Communication in aquatic animals

Last updated
Aggressive zebra pattern display in cuttlefish Cuttlefish zebra pattern.jpg
Aggressive zebra pattern display in cuttlefish

Communication occurs when an animal produces a signal and uses it to influences the behaviour of another animal. [1] [2] A signal can be any behavioural, structural or physiological trait that has evolved specifically to carry information about the sender and/or the external environment and to stimulate the sensory system of the receiver to change their behaviour. [1] [2] [3] A signal is different from a cue in that cues are informational traits that have not been selected for communication purposes. [3] For example, if an alerted bird gives a warning call to a predator and causes the predator to give up the hunt, the bird is using the sound as a signal to communicate its awareness to the predator. On the other hand, if a rat forages in the leaves and makes a sound that attracts a predator, the sound itself is a cue and the interaction is not considered a communication attempt.

Contents

Air and water have different physical properties which lead to different velocity and clarity of the signal transmission process during communication. [4] This means that common understanding of communication mechanisms and structures of terrestrial animals cannot be applied to aquatic animals. For example, a horse can sniff the air to detect pheromones but a fish which is surrounded by water will need a different method to detect chemicals.

Aquatic animals can communicate through various signal modalities including visual, auditory, tactile, chemical and electrical signals. Communication using any of these forms requires specialised signal producing and detecting organs. Thus, the structure, distribution and mechanism of these sensory systems vary amongst different classes and species of aquatic animals and they also differ greatly to those of terrestrial animals.

The basic functions of communication in aquatic animals are similar to those of terrestrial animals. In general, communication can be used to facilitate social recognition and aggregation, to locate, attract and evaluate mating partners and to engage in territorial or mating disputes. Different species of aquatic animals can sometimes communicate. Interspecies communication is most common between prey and predator or between animals engaged in mutualistic symbiotic relationships.

Modes of communication

Acoustic

Acoustic communication is the use of sound as signals. Acoustic communication is widespread in both aquatic and semi-aquatic invertebrates and vertebrates, [5] with many species capable of using both infrasound and ultrasound for communication. As sound travels faster and over a larger distance in water than in air, aquatic animals can use sound signals for long-distance communication while terrestrial animals cannot. [4] For example, a blue whale can communicate with another blue whale using sound over thousands of miles across the sea. [6]

While terrestrial animals often have a uniform method of producing and detecting sounds, aquatic animals have a range of mechanisms to produce and detect both vocal and non-vocal sounds. [7] In terms of sound production, fish can produce sounds such as boat-whistles, grunts and croaks using their swim bladder or pectoral fin. Amphibians like frogs and toads can vocalise using vibrating tissues in airflow. For example, frogs use vocal sacs and an air-recycling system to make sound, while pipid frogs use laryngeal muscles to produce an implosion of air and create clicking noise. [7] Aquatic mammals such as seals and otters can produce sound using the larynx. Fiddler and ghost crabs can produce non-vocal noise by striking, drumming or tapping on a substrate while they are on shore, [8] while aquatic invertebrates like cleaner shrimp often produce noises by clapping their claws. [9] The small fish Danionella cerebrum makes the loudest sound for its size of any fish, using muscles to tension a cartilage; this is released to strike the swim bladder. [10]

Aquatic animals use mechanoreceptors to detect acoustic signals. Aside from aquatic mammals which have external ears, other aquatic vertebrates have ear holes containing mechanoreceptors. [7] Aquatic invertebrates such as lobster, crabs and shrimps have external sensory hairs and internal statocysts as their sound-detecting organs. [11] [12]

Acoustic signals are used for:

Visual

S-posture exhibited by Beluga whale during agonistic contexts S posture in Beluga whale.png
S-posture exhibited by Beluga whale during agonistic contexts

Aquatic animals use visual signals such as movement, postures, colouration, patterns and size. A change in these visual traits can also be considered a signal. Coastal or oceanic species are more likely to use visual signals than species inhabiting the riverine or turbid environment, due to the poor light transmission in turbid areas or in areas with increasing depth and high habitat complexity. [15] [14]

It is suggested that some fish and cephalopods can actively produce and regulate polarised light patterns for communication purposes using light-reflective structures on their bodies. [16] [17] For example, the loliginid squid has a stripe of iridophores along their dorsolateral side, commonly known as the ‘red’ stripe, which reflects polarised light at oblique angles. The degree and pattern of polarisation on the loliginid squid can be controlled using physiological processes. [16]

Visual signals are detected in animals by photoreceptors. Some semi-aquatic mammals have adaptations for visions (larger eyes, tapetum) that allow them to see and potentially communicate using visual signals even in low light conditions. [14] In some fish, mantis shrimp and squid, their eyes have a specific photoreceptor structure/orientation that is thought to give them the ability to detect polarised light. [16] [17]

Unlike in the air, the specific light spectral bandwidth and intensity changes across water habitats. The spectral sensitivity of an animal's retinal photoreceptors appears to depend on the colour of the water they live in and can sometimes shift when they move to a different location to maximise visual acuity.

Visual signals are used for:

Chemical

Many aquatic species can communicate using chemical molecules known as pheromones. [20]

Production and secretion of pheromones are often controlled by specialised glands or organs. [21] Aquatic animals can produce both water-soluble and water-insoluble pheromones, though they mostly produce soluble signals for ease of dispersion in the water environment. [22] Water-soluble chemicals are often dispersed into the surrounding fluid, while water-insoluble chemicals are expressed at the body surface of the animal.

Hermit crab uses antennules bearing shingle-shaped aesthetascs to capture odours. Caribbean hermit crab Antennule.JPG
Hermit crab uses antennules bearing shingle-shaped aesthetascs to capture odours.

Crustaceans can release urine containing chemical signals anteriorly through a pair of nephron-pores [23] and can also deposit pheromones on their body surface which are produced by their tegmental glands. [21] Fish release pheromones through urine using their excretory pores and gills. [21] Amphibians such as frogs and toads produced water-soluble pheromones using their breeding glands. [24] Mammals such as dolphins release water-soluble pheromones in their excretions, while pinnipeds have scent glands around the vibrissae and hindquarters that are thought to produce pheromones. [14]

Chemical signals are detected using mechanoreceptors. Crustaceans have chemoreceptors on the antennules. They can sample chemical signals around them by flicking their antennas and by creating water currents that draw the chemicals in their surrounding towards them. [21] Fish have mechanoreceptors lined in their nasal cavity. It is suggested that the multi-ciliated cells around the rim of their nasal cavities generate a water flow to increase chemical detection. [25]

Most semi-aquatic amphibians, reptiles and mammals have nose and tongues. On land, sea otters and pinnipeds often perform ‘nosing’ behaviours at prominent scent glands which indicate some level of detection of chemical signals. It was previously perceived that they do not undergo chemical communication underwater, as most of these animals close their nasal opening underwater and the semi-aquatic mammals are known to have reduced olfactory nerves, bulbs and tracts. [21] However, it has been found that the semi-aquatic star-nosed mole and water shrew can detect chemicals underwater by exhaling air bubbles onto objects or scent trails and re-inhaling the bubbles which now carry the chemical signals back through the nose. [26]

Chemical signals are used for:

Electrocommunication

Electric eel Electric Eel.jpg
Electric eel

As water is a much better electrical conductor than air, electrocommunication is only observed in aquatic animals. There are various animals that can detect electrical signals, but fish are the only aquatic animals that can both send and receive EOD, making them the only animals to effectively communicate using electrical signals. Weakly electric fish can use specialised electric organs to generate a constant electrical discharge, also known as electric organ discharge (EOD). [27] Electric eels, for example, have three pairs of abdominal organs containing electrolytes that can produce electricity: the main organ, the hunter's organ and the sach's organ. The EOD can be species specific and can even sometimes be unique to each individual. [28] Electric fish can also modify the frequency, amount, duration, silent periods, amplitude and chords of their EOD. [28] The natural EOD and the conscious alterations to EOD are all social signals which have been observed to correlate with many social situations.

Electric fish can detect electrical signals using tuberous electroreceptors which are sensitive to high-frequency stimuli. Electroreceptors exist in different forms and can be found in various parts of the body. Sharks, for example, have electroreceptors called ampullae of Lorenzini in the pores on their snouts and other zones of the head. Electric eels have various patches of tuberous receptors over its body.

Electrical signals are used for:

Tactile

Tactile communication, also known as touch, is limited for very short distances as it requires physical contact. Visual displays in very short-range situations often readily become tactile signals. Tactile signals include extensive touching and rubbing during social context using the nose, rostrum, flippers, pectoral fins, dorsal fin, flukes, abdomen, or even entire body. More aggressive tactile signals include biting, raking, butting, or ramming. [14]

Animals detect touch by the somatosensory system which responses to changes at the surface or inside the body. The mechanoreceptors in the somatosensory system can be found the skin surface of most aquatic animals, as well as on the vibrissae of pinnipeds or on the hair of whales.

Tactile signals are used for:

Multimodal communication

A multimodal signal is one that incorporates various sensory modalities. For example, the male Hylopes japi frog's mating display incorporates both visual signals (foot shaking, throat display, toe flagging) and acoustic signals (peep and squeals) simultaneously. [30] The use of multimodal signalling is not only observed in aquatic animals but also in other terrestrial animals. Multimodal signalling is thought to increase the effectiveness of the signal under noisy or variable environments [31] and offer the ability to signal multiple quality at once. [32] From the basis of the game theory, constraints on cost functions, possible errors across modalities and instances of multiple qualities, signallers and audiences all provide biological benefits that favour multimodal signalling. [33]

Interspecific communication

Interspecific communication is communication between members of different species.

The fire-bellied toad has a brightly coloured belly. Fire Bellied Toad - CNP 3431 (7138473433).jpg
The fire-bellied toad has a brightly coloured belly.

Prey and predator interaction

Preys often exhibit pursuit-deterrent signals to convince the predator to not pursue and/or eat them. A pursuit-deterrent signal can indicate toxicity. For example, when the fire-bellied toad is attacked, it will adopt a defensive pose and exhibit its bright-coloured belly to the predator. The bright colour signals to the predator that the toad is toxic and therefore deter the predator from striking.

Prey may also reliably signal to a predator that they are difficult to catch or subdue, and that causes the predator to desist from attacking or switch their attack to another prey individual. For example, guppies might exhibit a visual signal of approaching and inspecting possible predator which communicates to the predator that the guppies are aware and will be harder to catch. It has been shown that cichlid (the guppies’ predator) are less likely to attack the guppies which exhibit inspecting behaviours. [34]

Predators do not often communicate with their preys, but if they do most of the signals they produced are dishonest.

Mutualistic symbiotic interaction

A mutualistic relationship occurs when two organisms of different species ‘work together’ and benefit from each other. In some cases, the communication between two organisms provides the basis for this mutual benefit.

An example of this is the mutualistic symbiotic relationship between the goby, a small bottom-dwelling fish, and an alpheid, or snapping, shrimp. The goby usually sits at the entrance of a burrow that the shrimp digs and maintains. While the shrimp works on the burrow, the goby would stand watch. If the goby sees a potential danger, it will flick or beat its tail on the shrimp's antennae. This tactile signal communicates the existence of possible danger to the shrimp and the shrimp will withdraw into the burrow with the goby following suit. This communication benefits both the goby (the shrimp will allow it to use the burrow for shelter) and the shrimp (it can safely put more energy into shelter preparation and maintenance). [35]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pheromone</span> Secreted or excreted chemical factor that triggers a social response in members of the same species

A pheromone is a secreted or excreted chemical factor that triggers a social response in members of the same species. Pheromones are chemicals capable of acting like hormones outside the body of the secreting individual, to affect the behavior of the receiving individuals. There are alarm pheromones, food trail pheromones, sex pheromones, and many others that affect behavior or physiology. Pheromones are used by many organisms, from basic unicellular prokaryotes to complex multicellular eukaryotes. Their use among insects has been particularly well documented. In addition, some vertebrates, plants and ciliates communicate by using pheromones. The ecological functions and evolution of pheromones are a major topic of research in the field of chemical ecology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

Stimulus modality, also called sensory modality, is one aspect of a stimulus or what is perceived after a stimulus. For example, the temperature modality is registered after heat or cold stimulate a receptor. Some sensory modalities include: light, sound, temperature, taste, pressure, and smell. The type and location of the sensory receptor activated by the stimulus plays the primary role in coding the sensation. All sensory modalities work together to heighten stimuli sensation when necessary.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bioacoustics</span> Study of sound relating to biology

Bioacoustics is a cross-disciplinary science that combines biology and acoustics. Usually it refers to the investigation of sound production, dispersion and reception in animals. This involves neurophysiological and anatomical basis of sound production and detection, and relation of acoustic signals to the medium they disperse through. The findings provide clues about the evolution of acoustic mechanisms, and from that, the evolution of animals that employ them.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Channel catfish</span> Species of fish

The channel catfish is North America's most numerous catfish species. It is the official fish of Kansas, Missouri, Nebraska, and Tennessee, and is informally referred to as a "channel cat". In the United States, they are the most fished catfish species with around 8 million anglers targeting them per year. They also have very few teeth and swallow food whole. The popularity of channel catfish for food has contributed to the rapid expansion of aquaculture of this species in the United States. It has also been widely introduced in Europe, Asia and South America, and it is legally considered an invasive species in many countries.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cleaning station</span> Location where aquatic life congregate to be cleaned

A cleaning station is a location where aquatic life congregate to be cleaned by smaller beings. Such stations exist in both freshwater and marine environments, and are used by animals including fish, sea turtles and hippos.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alarm signal</span> Signal made by social animals to warn others of danger

In animal communication, an alarm signal is an antipredator adaptation in the form of signals emitted by social animals in response to danger. Many primates and birds have elaborate alarm calls for warning conspecifics of approaching predators. For example, the alarm call of the blackbird is a familiar sound in many gardens. Other animals, like fish and insects, may use non-auditory signals, such as chemical messages. Visual signs such as the white tail flashes of many deer have been suggested as alarm signals; they are less likely to be received by conspecifics, so have tended to be treated as a signal to the predator instead.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Crypsis</span> Aspect of animal behaviour and morphology

In ecology, crypsis is the ability of an animal or a plant to avoid observation or detection by other animals. It may be a predation strategy or an antipredator adaptation. Methods include camouflage, nocturnality, subterranean lifestyle and mimicry. Crypsis can involve visual, olfactory or auditory concealment. When it is visual, the term cryptic coloration, effectively a synonym for animal camouflage, is sometimes used, but many different methods of camouflage are employed by animals or plants.

A kairomone is a semiochemical, emitted by an organism, which mediates interspecific interactions in a way that benefits an individual of another species which receives it and harms the emitter. This "eavesdropping" is often disadvantageous to the producer. The kairomone improves the fitness of the recipient and in this respect differs from an allomone and a synomone. The term is mostly used in the field of entomology. Two main ecological cues are provided by kairomones; they generally either indicate a food source for the receiver, or the presence of a predator, the latter of which is less common or at least less studied.

Interspecies communication is communication between different species of animals, plants, or microorganisms.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat communication</span> Feline means of sending or receiving information

Cats communicate for a variety of reasons, including to show happiness, express anger, solicit attention, and observe potential prey. Additionally, they collaborate, play, and share resources. When cats communicate with humans, they do so to get what they need or want, such as food, water, attention, or play. As such, cat communication methods have been significantly altered by domestication. Studies have shown that domestic cats tend to meow much more than feral cats. They rarely meow to communicate with fellow cats or other animals. Cats can socialize with each other and are known to form "social ladders," where a dominant cat is leading a few lesser cats. This is common in multi-cat households.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Display (zoology)</span> Set of ritualized behaviours in animals

Display behaviour is a set of ritualized behaviours that enable an animal to communicate to other animals about specific stimuli. Such ritualized behaviours can be visual, but many animals depend on a mixture of visual, audio, tactical and chemical signals. Evolution has tailored these stereotyped behaviours to allow animals to communicate both conspecifically and interspecifically which allows for a broader connection in different niches in an ecosystem. It is connected to sexual selection and survival of the species in various ways. Typically, display behaviour is used for courtship between two animals and to signal to the female that a viable male is ready to mate. In other instances, species may make territorial displays, in order to preserve a foraging or hunting territory for its family or group. A third form is exhibited by tournament species in which males will fight in order to gain the 'right' to breed. Animals from a broad range of evolutionary hierarchies avail of display behaviours - from invertebrates such as the simple jumping spider to the more complex vertebrates like the harbour seal.

Sensory ecology is a relatively new field focusing on the information organisms obtain about their environment. It includes questions of what information is obtained, how it is obtained, and why the information is useful to the organism.

Whispering is an unvoiced mode of phonation in which the vocal cords are abducted so that they do not vibrate; air passes between the arytenoid cartilages to create audible turbulence during speech. Supralaryngeal articulation remains the same as in normal speech.

A sense is a biological system used by an organism for sensation, the process of gathering information about the surroundings through the detection of stimuli. Although, in some cultures, five human senses were traditionally identified as such, many more are now recognized. Senses used by non-human organisms are even greater in variety and number. During sensation, sense organs collect various stimuli for transduction, meaning transformation into a form that can be understood by the brain. Sensation and perception are fundamental to nearly every aspect of an organism's cognition, behavior and thought.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Seismic communication</span>

Seismic or vibrational communication is a process of conveying information through mechanical (seismic) vibrations of the substrate. The substrate may be the earth, a plant stem or leaf, the surface of a body of water, a spider's web, a honeycomb, or any of the myriad types of soil substrates. Seismic cues are generally conveyed by surface Rayleigh or bending waves generated through vibrations on the substrate, or acoustical waves that couple with the substrate. Vibrational communication is an ancient sensory modality and it is widespread in the animal kingdom where it has evolved several times independently. It has been reported in mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians, insects, arachnids, crustaceans and nematode worms. Vibrations and other communication channels are not necessarily mutually exclusive, but can be used in multi-modal communication.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hydrodynamic reception</span> Ability of an organism to sense water movements

In animal physiology, hydrodynamic reception refers to the ability of some animals to sense water movements generated by biotic or abiotic sources. This form of mechanoreception is useful for orientation, hunting, predator avoidance, and schooling. Frequent encounters with conditions of low visibility can prevent vision from being a reliable information source for navigation and sensing objects or organisms in the environment. Sensing water movements is one resolution to this problem.

Most fish possess highly developed sense organs. Nearly all daylight fish have colour vision that is at least as good as a human's. Many fish also have chemoreceptors that are responsible for extraordinary senses of taste and smell. Although they have ears, many fish may not hear very well. Most fish have sensitive receptors that form the lateral line system, which detects gentle currents and vibrations, and senses the motion of nearby fish and prey. Sharks can sense frequencies in the range of 25 to 50 Hz through their lateral line.

Lizards are among the most diverse groups of reptiles with more than 5,600 species. With such diversity in physical and behavioral characteristics, lizards have evolved many different ways to communicate. Lizards communicate to gain information about the individuals around them by paying attention to various characteristics exhibited by individuals and using various physical and behavioral traits to communicate. These traits differ based on the mode of communication being used.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Elephant communication</span> Communication between elephants

Elephants communicate with each other in various ways, including touching, visual displays, vocalisations, seismic vibrations, and semiochemicals.

References

  1. 1 2 Ruxton, G.D.; Schaefer, H.M. (December 2011). "Resolving current disagreements and ambiguities in the terminology of animal communication". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 24 (12): 2574–85. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02386.x. PMID   21902751.
  2. 1 2 Scarantino, A. (2013). "Animal communication as information-mediated influence". In Steggman, U.E. (ed.). Animal Communication Theory: Information and Influence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 63–88. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139003551.005. ISBN   9781139003551.
  3. 1 2 Smith, J.M.; Harper, D. (2003). Animal Signals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   9780198526858.
  4. 1 2 3 Hopkins, C.D. (1988). "Social Communication in the Aquatic Environment". Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals. New York, NY: Springer. pp. 233–268. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-3714-3_9. ISBN   978-1-4612-8317-1.
  5. Owren, M.J.; Rendall, D.; Ryan, M.J. (2010). "Redefining animal signalling: influence versus information in communication". Biology & Philosophy. 25 (5): 755–780. doi:10.1007/s10539-010-9224-4. S2CID   13443399.
  6. "Blue Whales and communication". National Geographic. March 26, 2011.
  7. 1 2 3 Ladich, F.; Winkler, H. (2017). "Acoustic communication in terrestrial and aquatic vertebrates". Journal of Experimental Biology. 220 (13): 2306–2317. doi: 10.1242/jeb.132944 . PMID   28679789.
  8. Salmon, M. (1983). "Acoustic "calling" by fiddler and ghost crabs" (PDF). In Lowery, J.K. (ed.). Papers from the Conference on the Biology and Evolution of Crustacea. Australian Museum Memoir. Vol. 18. Sydney, New South Wales: Australia Museum. pp. 63–76.
  9. Chapuis, L.; Bshary, R. (2010). "Signalling by the cleaner shrimp Periclimenes longicarpus". Animal Behaviour. 79 (3): 645–647. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.012. S2CID   604480.
  10. McGrath, Matt (27 February 2024). "Gills Aloud? Tiny fish found making very big noise". BBC News. Retrieved 27 February 2024.
  11. "Marine invertebrate communication". The University of Rhode Island. 2019. Retrieved 6 May 2019.
  12. Breithaupt, T.; Tautz, J. (1990). "The Sensitivity of Crayfish Mechanoreceptors to Hydrodynamic and Acoustic stimuli". In Wiese, K.; Krenz, WD.; Tautz, J.; Reichert, H.; Mulloney, B. (eds.). Frontiers in Crustacean Neurobiology. Birkhäuser, Basel: Advances in Life Sciences. pp. 114–120. doi:10.1007/978-3-0348-5689-8_12. ISBN   978-3-0348-5691-1.
  13. van Oosterom, L.; Montgomery, J.C.; Jeffs, A.G.; Radford, C.A. (2016). "Evidence for contact calls in fish: conspecific vocalisations and ambient soundscape influence group cohesion in a nocturnal species". Scientific Reports. 6: 19098. Bibcode:2016NatSR...619098V. doi: 10.1038/srep19098 . PMC   4707522 . PMID   26750559.
  14. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Dudzinski, K.; Thomas, J.A.; Gregg, J. (2009). "Communication in Marine Mammals". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thiesselwait, J.G.M. (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammal . Academic Press. pp.  260–269. ISBN   9780123735539.
  15. Bronmark, C.; Hasson, L. (2003). "Chemical communication in aquatic systems: an introduction". Oikos. 88 (1): 103–109. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2000.880112.x.
  16. 1 2 3 4 Mathger, L.M.; Shashar, N.; Hanlon, R.T. (2009). "Do cephalopods communicate using polarized light reflections from their skin?". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (14): 2133–2140. doi:10.1242/jeb.020800. PMID   19561202.
  17. 1 2 Shashar, N.; Rutledge, P.; Cronin, T. (1996). "Polarization vision in cuttlefish in a concealed communication channel?" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 199 (Pt 9): 2077–2084. doi:10.1242/jeb.199.9.2077. PMID   9319987.
  18. Pryor, K.W. (1990). "Non-Acoustic Communication in Small Cetaceans: Glance, Touch, Position, Gesture, and Bubbles". In Thomas, J.A.; Kastelein, R.A (eds.). Sensory Abilities of Cetaceans. Boston: Springer. pp. 537–544. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-0858-2_37. ISBN   978-1-4899-0860-5.
  19. Popper, A.N.; Salmon, M.; Horch, K.W. (2001). "Acoustic detection and communication by decapod crustaceans". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 187 (2): 83–89. doi:10.1007/s003590100184. ISSN   0340-7594. PMID   15523997. S2CID   29630146.
  20. Wyatt, T.D. (2009). "Pheromones and other Chemical Communication in animals". In Squire, L.R. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. pp. 611–616. ISBN   978-0-08-045046-9.
  21. 1 2 3 4 5 Breithaupt, T.; Hardege, J.D. (2012). "Pheromones mediating sex and dominance in aquatic animals". In Bronmark, C.; Hasson, L. (eds.). Chemical Ecology in Aquatic Systems. Oxford University Press. ISBN   9780199583102.
  22. Wisenden, B.D. (2000). "Olfactory assessment of predation risk in the aquatic environment". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 355 (1401): 1205–1208. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0668. PMC   1692838 . PMID   11079399.
  23. 1 2 Breithaupt, T.; Thiel, M., eds. (2011). Chemical communication in Crustaceans. Springer Science + Business Media. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-77101-4. ISBN   978-0-387-77100-7.
  24. 1 2 Belanger, R.M.; Corkum, L.D. (2009). "Review of Aquatic Sex Pheromones and Chemical Communication in Anurans". Journal of Herpetology. 42 (2): 184–191. doi:10.1670/08-054R1.1. S2CID   59470544.
  25. Neuhauss, S.C.F. (2017). "Olfaction: How Fish Catch a Whiff". Current Biology. 27 (2): 166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.007 . PMID   28118587.
  26. Catania, K.C. (2006). "Olfaction: underwater 'sniffing' by semi-aquatic mammals". Nature. 444 (7122): 1024–1025. Bibcode:2006Natur.444.1024C. doi:10.1038/4441024a. PMID   17183311. S2CID   4417227.
  27. Hopkins, C.D. (1999). "Design features for electric communication" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (Pt 10): 1217–1228. doi:10.1242/jeb.202.10.1217. ISSN   0022-0949. PMID   10210663.
  28. 1 2 Bullock, T.H. (1973). "Seeing the World through a New Sense: Electroreception in Fish: Sharks, catfish, and electric fish use low- or high-frequency electroreceptors, actively and passively, in object detection and social communication". American Scientist. 61 (3): 316–325. JSTOR   27843789.
  29. Black-Cleworth, P. (1970). "The Role of Electrical Discharges in the Non-Reproductive Social Behaviour of Gymnotus carapo (Gymnotidae, Pisces)". Animal Behaviour Monographs. 3 (1): 1–77. doi:10.1016/S0066-1856(70)80001-2.
  30. 1 2 Sa, F.P.; Zina, J.; Haddad, C.F.B. (2016). "Sophisticated Communication in the Brazilian Torrent Frog Hylodes japi". PLOS ONE. 11 (1): e0145444. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1145444D. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145444 . PMC   4719245 . PMID   26760304.
  31. Stevens, M. (2013). "Multimodal Signals and Communication". Sensory Ecology, Behaviour, and Evolution. Oxford Scholarship Online. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199601776.001.0001. ISBN   9780199601776.
  32. Highham, J.P.; Hebets, E.A. (2013). "An introduction to multimodal communication". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 67 (9): 1381–1388. doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1590-x. ISSN   0340-5443. S2CID   253810935.
  33. Wilson, A.J.; Dean, M.; Highham, J. (2013). "A game theoretic approach to multimodal communication". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 67 (9): 1399–1415. doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1589-3. ISSN   0340-5443. S2CID   253818346.
  34. Goldin, J.J.; Davis, S.A. (1995). "Who Dares, Benefits: Predator Approach Behaviour in the Guppy (Poecilia reticulata) Deters Predator Pursuit". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 259 (1355): 193–200. Bibcode:1995RSPSB.259..193G. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0028. S2CID   84573281.
  35. Preston, J.L. (1978). "Communication systems and social interactions in a goby-shrimp symbiosis". Animal Behaviour. 26: 791–802. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90144-6. S2CID   53163667.