Clusivity

Last updated

In linguistics, clusivity [1] is a grammatical distinction between inclusive and exclusive first-person pronouns and verbal morphology, also called inclusive "we" and exclusive "we". Inclusive "we" specifically includes the addressee, while exclusive "we" specifically excludes the addressee; in other words, two (or more) words that both translate to "we", one meaning "you and I, and possibly someone else", the other meaning "me and some other person or persons, but not you". While imagining that this sort of distinction could be made in other persons (particularly the second) is straightforward, in fact the existence of second-person clusivity (you vs. you and they) in natural languages is controversial and not well attested. [2] While clusivity is not a feature of standard English language, it is found in many languages around the world.

Contents

The first published description of the inclusive-exclusive distinction by a European linguist was in a description of languages of Peru in 1560 by Domingo de Santo Tomás in his Grammatica o arte de la lengua general de los indios de los Reynos del Perú, published in Valladolid, Spain. [3]

Schematic paradigm

Sets of reference: Inclusive form (left) and exclusive form (right) Inclusive-exclusive.jpg
Sets of reference: Inclusive form (left) and exclusive form (right)

Clusivity paradigms may be summarized as a two-by-two grid:

Includes the addressee?
YesNo
Includes
the speaker?
YesInclusive we:
I and you (and possibly they)
Exclusive we:
I and they, but not you
Noyou / yous(e) / y'allthey

Morphology

In some languages, the three first-person pronouns appear to be unrelated roots. That is the case for Chechen, which has singular со (so), exclusive тхо (txo), and inclusive вай (vay). In others, however, all three are transparently simple compounds, as in Tok Pisin, an English creole spoken in Papua New Guinea, which has singular mi, exclusive mi-pela, and inclusive yu-mi (a compound of mi with yu "you") or yu-mi-pela. However, when only one of the plural pronouns is related to the singular, that may be the case for either one. In some dialects of Mandarin Chinese, for example, inclusive or exclusive 我們我们wǒmen is the plural form of singular "I", and inclusive 咱們咱们zánmen is a separate root. However, in Hadza, the inclusive, ’one-be’e, is the plural of the singular ’ono (’one-) "I", and the exclusive, ’oo-be’e, is a separate [ dubious ] root.[ citation needed ]

It is not uncommon for two separate words for "I" to pluralize into derived words, which have a clusivity distinction. For example, in Vietnamese, the familiar word for "I" (ta) pluralizes to inclusive we (chúng ta), and the formal or cold word for "I" (tôi) pluralizes into exclusive we (chúng tôi). In Samoan, the singular form of the exclusive is the regular word for "I", and the singular form of the inclusive may also occur on its own and then also means "I" but with a connotation of appealing or asking for indulgence.

In the Kunama language of Eritrea, the first-person inclusive and exclusive distinction is marked on dual and plural forms of verbs, independent pronouns, and possessive pronouns. [4]

Distinction in verbs

Where verbs are inflected for person, as in the native languages of Australia and in many Native American languages, the inclusive-exclusive distinction can be made there as well. For example, in Passamaquoddy, "I/we have it" is expressed

Singular n-tíhin (first person prefix n-)
Exclusive n-tíhin-èn (first person n- + plural suffix -èn)
Inclusive k-tíhin-èn (inclusive prefix k- + plural -èn)

In Tamil, on the other hand, the two different pronouns have the same agreement in the verb.

First-person clusivity

First-person clusivity is a common feature among Dravidian, Kartvelian, and Caucasian languages, [5] Australian and Austronesian languages, and is also found in languages of eastern, southern, and southwestern Asia, Americas, and in some creole languages. Some African languages also make the distinction, such as the Fula language. No European language outside the Caucasus makes this distinction grammatically, but some constructions[ example needed ] may be semantically inclusive or exclusive.

Singular inclusive forms

Several Polynesian languages, such as Samoan and Tongan, have clusivity with overt dual and plural suffixes in their pronouns. The lack of a suffix indicates the singular. The exclusive form is used in the singular as the normal word for "I", but the inclusive also occurs in the singular. The distinction is one of discourse: the singular inclusive has been described as the "modesty I" in Tongan. It is often rendered in English as one, but in Samoan, its use has been described as indicating emotional involvement on the part of the speaker.

Second-person clusivity

In theory, clusivity of the second person should be a possible distinction, but its existence is controversial. Clusivity in the second person is conceptually simple but nonetheless if it exists is extremely rare, unlike clusivity in the first. Hypothetical second-person clusivity would be the distinction between "you and you (and you and you ... all present)" and "you (one or more addressees) and someone else whom I am not addressing currently." These are often referred to in the literature as "2+2" and "2+3", respectively (the numbers referring to second and third person as appropriate).

Some notable linguists, such as Bernard Comrie, [6] have attested that the distinction is extant in spoken natural languages, while others, such as John Henderson, [7] maintain that a clusivity distinction in the second person is too complex to process. Many other linguists take the more neutral position that it could exist but is nonetheless not currently attested. [2] Horst J. Simon provides a deep analysis of second-person clusivity in his 2005 article. [2] He concludes that oft-repeated rumors regarding the existence of second-person clusivity—or indeed, any [+3] pronoun feature beyond simple exclusive we [8] – are ill-founded, and based on erroneous analysis of the data.

Distribution of the clusivity distinction

The inclusive–exclusive distinction occurs nearly universally among the Austronesian languages and the languages of northern Australia, but rarely in the nearby Papuan languages. (Tok Pisin, an English-Melanesian creole, generally has the inclusive–exclusive distinction, but this varies with the speaker's language background.) It is widespread in India, featuring in the Dravidian and Munda languages, as well as in several Indo-European languages of India such as Oriya, Marathi, Rajasthani, Punjabi, Dakhini, and Gujarati (which either borrowed it from Dravidian or retained it as a substratum while Dravidian was displaced). It can also be found in the languages of eastern Siberia, such as Tungusic, as well as northern Mandarin Chinese. In indigenous languages of the Americas, it is found in about half the languages, with no clear geographic or genealogical pattern. It is also found in a few languages of the Caucasus and Sub-Saharan Africa, such as Fulani, and Khoekhoe. [9] [10]

It is, of course, possible in any language to express the idea of clusivity semantically, and many languages provide common forms that clarify the ambiguity of their first person pronoun (English "the rest of us", Italian noialtri). A language with a true clusivity distinction, however, does not provide a first-person plural with indefinite clusivity in which the clusivity of the pronoun is ambiguous; rather, speakers are forced to specify by the choice of pronoun or inflection, whether they are including the addressee or not. That rules out most European languages, for example. Clusivity is nonetheless a very common language feature overall. Some languages with more than one plural number make the clusivity distinction only in, for example, the dual but not in the greater plural, but other languages make it in all numbers. In the table below, the plural forms are the ones preferentially listed.

Examples of the clusivity distinction in specific languages
LanguageInclusive formExclusive formSingular related toNotesLanguage family
Ainu a-/an-ci- ?? Ainu
American Sign Language (ASL)"K" tips up palm in (dual)

"1" tap chest + twist (pl)

"K" tips up palm side (dual)

"1" tap each side of chest (pl)

Exclusive plural French Sign Language
Anishinaabemowin giinwiniinwiBothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person singular, and the exclusive form is derived from first-person singular. Algonquian
Apma kidigemaNeitherSubject prefixes are ta- (inclusive) and kaa(ma)- (exclusive). There are also dual forms, which are derived from the plurals. Austronesian
Aymara jiwasanayaExclusiveThe derived form jiwasanaka of the inclusive refers to at least three people. Aymaran
Bikol kitakamiNeither Austronesian
Bislama yumimifalaBothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person pronoun and the first-person pronoun. There are also dual and trial forms. English creole
Cebuano kitakamiNeitherShort forms are ta (incl.) and mi (excl.) Austronesian
Chechen vaitxoNeither Caucasian
Cherokee ᎢᏂ (ini- (dual)), ᎢᏗ (idi- (plural))ᎣᏍᏗ (osdi- (dual)), ᎣᏥ (oji- (plural))NeitherThe forms given here are the active verb agreement prefixes. Free pronouns do not distinguish clusivity. Iroquoian
Cree, Plains ᑭᔮᓇᐤ
(kiyānaw)
ᓂᔭᓈᐣ
(niyanān)
BothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person singular pronoun, and the exclusive form is derived from the first-person singular. Algonquian
Cree, Moose ᑮᓛᓈᐤ
(kîlânâw)
ᓃᓛᐣ
(nîlân)
BothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person singular pronoun, and the exclusive form is derived from the first-person singular. Algonquian
Cree, Swampy ᑮᓈᓇᐤ
(kīnānaw)
ᓃᓇᓈᐣ
(nīnanān)
BothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person singular, whereas the exclusive form is derived from the first-person singular. Algonquian
Cree, Woods ᑮᖭᓇᐤ
(kīthānaw)
ᓂᖬᓈᐣ
(nīthanān)
BothThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person singular, and the exclusive form is derived from the first-person singular. Algonquian
Dakhini apan اپنham ہم Indo-European
Daur baabiede ?? Mongolic
Evenki mit ?? Tungusic
Fijian kedatou†keitou ("we" but excludes the person spoken to)"kedaru" also means "we" but is limited to the speaker and the person spoken to and can be translated as "you and me".
† ("we" but includes both the person spoken to and the speaker as part of a finite group. To refer to a much larger group, like humanity or a race of people, "keda" is used instead.
Austronesian
Fula 𞤫𞤲 (en), 𞤫𞤯𞤫𞤲 (eɗen)𞤥𞤭𞤲 (min), 𞤥𞤭𞤯𞤫𞤲 (miɗen)Exclusive (?)Examples show short- and long-form subject pronouns. Niger-Congo
Guarani ñandeoreNeither Tupi-Guarani
Gujarati આપણે /aˑpəɳ(eˑ)/અમે /əmeˑ/Exclusive Indo-European
Hadza onebeeôbeeInclusive Hadza (isolate)
Hawaiian kāua (dual); kākou (plural)māua (dual); mākou (plural)Neither Austronesian
Ilocano datayó, sitayódakamí, sikamíNeitherThe dual inclusives datá and sitá are widely used. Austronesian
Juǀʼhoan mtsá (dual); m, mǃá (plural)ètsá (dual); è, èǃá (plural)NeitherThe plural pronouns è and m are short forms. Kxʼa
Kapampangan ikatamuikamiNeitherThe dual inclusive ikata is widely used. Austronesian
Australian Kriol yunmimelabatExclusiveThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person pronoun and the first-person pronoun. The exclusive form is derived from the first-person singular and the third-person plural. There are significant dialectal and diachronic variations in the exclusive form. English creole
Lakota uŋ(k)-uŋ(k)- ... -piNeitherThe inclusive form has a dual number. By adding the suffix -pi, it takes the plural number. In the plural form, no clusivity distinction is made. Siouan
Lojban mi'omi'a/miBothThere is also the form ma'a, which means the speaker, the listener, and others unspecified. However, the first-person pronoun mi take no number and can refer to any number of individuals in the same group; mi'a and mi'o are usually preferred. Constructed language
Maguindanaon sekitanusekamiNeither Austronesian
Malagasy IsikaIzahay Austronesian
Manchu musebeExclusive Tungusic
Malay kitakamiNeitherThe exclusive form is hardly used in informal Indonesian in the Jakarta area. Instead, kita is almost always used colloquially to indicate both inclusive and exclusive "we". However, in more formal circumstances (both written and spoken), the distinction is clear and well-practiced. Therefore, kami is absolutely exclusive, but kita may generally mean both inclusive and exclusive "we" depending on the circumstances. That phenomenon is less frequently encountered in Malaysian. Austronesian
Malayalam നമ്മൾ (nammaḷ)ഞങ്ങൾ (ñaṅṅaḷ)Exclusive Dravidian
Mandarin 咱們 / 咱们 (zánmen)我們 / 我们 (wǒmen)Exclusive我们 is used both inclusively and exclusively by most speakers, especially in formal situations. Use of 咱们 is common only in northern dialects, notably that of Beijing, and may be from Manchu influence. [11] Sino-Tibetan
Māori tāua (dual); tātou (plural)māua (dual); mātou (plural)Neither Austronesian
Marathi आपण /aˑpəɳ/आम्ही /aˑmʱiˑ/Exclusive Indo-European
Marwari /aˑpãˑ//mɦẽˑ/Exclusive Indo-European
Southern Min 咱 (lán)阮 (goán/gún)Exclusive Sino-Tibetan
Newar language Jhi: sa:n (झि:सं:)Jim sa:n (जिम् सं:)Both are used as possessive pronouns. Sino-Tibetan
Pohnpeian kitail (plural), kita (dual)kiht (independent), se (subject)There is an independent (non-verbal) and subject (verbal) pronoun distinction. The exclusive includes both dual and plural, and the independent has a dual-plural distinction. Austronesian
Potawatomi ginan, g- -men (short), gde- -men (long)ninan, n- -men(short), nde- -men (long)??Potawatomi has two verb conjugation types, "short" and "long." Both are listed here, and labeled as such. Central Algonquian
Punjabi ਆਪਾਂ (apan)ਅਸੀਂ (asin) Indo-European
Quechuan languages ñuqanchikñuqaykuBoth Quechuan
Shawnee kiilaweniilaweExclusiveThe inclusive form is morphologically derived from the second-person pronoun kiila. Algic
Tagalog táyokamíNeither Austronesian
Tausug kitaniyukami ??The dual inclusive is kita. Austronesian
Tamil நாம் (nām)நாங்கள் (nāṅkaḷ)Exclusive Dravidian
Telugu మనము (manamu)మేము (memu)Exclusive Dravidian
Tetum itaamiNeither Austronesian
Tok Pisin yumipelamipelaExclusiveThe inclusive form is derived from the second-person pronoun and the first-person pronoun. There are also dual and trial forms. English creole
Tupi îandéoréInclusive Tupi-Guarani
Tulu namayenkul Dravidian
Udmurt [12] as'meos (асьмеос); [12] ваньмы ("we all together") [13] mi (ми)exclusiveThe form as'meos seem to become obsolete and 'mi' is always used instead by younger generations, probably under the influence of Russian 'my' (мы) for 'we'. [12] Permic
Vietnamese chúng tachúng tôiExclusiveThe exclusive form is derived from the formal form of I, tôi Austroasiatic

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dravidian languages</span> Language family mostly of southern India

The Dravidian languages are a family of languages spoken by 250 million people, mainly in southern India, north-east Sri Lanka, and south-west Pakistan. Dravidian is first attested in the 2nd century BCE, as inscriptions in Tamil-Brahmi script on cave walls in the Madurai and Tirunelveli districts of Tamil Nadu.

In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase.

In linguistics, grammatical person is the grammatical distinction between deictic references to participant(s) in an event; typically, the distinction is between the speaker, the addressee, and others. A language's set of pronouns is typically defined by grammatical person. First person includes the speaker, second person is the person or people spoken to, and third person includes all that are not listed above. It also frequently affects verbs and sometimes nouns or possessive relationships.

In linguistics, grammatical number is a feature of nouns, pronouns, adjectives and verb agreement that expresses count distinctions. English and other languages present number categories of singular or plural, both of which are cited by using the hash sign (#) or by the numero signs "No." and "Nos." respectively. Some languages also have a dual, trial and paucal number or other arrangements.

In Modern English, we is a plural, first-person pronoun.

In Modern English, you is the second-person pronoun. It is grammatically plural, and was historically used only for the dative case, but in most modern dialects is used for all cases and numbers.

Kwadi was a "click language" once spoken in the southwest corner of Angola. It went extinct some time around 1960. There were only fifty Kwadi in the 1950s, of whom only 4–5 were competent speakers of the language. Three partial speakers were known in 1965, but in 1981 no speakers could be found.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Yuchi language</span> Language of the Yuchi people in the southeastern United States

Yuchi or Euchee is the language of the Tsoyaha, also known as the Yuchi people, now living in Oklahoma. Historically, they lived in what is now known as the southeastern United States, including eastern Tennessee, western Carolinas, northern Georgia, and Alabama, during the period of early European colonization. Many speakers of the Yuchi language became allied with the Muscogee Creek when they migrated into their territory in Georgia and Alabama. They were forcibly relocated with them to Indian Territory in the early 19th century.

Personal pronouns are pronouns that are associated primarily with a particular grammatical person – first person, second person, or third person. Personal pronouns may also take different forms depending on number, grammatical or natural gender, case, and formality. The term "personal" is used here purely to signify the grammatical sense; personal pronouns are not limited to people and can also refer to animals and objects.

The Ojibwe language is an Algonquian American Indian language spoken throughout the Great Lakes region and westward onto the northern plains. It is one of the largest American Indian languages north of Mexico in terms of number of speakers, and exhibits a large number of divergent dialects. For the most part, this article describes the Minnesota variety of the Southwestern dialect. The orthography used is the Fiero Double-Vowel System.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anindilyakwa language</span> Indigenous Australian language

Anindilyakwa is an Australian Aboriginal language spoken by the Anindilyakwa people on Groote Eylandt and Bickerton Island in the Gulf of Carpentaria in the Northern Territory of Australia. Anindilyakwa is a multiple-classifying prefixing language in which all traditional nouns, adjectives, personal and demonstrative pronouns are prefixed for person, number and gender. According to the 2021 Australian Census, Anindilyakwa was spoken natively by 1,516 people, an increase from 1,283 in 2006.

Ughele is an Oceanic language spoken by about 1200 people on Rendova Island, located in the Western Province of the Solomon Islands.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jemez language</span> Tanoan language

Jemez is a Tanoan language spoken by the Jemez Pueblo people in New Mexico. It has no common written form, as tribal rules do not allow the language to be transcribed; linguists describing the language use the Americanist phonetic notation.

Universal is an Esperantido, a constructed language based on Esperanto. It has inclusive and exclusive pronouns, uses partial reduplication for the plural, and inversion for antonyms. Inversion can be seen in,

In linguistics, an honorific is a grammatical or morphosyntactic form that encodes the relative social status of the participants of the conversation. Distinct from honorific titles, linguistic honorifics convey formality FORM, social distance, politeness POL, humility HBL, deference, or respect through the choice of an alternate form such as an affix, clitic, grammatical case, change in person or number, or an entirely different lexical item. A key feature of an honorific system is that one can convey the same message in both honorific and familiar forms—i.e., it is possible to say something like "The soup is hot" in a way that confers honor or deference on one of the participants of the conversation.

Warndarrang (waɳʈaraŋ), also spelt Warndarang, Wanderang, Wandaran, and other variants is an extinct Aboriginal Australian language in the Arnhem family, formerly spoken by the Warndarrang people in southern Arnhem Land, along the Gulf of Carpentaria. The last speaker was Isaac Joshua, who died in 1974, while working with the linguist Jeffrey Heath.

Kéo or Nagé-Kéo is a Malayo-Polynesian dialect cluster spoken by the Kéo and Nage people that reside in an area southeast of the Ebu Lobo volcano in the south-central part of Nusa Tenggara Timur Province on the island of Flores, eastern Indonesia.

Ponosakan is an Austronesian language spoken in the vicinity of the town of Belang, North Sulawesi, Indonesia. This language is almost extinct, with only four fluent speakers left as of November 2014.

The grammar of the Massachusett language shares similarities with the grammars of related Algonquian languages. Nouns have gender based on animacy, based on the Massachusett world-view of what has spirit versus what does not. A body would be animate, but the parts of the body are inanimate. Nouns are also marked for obviation, with nouns subject to the topic marked apart from nouns less relevant to the discourse. Personal pronouns distinguish three persons, two numbers, inclusive and exclusive first-person plural, and proximate/obviative third-persons. Nouns are also marked as absentative, especially when referring to lost items or deceased persons.

The T–V distinction is a contrast, within one language, between various forms of addressing one's conversation partner or partners. This may be specialized for varying levels of politeness, social distance, courtesy, familiarity, age or insult toward the addressee. The distinction occurs in a number of the world's languages.

References

  1. Filimonova, Elena (30 November 2005). Clusivity: Typology and case studies of the inclusive-exclusive distinction. John Benjamins Publishing. ISBN   9789027293886 via Google Books.
  2. 1 2 3 Simon, Horst J. (2005). "Only you? Philological investigations into the alleged inclusive-exclusive distinction in the second person plural" (PDF). In Filimonova, Elena (ed.). Clusivity: Typology and case studies of the inclusive-exclusive distinction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-06-09. Retrieved 2010-08-02.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  3. Mary Haas. 1969. "Exclusive" and "inclusive": A look at early usage. International Journal of American Linguistics 35:1-6. JSTOR   1263878.
  4. Thompson, E. D. 1983. "Kunama: phonology and noun phrase" in M. Lionel Bender (ed.): Nilo-Saharan Language Studies, pp. 280–322. East Lansing: African Studies Center, Michigan State University.
  5. Awde, Nicholas; Galaev, Muhammad (22 May 2014). Chechen-English English-Chechen Dictionary and Phrasebook. Routledge. ISBN   9781136802331 via Google Books.
  6. Comrie, Bernard. 1980. "Review of Greenberg, Joseph H. (ed.), Universals of human language, Volume 3: Word Structure (1978)". Language 56: p837, as quoted in Simon 2005. Quote: One pair of combinations not discussed is the opposition between 2nd person non-singular inclusive (i.e. including some third person) and exclusive, which is attested in Southeast Ambrym.
  7. Henderson, T.S.T. 1985. "Who are we, anyway? A study of personal pronoun systems". Linguistische Berichte 98: p308, as quoted in Simon 2005. Quote: My contention is that any language which provided more than one 2nd person plural pronoun, and required the speaker to make substantial enquiries about the whereabouts and number of those referred to in addition to the one person he was actually addressing, would be quite literally unspeakable.
  8. One treated example is the Ghomala' language of Western Cameroon, which has been said to have a [1+2+3] first-person plural pronoun, but a more recent analysis by Wiesemann (2003) indicates that such pronouns may be limited to ceremonial use.
  9. http://wals.info/chapter/39 World Atlas of Language Structures 39: Inclusive/Exclusive Distinction in Independent Pronouns
  10. http://wals.info/chapter/40 World Atlas of Language Structures 40: Inclusive/Exclusive Distinction in Verbal Inflection
  11. Matthews, 2010. "Language Contact and Chinese". In Hickey, ed., The Handbook of Language Contact, p 760.
  12. 1 2 3 К проблеме категории инклюзивности местоимений в удмуртском языке

Further reading