total sleep deprivation. A study by Van Dongen et al., [15] which used 48 subjects, evaluated the specific performance effects of chronic sleep restriction in comparison to the effects of 3 nights of total sleep deprivation. Sleep restriction conditions included 14 consecutive nights of 8, 6, or 4 hours of sleep opportunity, with actual sleep quantity validated by polysomnography recordings. Subjects who were subjected to sleep restriction conditions underwent neurobehavioral assessments every 2 hours during their scheduled wakefulness, while subjects who were subjected to the sleep deprivation condition were tested every 2 hours throughout their total 88 hours of sleep deprivation.

The neurobehavioral assessment battery that was used in the Van Dongen et al. [15] study included the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT). The PVT - which determines alertness and the effects of fatigue on cognitive performance (as determined by lapses in response time and accuracy of responses) by measuring the speed with which subjects respond to a visual or auditory stimulus (by pressing a response button) - has become a standard laboratory tool for the assessment of sustained performance in a variety of experimental conditions. [16] The PVT detects changes in basic neurobehavioral performance that involve vigilant attention, response speed, and impulsivity; and it has been extensively validated in ground-based laboratory studies to detect cognitive deficits that are caused by a variety of factors (e.g., restricted sleep, sleep/wake shifts, motion sickness, residual sedation from sleep medications). [15] [17] [18] The PVT is an optimal tool for repeated use, in contrast to some other cognitive measures, as studies have shown no minimal learning effects and aptitude differences when using the PVT. [15] [19] [20]

Figure 3-2. Performance lapses for time in bed (TIB) over 14 days of sleep restriction. HHPS Figure 3-2.jpg
Figure 3–2. Performance lapses for time in bed (TIB) over 14 days of sleep restriction.

Results from these laboratory studies indicate that multiple consecutive sleep episodes of 4 or 6 hours significantly erode performance on the PVT and on measures of working memory, and that performance under these two conditions (i.e., 4 or 6 hours) was comparable to the performance that is found under conditions of 1 to 2 days of total sleep deprivation. Surprisingly, by the end of the 14 days of sleep restriction, subjects in the 4- and 6-hour sleep period conditions reported feeling only slightly sleepy. As these reports were taken when performance was at its lowest level, this indicates that the subjects may no longer have been aware of their performance deficits because of inadequate recovery sleep (figure 3–2). [15]

Subjects who spent 4 hours in bed reached levels of impairment at 6 days and of severe impairment at 11 days. Subjects who spent 6 hours in bed reached levels of impairment at 7 days. It appears that subjects who spent 8 hours in bed approached levels of impairment. Figure 3-3, which is from Belenky et al., [21] however, demonstrates that subjects who spent 9 hours in bed did not approach these levels of impairment, indicating that 9 hours in bed may be needed to alleviate the risk of performance errors.

Figure 3-3. PVT performance lapses for TIB over 7 days sleep restriction. HHPS Figure 3-3.jpg
Figure 3-3. PVT performance lapses for TIB over 7 days sleep restriction.

Similar performance effects resulting from chronically restricted can also be seen in the Category I study by Belenky et al. [21] and in figure 3-3. This study involved 66 subjects who were observed in four conditions (i.e., 3, 5, 7 and 9 hours in bed) for 7 days. PVT testing showed severe impairments in reaction time under the 3-hour condition, with lapses in responses increasing steadily across the 7 days of sleep restriction. Subjects who spent 3 hours in bed reached levels of severe impairment at 5 days, while subjects who spent 5 hours in bed reached levels of impairment at 4 days.

These Category I laboratory studies by Van Dongen et al. [15] and Belenky et al. [21] clearly show that subjects suffered performance impairments resulting from total sleep deprivation and/or chronic sleep restriction.

Cognitive impairments are present even after an individual has been awake for approximately 17 hours; in fact, recent studies have shown that these decrements are similar to those that result from an elevated blood alcohol level. A compelling Category I laboratory study from Williamson and Feyer [22] used a cross-over randomized control design to observe cognitive and motor performance after minor sleep deprivation to performance after alcohol consumption. All subjects participated in both alcohol consumption and sleep deprivation, and the order of testing was counterbalanced so that half of the subjects participated in the alcohol consumption part first while the other half participated in the sleep deprivation part first. To avoid carry-over effects from one condition to the next, subjects were provided with a night of rest in a motel between each condition.

Results indicate that, on average, performance with a blood alcohol level of 0.05% remained equivalent to performance after being awake for 16.9 to 18.6 hours. Performance with a blood alcohol level of 0.1% was equivalent to performance after being awake for 17.7 to 19.7 hours, or to restricted sleep of 4 to 5 hours per night for 1 week. [23] Similar studies that compare performance after a time of sleep deprivation to performance with elevated blood alcohol level have confirmed these results. [24] These findings are compelling as the duration of wakefulness (17 hours), which results in decrements that are similar to those that are induced by a 0.05% blood alcohol level, is considered by many to be within the range of a "normal" waking "day"; many individuals can recall an incident in which they had to waken early in the morning and work all day into the night. Astronauts, who sleep on average of 6 hours per night, [4] [6] [7] [8] may be performing critical tasks 17 hours or more after wakening.

Performance errors relative to sleep desynchronization and work overload

Research suggests that circadian desynchronization and work overload may also impair performance. Specifically, a controlled laboratory study by Wright et al. [25] evaluated the relationship between circadian rhythms and performance by assessing body temperature, which is regulated by the circadian mechanisms of the body. Body temperature is at its highest near the circadian peak and lowest near the circadian minimum (this is when the body is driven to sleep). It has long been recognized that a positive relationship exists between daily rhythms of the body temperature and neurobehavioral performance and alertness in humans. [25]

The study protocol [25] forced circadian desynchronization for 12 consecutive 28-hour days; participants were allowed 9.3 hours of scheduled time in bed and 18.7 hours of scheduled wakefulness. Performance on validated measures was evaluated every 2 hours, beginning 2 hours after the scheduled wake time. The protocol, therefore, assessed performance when the body is normally driven to sleep (which is related to the point at which body temperature is at its lowest) relative to performance during normal waking hours, and allowed for assessment of the effects of body temperature independent of (and associated with) sleep hours and time of day. During the circadian peak ( when body temperature is high), performance and alertness are high; conversely, near the circadian phase of low body temperature, performance and alertness are low. These results have been replicated in other forced desynchrony and extended wakefulness laboratory protocols.

Results from these laboratory protocols can be extrapolated to field conditions. Studies in the medical industry, where highly educated and trained individuals (e.g., physicians) are subject to circadian shifting and extended work shifts in addition to sleep loss, further demonstrate serious performance errors with populations that are analogous to astronauts. In a two-session, with subject, Category II experiment that was conducted by Arnedt et al., [26] the performance of 34 medical interns was observed under four conditions:

Performance measures included the PVT and a simulated driving task. Findings of the Arnedt et al. [26] experiment indicate that performance impairment after a heavy-call rotation is comparable to the impairment that is associated with a combined 0.04% to 0.05% blood alcohol level and a light-call rotation. Results of this experiment demonstrate that decrements that are created by extended work shifts are similar to the decrements that are created by elevated blood alcohol levels.

Work hours and sleep loss were shown to impact performance in a Category III evaluation by Rogers et al. [27] A total of 393 registered nurses logged scheduled hours worked, actual hours worked, time of day worked, overtime, days off, and sleep/wake patterns. Questions concerning errors and near-errors were also included. Analysis showed that work duration, overtime, and number of hours worked per week significantly affected the number of errors. The likelihood of making an error increased with longer work hours, and was three times higher when the nurses worked shifts lasting 12.5 hours or more. Working overtime increased the odds of making at least one error, regardless of the originally scheduled length of the shift. Working more than 40 and more than 50 hours per week significantly increased the risk of making an error.

Similar findings were attained in a subsequent Category III evaluation of 2,737 medical interns. [14] A Web-based survey was conducted across the U.S. in which interns completed 17,003 confidential monthly reports. These 60-item reports contained information concerning work hours, sleep, and activities during the month, number of days off, and the number of extended-duration work shifts (defined as at least 24 hours of continuous work). These interns were also asked to report whether they had made significant fatigue-related or non-fatigue-related medical errors. Other questions assessed how often they had nodded off or fallen asleep during patient care or educational activities.

Analysis revealed a significant relationship between the number of extended-duration work shifts and the reported rates of fatigue-related noteworthy medical errors. Specifically, the number of reported fatigue-related medical errors increased as the number of extended-duration shifts per month increased. At least one fatigue-related significant medical error was reported in 3.8% of months with no extended-duration work shifts; and at least one fatigue-related significant medical error was reported in 9.8% of months that had between one and four extended-duration work shifts and in 16% of months that had five or more extended-duration work shifts. [14] Furthermore, the frequency of attentional failures was strongly associated with the frequency of extended-duration work shifts. Evidence from this study further corroborates the negative impact that extended-duration work shifts may have on performance, as well as increased accidents and injuries. [13] [14]

Working extended hours or overnight shifts also poses the added difficulty of requiring performance from an individual at a time with the body is driven to sleep by the circadian system. Sleep, alertness, and cognitive functioning are determined by the interaction of two processes: the endogenous circadian pacemaker and the homeostatic drive for sleep. [3] :94 The endogenous circadian pacemaker generates the 24-hour circadian rhythm that regulates subjective alertness and sleep propensity as well as core body temperature, cognitive functions, and melatonin secretion, as described above. [23] It is also highly sensitive to light, which is its primary synchronization. Misalignment of the circadian rhythm results in disturbed sleep, impaired performance alertness, waking-hour melatonin secretion, and reduced levels of nocturnal secretion of growth hormone. [28] The outcome, therefore, can range from performance error to long-term health decrements.

Individuals who work at night and attempt to sleep during the day suffer because the timing of their sleep/wake schedule remains out of phase with the timing of the environmental light. Night workers are particularly prone to vehicle accidents, and their decreased alertness, performance, and vigilance are likely to blame for a higher rate of industrial accidents and quality control errors on the job, injuries and a general decline in work productivity rate. [3] :95 Recent information also suggests that as the body normally releases melatonin when it is dark, working under artificial like at night suppresses the released of melatonin, which may increase the risk of developing cancer. [29] [30] [31] [32]

In summation, ground-based evidence demonstrates that sleep loss, circadian desynchronization, and extended work shifts lead to increased performance errors and accidents. The extent to which these risk factors are also present in the space flight environment is therefore an important consideration.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jet lag</span> Physiological condition caused by travel across time zones

Jet lag is a temporary physiological condition that occurs when a person's circadian rhythm is out of sync with the time zone they are in, and is a typical result from travelling rapidly across multiple time zones. For example, someone travelling from New York to London, i.e. from west to east, feels as if the time were five hours earlier than local time, and someone travelling from London to New York, i.e. from east to west, feels as if the time were five hours later than local time. The phase shift when travelling from east to west is referred to as phase-delay of the circadian cycle, whereas going west to east is phase-advance of the cycle. Most travellers find that it is harder to adjust time zones when travelling east. Jet lag was previously classified as a circadian rhythm sleep disorder.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Circadian rhythm</span> Natural internal process that regulates the sleep-wake cycle

A circadian rhythm, or circadian cycle, is a natural oscillation that repeats roughly every 24 hours. Circadian rhythms can refer to any process that originates within an organism and responds to the environment. Circadian rhythms are regulated by a circadian clock whose primary function is to rhythmically co-ordinate biological processes so they occur at the correct time to maximize the fitness of an individual. Circadian rhythms have been widely observed in animals, plants, fungi and cyanobacteria and there is evidence that they evolved independently in each of these kingdoms of life.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Delayed sleep phase disorder</span> Chronic sleep disorder

Delayed sleep phase disorder (DSPD), more often known as delayed sleep phase syndrome and also as delayed sleep–wake phase disorder, is the delaying of a person's circadian rhythm compared to those of societal norms. The disorder affects the timing of biological rhythms including sleep, peak period of alertness, core body temperature, and hormonal cycles. People with this disorder are often called night owls.

Shift work is an employment practice designed to keep a service or production line operational at all times. The practice typically sees the day divided into shifts, set periods of time during which different groups of workers perform their duties. The term "shift work" includes both long-term night shifts and work schedules in which employees change or rotate shifts.

Sleep inertia is a physiological state of impaired cognitive and sensory-motor performance that is present immediately after awakening. It persists during the transition of sleep to wakefulness, where an individual will experience feelings of drowsiness, disorientation and a decline in motor dexterity. Impairment from sleep inertia may take several hours to dissipate. In the majority of cases, morning sleep inertia is experienced for 15 to 30 minutes after waking.

Non-24-hour sleep–wake disorder is one of several chronic circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSDs). It is defined as a "chronic steady pattern comprising [...] daily delays in sleep onset and wake times in an individual living in a society". Symptoms result when the non-entrained (free-running) endogenous circadian rhythm drifts out of alignment with the light–dark cycle in nature. Although this sleep disorder is more common in blind people, affecting up to 70% of the totally blind, it can also affect sighted people. Non-24 may also be comorbid with bipolar disorder, depression, and traumatic brain injury. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) has provided CRSD guidelines since 2007 with the latest update released in 2015.

Medical resident work hours refers to the shifts worked by medical interns and residents during their medical residency.

Shift work sleep disorder (SWSD) is a circadian rhythm sleep disorder characterized by insomnia, excessive sleepiness, or both affecting people whose work hours overlap with the typical sleep period. Insomnia can be the difficulty to fall asleep or to wake up before the individual has slept enough. About 20% of the working population participates in shift work. SWSD commonly goes undiagnosed, and it is estimated that 10–40% of shift workers have SWSD. The excessive sleepiness appears when the individual has to be productive, awake and alert. Both symptoms are predominant in SWSD. There are numerous shift work schedules, and they may be permanent, intermittent, or rotating; consequently, the manifestations of SWSD are quite variable. Most people with different schedules than the ordinary one might have these symptoms but the difference is that SWSD is continual, long-term, and starts to interfere with the individual's life.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sleep deprivation</span> Condition of not having enough sleep

Sleep deprivation, also known as sleep insufficiency or sleeplessness, is the condition of not having adequate duration and/or quality of sleep to support decent alertness, performance, and health. It can be either chronic or acute and may vary widely in severity. All known animals sleep or exhibit some form of sleep behavior, and the importance of sleep is self-evident for humans, as nearly a third of a person's life is spent sleeping. Sleep deprivation is common as it affects about one-third of the population.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Effects of fatigue on safety</span>

Fatigue is a major safety concern in many fields, but especially in transportation, because fatigue can result in disastrous accidents. Fatigue is considered an internal precondition for unsafe acts because it negatively affects the human operator's internal state. Research has generally focused on pilots, truck drivers, and shift workers.

The effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance are a broad range of impairments resulting from inadequate sleep, impacting attention, executive function and memory. An estimated 20% of adults or more have some form of sleep deprivation. It may come with insomnia or major depressive disorder, or indicate other mental disorders. The consequences can negatively affect the health, cognition, energy level and mood of a person and anyone around. It increases the risk of human error, especially with technology.

In chronobiology, a circasemidian rhythm is a physiological arousal cycle that peaks twice in a 24-hour day. It may also be called the semicircadian rhythm. Numerous studies have demonstrated that human circadian rhythms in many measures of performance and physiological activity have a 2-peak daily (circasemidian) pattern. The word, circasemidian, is based upon the Latin words circa ("about"), semi ("half") and dia ("day"). Thus, this is a rhythm that has two cycles per day, and some investigators have referred to it as the semicircadian rhythm. It usually serves to (1) deepen the pre-dawn nadir in body temperature and cognitive performance, (2) create a flat spot during the early afternoon in the daytime increase in body temperature and cognitive performance, and (3) heighten the early-evening peak in body temperature and cognitive performance. Broughton was the first to bring this characteristic of human performance to the attention of researchers.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Charles Czeisler</span> American physician and sleep researcher

Charles Andrew Czeisler is a Hungarian-American physician and sleep and circadian researcher. He is a leading researcher and author in the fields of the effects of light on human physiology, circadian rhythms and sleep medicine.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sleep in space</span> Sleep in an unusual place

Sleeping in space is part of space medicine and mission planning, with impacts on the health, capabilities and morale of astronauts.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sleep deprivation in higher education</span> Health issue in students

Sleep deprivation – the condition of not having enough sleep – is a common health issue for students in higher education. This issue has several underlying and negative consequences, but there are a few helpful improvements that students can make to reduce its frequency and severity.

A constant routine protocol is a common method used in human circadian rhythm research to study internally generated, or endogenous, circadian rhythms without the effect of external, or exogenous, influences. In the method, subjects are kept in constant conditions for at least 24 hours. These include constant light and temperature, as well as constant semi-recumbent posture. In addition, subjects' food intake is evenly distributed throughout the protocol, and subjects are typically not allowed to sleep for the duration. While in these conditions, subjects are often assessed for a number of variables of interest. Two of the most common and best understood of these variables are core body temperature and melatonin.

Dr. Debra J. Skene is a chronobiologist with specific interest in the mammalian circadian rhythm and the consequences of disturbing the circadian system. She is also interested in finding their potential treatments for people who suffer from circadian misalignment. Skene and her team of researchers tackle these questions using animal models, clinical trials, and most recently, liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry. Most notably, Skene is credited for her evidence of a novel photopigment in humans, later discovered to be melanopsin. She was also involved in discovering links between human PER3 genotype and an extremely shifted sleep schedules categorized as extreme diurnal preference. Skene received her Bachelor of Pharmacy, Master of Science, and Ph.D. in South Africa.

Anne-Marie Chang is a researcher in the fields of sleep and circadian rhythms and is currently an Assistant Professor of Bio-behavioral Health at Pennsylvania State University. She earned her Ph.D. in neuroscience from Northwestern University in 2003.

Elizabeth Klerman is a professor of neurology at Harvard Medical School. Her research focuses on applying circadian and sleep research principles to human physiology and pathophysiology. She also uses mathematical analysis and modeling to study human circadian, sleep, and objective neurobehavioral performance and subjective (self-reported) mood and alertness rhythms.

Chronodisruption is a concept in the field of circadian biology that refers to the disturbance or alteration of the body's natural biological rhythms, for example the sleep-wake cycle, due to various environmental factors. The human body is synchronized to a 24-hour light-dark cycle, which is essential for maintaining optimal health and well-being. However, modern lifestyles —which involve exposure to artificial light, irregular sleep schedules, and shift work — can disrupt this natural rhythm, leading to a range of adverse physiological outcomes. Chronodisruption has been linked to a variety of health disorders and diseases, including neurodegenerative diseases, diabetes, mood disorders, cardiovascular disease, and cancer. Such disruptors can lead to dysregulation of hormones and neurotransmitters, though researchers continue to investigate the physiological implications of chronodisruption. Indeed, research in chronobiology is rapidly advancing, with an increasing focus on understanding the underlying mechanisms of chronodisruption and developing strategies to prevent or mitigate its adverse effects. This includes the development of pharmacological interventions, as well as lifestyle modifications such as optimizing one's sleeping environment and timing of meals and physical activity.

References

  1. Pilcher, June J.; Huffcutt, Allen I. (1996). "Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Performance: A Meta-Analysis". Sleep . 19 (4). American Sleep Disorders Association & Sleep Research Society: 318–326. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.4.318 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   8776790.
  2. Philibert, Ingrid (November 2005). "Sleep loss and performance in residents and nonphysicians: A meta-analytic examination". Sleep . 28 (11): 1392–1402. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.11.1392 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   16335329.
  3. 1 2 3 Whitmire, Alexandra M.; Leveton, Lauren B.; Barger, Laura; Brainard, George; Dinges, David F.; Klerman, Elizabeth; Shea, Camille (2009). "Risk of Performance Errors due to Sleep Loss, Circadian Desynchronization, Fatigue, and Work Overload". In McPhee, Jancy C.; Charles, John B. (eds.). Human Health and Performance Risks of Space Exploration Missions (PDF). NASA. pp. 90–95. ISBN   978-0-16-084634-2. LCCN   2010368251. OCLC   549504351. OL   24387319M. SP-2009-3405. Archived (PDF) from the original on 22 June 2024.PD-icon.svg This article incorporates text from this source, which is in the public domain .
  4. 1 2 Mallis, MM; Deroshia, CW (2005). "Circadian rhythms, sleep, and performance in space". Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine . 76 (6 Suppl): B94–107. ISSN   0095-6562. PMID   15943202.
  5. Dijk, Derk-Jan; Neri, David F.; Wyatt, James K.; Ronda, Joseph M.; Riel, Eymard; Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Hughes, Rod J.; Elliott, Ann R.; et al. (November 2001). "Sleep, performance, circadian rhythms, and light-dark cycles during two space shuttle flights". American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology . 281 (5): R1647–1664. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2001.281.5.R1647 . ISSN   1522-1490. PMID   11641138.
  6. 1 2 Kelly, Thomas H.; Hienz, Robert D.; Zarcone, Troy J.; Wurster, Richard M.; Brady, Joseph V. (September 2005). "Crewmember Performance Before, During, and After Spaceflight". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior . 84 (2): 227–241. doi:10.1901/jeab.2005.77-04. ISSN   0022-5002. PMC   1243980 . PMID   16262187.
  7. 1 2 Gundel, A.; Polyakov, V.V.; Zulley, J. (March 1997). "The alteration of human sleep and circadian rhythms during spaceflight". Journal of Sleep Research. 6 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00028.x. ISSN   0962-1105. PMID   9125693.
  8. 1 2 Santy, PA; Kapanka, H; Davis, JR; Stewart, DF (1988). "Analysis of sleep on Shuttle missions". Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine . 59 (11 Pt 1): 1094–7. ISSN   0095-6562. PMID   3202794.
  9. Frost Jr, JD; Shumate, WH; Salamy, JG; Booher, CR (1976). "Sleep monitoring: The second manned Skylab mission". Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine . 47 (4): 372–82. ISSN   0095-6562. PMID   179518.
  10. 1 2 Dinges, David F.; Pack, Frances; Williams, Katherine; Gillen, Kelly A.; Powell, John W.; Ott, Geoffrey E.; Aptowicz, Caitlin; Pack, Allan I. (April 1997). "Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4–5 Hours per Night". Sleep . 20 (4): 267–277. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.4.267 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   9231952.
  11. Lockley, Steven W.; Cronin, John W.; Evans, Erin E.; Cade, Brian E.; Lee, Clark J.; Landrigan, Christopher P.; Rothschild, Jeffrey M.; Katz, Joel T.; et al. (28 October 2004). "Effect of Reducing Interns' Weekly Work Hours on Sleep and Attentional Failures". The New England Journal of Medicine . 351 (18): 1829–1837. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041404 . PMID   15509816.
  12. Landrigan, Christopher P.; Rothschild, Jeffrey M.; Cronin, John W.; Kaushal, Rainu; Burdick, Elisabeth; Katz, Joel T.; Lilly, Craig M.; Stone, Peter H.; et al. (28 October 2004). "Effect of Reducing Interns' Work Hours on Serious Medical Errors in Intensive Care Units". New England Journal of Medicine. 351 (18): 1838–1848. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041406 . PMID   15509817.
  13. 1 2 Ayas, Najib T.; Barger, Laura K.; Cade, Brian E.; Hashimoto, Dean M.; Rosner, Bernard; Cronin, John W.; Speizer, Frank E.; Czeisler, Charles A. (6 September 2006). "Extended Work Duration and the Risk of Self-reported Percutaneous Injuries in Interns". JAMA . 296 (9). American Medical Association: 1055–1062. doi:10.1001/jama.296.9.1055. eISSN   1538-3598. ISSN   0098-7484. PMID   16954484.
  14. 1 2 3 4 Barger, Laura K.; Ayas, Najib T.; Cade, Brian E.; Cronin, John W.; Rosner, Bernard; Speizer, Frank E.; Czeisler, Charles A. (12 December 2006). "Impact of Extended-Duration Shifts on Medical Errors, Adverse Events, and Attentional Failures". PLOS Medicine . 3 (12): 2440–2448. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030487 . eISSN   1549-1676. ISSN   1549-1277. PMC   1705824 . PMID   17194188. Archived from the original on 19 August 2024.
  15. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Van Dongen, Hans P.A.; Maislin, Greg; Mullington, Janet M.; Dinges, David F. (March 2003). "The Cumulative Cost of Additional Wakefulness: Dose-Response Effects on Neurobehavioral Functions and Sleep Physiology From Chronic Sleep Restriction and Total Sleep Deprivation". Sleep . 26 (2): 117–26. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   12683469.
  16. Dorrian, Jillian; Rogers; Dinges, David F. (2005). "Psychomotor Vigilance Performance: Neurocognitive Assay Sensitive to Sleep Loss". In Kushida, Clete A. (ed.). Sleep deprivation: clinical issues, pharmacology and sleep loss effects (PDF). Lung Biology in Health and Disease. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker. pp. 39–70. ISBN   978-0-8247-2094-0. LCCN   2005043209. OCLC   57422390. OL   22624575M. Archived from the original (PDF) on 25 June 2009.
  17. Dinges, David F.; Powell, John W. (November 1985). "Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations". Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 17 (6): 652–655. doi: 10.3758/BF03200977 . ISSN   1532-5970.
  18. Drummond, Sean P. A.; Bischoff-Grethe, Amanda; Dinges, David F.; Ayalon, Liat; Mednick, Sara C.; Meloy, M. J. (September 2005). "The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task". Sleep . 28 (9): 1059–1068. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.9.1059 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   16268374.
  19. Balkin, Thomas J.; Bliese, Paul D.; Belenky, Gregory; Sing, Helen; Thorne, David R.; Thomas, Maria; Redmond, Daniel P.; Russo, Michael; Wesensten, Nancy J. (September 2004). "Comparative utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness-related performance decrements in the operational environment". Journal of Sleep Research. 13 (3): 219–227. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00407.x. ISSN   0962-1105. PMID   15339257.
  20. Dorrian, Jillian; Lamond, Nicole; Holmes, Alexandra L.; Burgess, Helen J.; Roach, Gregory D.; Fletcher, Adam; Dawson, Drew (October 2003). "The ability to self-monitor performance during a week of simulated night shifts". Sleep . 26 (7): 871–877. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.7.871 . eISSN   1550-9109. ISSN   0161-8105. PMID   14655922.
  21. 1 2 3 4 Belenky, Gregory; Wesensten, Nancy J.; Thorne, David R.; Thomas, Maria L.; Sing, Helen C.; Redmond, Daniel P.; Russo, Michael B.; Balkin, Thomas J. (21 February 2003). "Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: A sleep dose-response study". Journal of Sleep Research. 12 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x. ISSN   0962-1105. PMID   12603781.
  22. Williamson, A M; Feyer, Anne-Marie (2000). "Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor performance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication". Occupational and Environmental Medicine . 57 (10): 649–655. doi:10.1136/oem.57.10.649. eISSN   1470-7926. ISSN   1351-0711. PMC   1739867 . PMID   10984335.
  23. 1 2 Fryer, Bronwyn; Czeisler, Charles A. (October 2006). "Sleep deficit: The performance killer - A conversation with Harvard Medical School Professor Charles A. Czeisler". Harvard Business Review . 84 (10): 53–59. ISSN   0017-8012. PMID   17040040.
  24. Dawson, Drew; Reid, Kathryn (17 July 1997). "Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment". Nature . 388 (6639): 235. Bibcode:1997Natur.388..235D. doi:10.1038/40775. PMID   9230429.
  25. 1 2 3 Wright, Kenneth P.; Hull, Joseph T.; Czeisler, Charles A. (December 2002). "Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans". American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology . 283 (6): R1370–R1377. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.1030.9291 . doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00205.2002 . ISSN   0363-6119. PMID   12388468.
  26. 1 2 Arnedt, J. Todd; Owens, Judith; Crouch, Megan; Stahl, Jessica; Carskadon, Mary A. (7 September 2005). "Neurobehavioral Performance of Residents After Heavy Night Call vs After Alcohol Ingestion". JAMA . 294 (9). American Medical Association: 1025–1033. doi:10.1001/jama.294.9.1025. eISSN   1538-3598. ISSN   0098-7484. PMID   16145022.
  27. Rogers, Ann E.; Hwang, Wei-Ting; Scott, Linda D.; Aiken, Linda H.; Dinges, David F. (2004). "The Working Hours of Hospital Staff Nurses and Patient Safety". Health Affairs . 23 (4): 202–212. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.23.4.202. ISSN   0278-2715. PMID   15318582.
  28. Ball, John R.; Evans, Charles H. Jr., eds. (2001). Safe passage: Astronaut care for exploration missions (Online-ausg. ed.). Washington, D.C.: Institute of Medicine/National Academies Press. doi:10.17226/10218. ISBN   978-0-309-07585-5. LCCN   2001005443. OCLC   704482825. OL   10358430M. PMID   25057582.
  29. Blask, David E.; Dauchy, Robert T.; Sauer, Leonard A.; Krause, Jean A.; Brainard, George C. (2002). "Light during darkness, melatonin suppression and cancer progression" (PDF). Neuroendocrinology Letters . 23 (Suppl 2): 52–6. ISSN   0172-780X. PMID   12163849. Archived (PDF) from the original on 20 May 2024.
  30. Glickman, Gena; Levin, Robert; Brainard, George C. (2002). "Ocular input for human melatonin regulation: Relevance to breast cancer" (PDF). Neuroendocrinology Letters . 23 (Suppl 2): 17–22. ISSN   0172-780X. PMID   12163843. Archived (PDF) from the original on 3 June 2024.
  31. Blask, David E.; Brainard, George C.; Dauchy, Robert T.; Hanifin, John P.; Davidson, Leslie K.; Krause, Jean A.; Sauer, Leonard A.; Rivera-Bermudez, Moises A.; et al. (December 2005). "Melatonin-Depleted Blood from Premenopausal Women Exposed to Light at Night Stimulates Growth of Human Breast Cancer Xenografts in Nude Rats". Cancer Research . 65 (23). American Association for Cancer Research: 11174–11184. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-05-1945 . eISSN   1538-7445. ISSN   0008-5472. PMID   16322268.
  32. Stevens, Richard G.; Blask, David E.; Brainard, George C.; Hansen, Johnni; Lockley, Steven W.; Provencio, Ignacio; Rea, Mark S.; Reinlib, Leslie (14 June 2007). "Meeting Report: The Role of Environmental Lighting and Circadian Disruption in Cancer and Other Diseases". Environmental Health Perspectives . 115 (9). National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences: 1357–1362. doi: 10.1289/ehp.10200 . PMC   1964886 . PMID   17805428.

PD-icon.svg This article incorporates public domain material from Human Health and Performance Risks of Space Exploration Missions (PDF). National Aeronautics and Space Administration. (NASA SP-2009-3405, pp. 90-95).