Anecdotal cognitivism

Last updated

Anecdotal cognitivism is a method of research using anecdotal, and anthropomorphic evidence through the observation of animal behaviour. [1]

Contents

Expression of the Emotions Figure 6 Expression of the Emotions Figure 6.png
Expression of the Emotions Figure 6

A psychological methodology that attributes mental states to animals on the basis of anecdotes and on the observation of particular cases, other than those observations made during controlled experiments. The purpose is to understand by what means animals interpret external stimuli from the world around them, and subsequently how and why they act on that information. [2] [3] Charles Darwin devised this method in the late nineteenth century, naming it anecdotal cognitivism. This method proved controversial within the academy for the first half of the twentieth century, as Behaviourist methods were favoured at this time. Behaviourists maintain that controlled experiments are necessary to measure stimuli and record observable behaviour. From the middle of the twentieth century ethology and later, cognitive ethology became increasingly important within the scientific and academic milieu.

After the introduction of natural history documentary film production in the 1960s animal behaviour became popular in the general population. [4] Presenters, such as David Attenborough on BBC England and George Page on PBS America, used anecdotes and anthropomorphic rhetoric thus providing access to a wider audience, increasing awareness and interest in animal behaviour and their relationship with humans and nature.

History

This illustration of feline behaviour is an example of an expression of fear and aggression in "The expression of the emotions in man and animals" by Charles Darwin. Cats; aggression, animal emotions and facial expression; ethology, physiology, biology.jpg
This illustration of feline behaviour is an example of an expression of fear and aggression in "The expression of the emotions in man and animals" by Charles Darwin.

Contemporary scientific interest in animal minds, cognition and behaviour stem from Charles Darwin's nineteenth century seminal text, Theory of Evolution . [5] Rather than showing the reader a series of tables, statistics and diagrams Darwin informed the reader with examples of animal behaviour he collected from both his observations, and the observations of others from all over the world. Anecdotal cognitivism was also practiced in his 1872 publication, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, with theories relating to the universality and evolution of the facial expressions in animals. [6] Darwin's interest in the mental states of animals instigated the anecdotal cognitivism method which involved collecting stories about animal behaviour from both lay people and amateur naturalists. These stories provided evidence of the cognitive states in particular animal cases. [7] Evidence collected was often from a single witness whose account of the incident may have been second or third hand and was often not from a scientifically trained person. [8]

In the late nineteenth and early twentieth century, anecdotal cognitivism's method was widely criticised in the broader Western scientific community which had come to favour behaviourism. Behaviourism is a cause and effect quantitative data approach and a method scientist argued had greater rigour. [9] Post 1930, anecdotal cognitivism was abandoned for the better part of the twentieth century due to the dominance of the behaviourist approach. [10]

Methods

Many academic disciplines contribute to the study of animal cognition and behaviour. Some example include: animal studies, anthropology, biology, psychology, and philosophy. Within each discipline a range of methodologies are implemented to record research. Two central methods are laboratory experiments to uncover systems and structures of behaviour as used by psychologists, and observational methods are more commonly used by ethologists and biologists to explain evolutionary behaviour. [11]

There are three philosophical issues in studying minds and cognition in animals. Methodological issues, or how to study animal minds, is the philosophical issue relating to anecdotal cognitivism. The other two are foundational issues, whether animals are worthwhile subjects of study and specific issues relating to the different discipline's approaches to methodologies in academia. [12]

Criticism of anecdotal cognitivism

Clever Hans CleverHans.jpg
Clever Hans

Anecdotal cognitivism is often criticised by behaviourists for relying on specific cases as evidence of particular animal behaviour, such as that of Clever Hans. [13] Clever Hans was a particularly clever horse, able to interpret his masters body language while carrying out simple arithmetic and answering various simple questions. These involuntary movements made by the owner Mr von Osten, were unconscious and led to the horse being able to correctly answer questions based on the very subtle angle changes of Mr von Osten's head. This case study was often cited by scientists as reasoning against the validity of using this methodology because this singular case was proved to be inaccurate. This exemplified the importance of eradicating unintentional cueing while researching animals and was the argument behaviourists cited for controlled procedures. [14]

Historical Western theorists

Supportive of anecdotal cognitivism

George Romanes: (1848-1894)

A protégé of Charles Darwin, George Romanes continued using anecdotal methodologies. Romanes documented an account that exemplifies this methodology, the example; a hunter who had shot and wounded a monkey, this monkey then held his blood-stained hand out to the hunter "to make him feel guilty". As Romanes did not further investigate the anecdotes with test experiments to consider an alternative hypothesis, this method became contentious among his fellow scientists. [15] According to Professor Frans de Waal from Emory University, Romanes reliance on these singular events and Darwin's internal subjective thinking to explain his own research experiences is what led subsequent scientists to favour the behaviourist approach. [16] Behaviourists were also concerned with the lack of statistical information collected with a particular interest in certain behaviours and a perceived bias from subjects whilst relaying their stories. [17] [18]

Lloyd Morgan: (1852-1936)

Lloyd Morgan, a British psychologist and student of Romanes, removed the anecdotal methodologies from his work to avoid criticism and to ensure an objective systematic speculation and research approach. [19] He was in favour of associating the mental states of animals by interpreting animal behaviour through introspection, though he was cautious of how humans over-intellectualise animal behaviour, as in the case of Clever Hans. [20] In 1894, Morgan developed the methodology that most psychologists now follow, known as Morgan's Canon. [21]

Criticism of anecdotal cognitivism

The behaviourists' critique of the anecdotal, anthropomorphic methods as subjective, rejected the possibility of internal states in animals, and were more interested in human control over animals than natural behaviour. [22]

John B. Watson: (1878-1958)

Considered the founder of behaviourism, American psychologist John B. Watson studied animal psychology and was critical of introspective psychology and anecdotal cognitivism. He thought it inferior to objective, observable experiments and did not accept the experimental observer's behaviour could be influenced by the subjective experience. [23] In 1909, at Johns Hopkins School of Medicine's Southern Society for Philosophy and Psychology, Watson stated that "these trends away from introspection will lead psychology toward the perfection of technique of the physical sciences." [24] Later, in 1913 he declared, "never use the term consciousness, mental state, mind, content, introspectively verifiable, imagery and the like…it can be done in terms of stimulus and response, in terms of habit formation, habit integrations and the like." [25]

Jacques Loeb: (1859-1924)

Loeb, a mechanist physiologist, believed animal behaviour was of no particular purpose or design and could be explained in terms of environmental stimulus. [26] He was known for attentive scientific research and as a founder to behaviourism. [27] Thomas H. Leahey, Professor Emeritus of Virginia Commonwealth University noted that European physiologists of the early twentieth century including Loeb, "pronounced psychological concepts "superstitions" and found no room for animal consciousness in the explanation of animal behaviour." [28]

E. L. Thorndikes: (1874-1949)

Puzzle box Puzzle box.jpg
Puzzle box

Both Edward Thorndike's puzzle boxes and PhD thesis, penned in 1898 at Columbia University, were modelled off previous work by Romanes and Morgan. Thorndike expected to assert similar connections to animal learning through imitation and passive tuition as his predecessors. [29] As his extensive laboratory research and observations of cats and dog's ability to escape the boxes came to a close, he found that the research conclusions greatly differed to what he expected (that animals could learn from imitation). This was the beginning of his research into "motor impulse." [30] Thorndike was highly critical of the 'anecdotalist school' and their lack of measurable and scientific methodologies. [31] Thorndike later went on to study The Law of Effect and S-R bonds (Stimulus-Response) and contribute to educational psychology. [32]

Burrhus Frederick Skinner: (1904-1990)

After WWII, B. F. Skinner's radical behaviourism gained popularity, he "applied [Auguste] Comte's vision to psychology" maintaining that science's goal was to ultimately control society with little need for introspection and maintained that mental processes were unobservable and illegitimate. [33] His theory borrowed some of Darwin's evolutionary theory, though he believed human behaviour was explained through environment. Skinner was focused on a methodology of observation alone. According to Thomas H. Leahey, Professor Emeritus, Virginia Commonwealth University "Skinner believed that truth is to be found in observations themselves, in "does" and "doesn't" rather than in our interpretations of our observations." [34]

Non-Western approaches

Japan: Studies of Ethology in Japan have regularly used anthropomorphism and anecdotal methods, Japanese culture did not follow the rationalist American behaviourist approach. [35] The cultural differences between the two countries underpin the different methods used to investigate animal behaviour in the academic disciplines. [36] A reason scholars cite for this difference is Japan's spiritual foundation of animism in Shintoism and Buddhism, where animate and inanimate objects have spirits. [37]

Current discussions

Within Classic Ethology, Darwin's anecdotal and anthropomorphic approach was modified by European researchers. Classical Ethologists, Konrad Lorenz (1903-1989) and Niko Tinbergen (1907-1988), undertook a return to Darwin's evolutionary theory and natural history with a reliance on more meticulous anecdotal and anthropomorphic methodologies. [38] The research carried out by both scientists focused on observations, free use of anecdotes and an emphasis on emotion and inner states, "instincts", "drives", "motivational impulses" and "outward flowing nervous energy". [39] In 1973 they were awarded the Nobel Prize with Karl von Frisch (1886-1982) for animal behaviour and were the founders of the field of ethology. [40]

Animal behaviour, curiosity and play between species. Cat playing with a lizard.jpg
Animal behaviour, curiosity and play between species.

Though there are many differences between the schools of behaviourism and ethology, they agreed upon scepticism of the "folk" interpretations of over emphasising anecdotes to explain animal intelligence. [41]

There has been a resurgence in the adaption and use of this method in the twenty-first century. In current research this methodology has evolved and is no longer called 'anecdotal cognitivism,' with the scientific vernacular having changed to 'cognitive ethology' a term coined by Don Griffin, which involves anecdotal and anthropomorphic observations with reference to the cognition, internal states and behaviour analysis of animals. [42] [43] Ethograms, a quantitative method consisting of an inventory table of descriptions, are used to document typical animal behaviour. In unusual or rare animal behaviour, an "incident" or "qualitative report" is written rather than referencing the term "anecdote", due to the negative connotation that resulted from Darwin's method. [44]

Current theorists' support

Female Toque macaque with baby - (Harmony of Life) Female Toque macaque with baby - (Harmony of Life).jpg
Female Toque macaque with baby - (Harmony of Life)

There has been a significant shift from the behaviourist methodologies, as Professor Frans de Waal states,

"We seek ecological validity in our studies and follow the advice of Uexkull, Lorenz, and Imanishi, who encouraged human empathy as a way to understand other species. True empathy is not self-focused but other-oriented." [45] De Waal also states anecdotes are appropriate to use in contemporary settings as long as they are videoed, or observed by a reputable observer aware and familiar with the animals in question. [46]

This is further evidenced in Sara Shettleworth's work. A professor of ecology and evolutionary biology at the University of Toronto, Shettleworth maintains the most exciting part of current study in animal cognition is detail and subtlety in the human picture of "how other species minds are both like and not like ours." [47]

Emeritus Professor in Animal Behaviour at University of New England, Gisela Kaplan noted the consolatory behaviour of Australian Magpies post conflict exhibited behaviour similar to the image seen in Darwin's crouching dog (figure 6 above), in The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Using anecdotes, Kaplan said that over several occasions it was noted that the "magpie standing with hunched up wings in a little spatial isolation from the others (presumably the victim)" would be met by a member of the flock that went over and extended its neck over the victim or touched the isolated bird. The isolated bird would "untangle" and after a minute was led back to the group. [48]

There are advantages in the use of historical anecdotal cognitivism from a pedagogical standpoint. The disciplinary objective of anecdotes, as it pertains to the teaching of science, provides the purpose of conveying information and explaining concepts. There are also cultural factors where the anecdotes provide a humanistic perspective to science, and of how science is evaluated and created by the scientists in a social science perspective. [49]

There have been many studies into Darwin's language and writing style. The style Darwin wrote in was seen as accessible prose with regular use of anecdotes and metaphors. This writing style made his texts more widely accessible and was instrumental in them becoming best sellers and popular within the broader community. As stated by Phillip Prodger, Darwin's theories were rapidly accepted by the public due to his writing style, "He wrote in clear, lucid language flavoured with pleasing anecdotes and clear examples." [50]

Current theorists' criticism

Researchers such as Nathan Emery and Nicola Clayton disagree, stating that any use of anecdotes, anthropomorphism and anthropocentrism are inappropriate and must be avoided in the study of animal cognition, citing the Clever Hans experiment and the walnut dropping crow study by Cristol et la. [51] Emery and Clayton state, "Given how easy it is for us, as humans, to place a human perspective on a problem, it may be an especially important caution when studying the mental lives of animals." [52]

Elephant Study 2 (6987533977) Elephant Study 2 (6987533977).jpg
Elephant Study 2 (6987533977)

Documentary film production, a source of education and entertainment popularised by natural history subjects including animal behaviour, became prominent in the later decades of the twentieth century. Natural history films and series, such as David Attenborough's Life on Earth in the 1970s, BBC's Planet Earth II in 2016 and the PBS Nature series by George Page, have contributed to people's awareness of animal behaviour, creating connection to nature and stimulating engagement in the ecological impact of humans on the planet at a time when more people are living in cities, disconnected from nature. [53]

George Page writes of anecdotal cases in Inside the Animal Mind, of primatologist Jane Goodall who cites evidence of emotions and mental states of chimpanzees, who adopt young chimps that are not their own. Also cited are stories of elephant behaviour from ethologist, elephant researcher and founder of Amboseli Elephant Research Project, Cynthia Moss. Moss notes the playful elephant behaviour in a matriarchal environment and contrasts this with the terror observed of elephants witnessing other elephants during a culling. While presenting for Nature, Page heard many stories about this behaviour from scientists, which led Page to believe that science and lay people benefit from greater understanding when anecdotes and anthropomorphic interpretations are allowed to be documented. [54]

Interesting facts

Jane Goodall, primatologist, environmentalist, prominent researcher in chimpanzee behaviour while at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Jane Goodall is holding her toy monkey "Mr. H", a regular companion during her travels. Jane Goodall HK.jpg
Jane Goodall, primatologist, environmentalist, prominent researcher in chimpanzee behaviour while at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Jane Goodall is holding her toy monkey "Mr. H", a regular companion during her travels.

Charles Darwin: Darwin had great affection for dogs, owning many in his lifetime. Shelah, Spark and Czar in his teen years, Sappho, Fun, Dash, Pincher and Nina in his early adulthood and when he had his own children Bob, Bran, Quiz, Tartar, Tony, Pepper, Butterton and Polly, whom he regularly incorporated into his studies. In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Darwin used anecdotes to explain his theories. Darwin described the 'hothouse face,' an expression Bob, Darwin's pie-bald family dog used expressing dejection when a dog becomes disappointed, expecting a walk that does not materialise. [55] Darwin's later years were spent with Polly, a white terrier, his favourite dog and the last dog he owned. Darwin mentioned Polly in The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals as licking his hand with "insatiable passion." [56] Polly was put down by Francis, Darwin's son days after Darwin's fatal heart attack, as Emma Darwin noted "Polly became very ill with a swelling in her throat...creeping away several times as if to die" and was buried in the family garden. [57] [58]

David Attenborough: Attenborough believes the most significant publication on nature and animal behaviour is Charles Darwin's, On the Origin of Species explaining the evolution and natural selection theory foundational to societies' understanding of nature. [59]

Jane Goodall: Goodall, a primatologist, was an unorthodox researcher. Her work led to new discoveries of behaviour in chimpanzee's such as tool use and the primate carnivorous diet. Professor Robert Hinde, research Professor at the University of Cambridge was initially critical of the unconventional methods Goodall employed. [60] Goodall was criticised for naming the chimps she observed at Gombe in Tanzania, she called one David Greybeard and showed photo's of him using tools to access termites at the Zoological Society of London conference in 1962. Goodall was not the only primatologist to name chimps while in the field, biologist Professor George Schaller also named the subjects he studied. [61] Sir Solly Zuckerman, an anatomist at the Zoological Society of London April 1962, criticised Goodall for using anecdotes,

"There are those who are here and who prefer anecdote – and what I must confess I regard as sometimes unbounded speculation, in scientific work it is far safer to base one's major conclusions and generalisations on a concordant and large body of data than on a few contradictory and isolated observations, the explanation of which sometimes leaves a little to be desired." [62]

By observing the chimpanzee troop in their native habitat in the 1960s, Goodall came to the conclusion counter to the scientific community at this time that chimpanzees had distinct personalities and were capable of friendship and altruism. Some behaviours and emotions observed included nervousness, aggression, hugging, kissing, grudges, kicking, punching, subordination to a dominant character and friendship. [63]

Jane Goodall writes about her studies of the Chimpanzee's in Gombe in her book, Reason for Hope, in anthropomorphic terms enabling the reader a more personal understanding of her observations encountered in Tanzania. [64]

See also

Related Research Articles

Cognitivism may refer to:

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ethology</span> Scientific objective study of animal behaviour

Ethology is the scientific study of non-human animal behavior, usually with a focus on behaviour under natural conditions, and viewing behaviour as an evolutionarily adaptive trait. Behaviourism as a term also describes the scientific and objective study of animal behavior, usually referring to measured responses to stimuli or to trained behavioral responses in a laboratory context, without a particular emphasis on evolutionary adaptivity. Throughout history, different naturalists have studied aspects of animal behaviour. Ethology has its scientific roots in the work of Charles Darwin and of American and German ornithologists of the late 19th and early 20th century, including Charles O. Whitman, Oskar Heinroth, and Wallace Craig. The modern discipline of ethology is generally considered to have begun during the 1930s with the work of Dutch biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen and Austrian biologists Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch, the three recipients of the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Ethology combines laboratory and field science, with a strong relation to some other disciplines such as neuroanatomy, ecology, and evolutionary biology. Ethologists typically show interest in a behavioral process rather than in a particular animal group, and often study one type of behavior, such as aggression, in a number of unrelated species.

Philosophy of psychology is concerned with the philosophical foundations of the study of psychology. It deals with both epistemological and ontological issues and shares interests with other fields, including philosophy of mind and theoretical psychology. Philosophical and theoretical psychology are intimately tied and are therefore sometimes used interchangeably or used together. However, philosophy of psychology relies more on debates general to philosophy and on philosophical methods, whereas theoretical psychology draws on multiple areas.

Wolfgang Köhler was a German psychologist and phenomenologist who, like Max Wertheimer and Kurt Koffka, contributed to the creation of Gestalt psychology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitivism (psychology)</span> Theoretical framework for understanding the mind

In psychology, cognitivism is a theoretical framework for understanding the mind that gained credence in the 1950s. The movement was a response to behaviorism, which cognitivists said neglected to explain cognition. Cognitive psychology derived its name from the Latin cognoscere, referring to knowing and information, thus cognitive psychology is an information-processing psychology derived in part from earlier traditions of the investigation of thought and problem solving.

Morgan's Canon, also known as Lloyd Morgan's Canon, Morgan's Canon of Interpretation or the principle or law of parsimony, is a fundamental precept of comparative (animal) psychology, coined by 19th-century British psychologist C. Lloyd Morgan. In its developed form it states that:

In no case is an animal activity to be interpreted in terms of higher psychological processes if it can be fairly interpreted in terms of processes which stand lower in the scale of psychological evolution and development.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">C. Lloyd Morgan</span>

Conwy Lloyd Morgan, FRS was a British ethologist and psychologist. He is remembered for his theory of emergent evolution, and for the experimental approach to animal psychology now known as Morgan's Canon, a principle that played a major role in behaviourism, insisting that higher mental faculties should only be considered as explanations if lower faculties could not explain a behaviour.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Frans de Waal</span> Dutch primatologist and ethologist

Franciscus Bernardus Maria "Frans" de Waal is a Dutch primatologist and ethologist. He is the Charles Howard Candler Professor of Primate Behavior in the Department of Psychology at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, director of the Living Links Center at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center at Emory, and author of numerous books including Chimpanzee Politics (1982) and Our Inner Ape (2005). His research centers on primate social behavior, including conflict resolution, cooperation, inequity aversion, and food-sharing. He is a member of the United States National Academy of Sciences and the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Comparative psychology</span> Discipline of psychology dedicated to the study of non-human animal behavior

Comparative psychology refers to the scientific study of the behavior and mental processes of non-human animals, especially as these relate to the phylogenetic history, adaptive significance, and development of behavior. Research in this area addresses many different issues, uses many different methods and explores the behavior of many different species from insects to primates.

The cognitive revolution was an intellectual movement that began in the 1950s as an interdisciplinary study of the mind and its processes. It later became known collectively as cognitive science. The relevant areas of interchange were between the fields of psychology, linguistics, computer science, anthropology, neuroscience, and philosophy. The approaches used were developed within the then-nascent fields of artificial intelligence, computer science, and neuroscience. In the 1960s, the Harvard Center for Cognitive Studies and the Center for Human Information Processing at the University of California, San Diego were influential in developing the academic study of cognitive science. By the early 1970s, the cognitive movement had surpassed behaviorism as a psychological paradigm. Furthermore, by the early 1980s the cognitive approach had become the dominant line of research inquiry across most branches in the field of psychology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Psychological adaptation</span>

A psychological adaptation is a functional, cognitive or behavioral trait that benefits an organism in its environment. Psychological adaptations fall under the scope of evolved psychological mechanisms (EPMs), however, EPMs refer to a less restricted set. Psychological adaptations include only the functional traits that increase the fitness of an organism, while EPMs refer to any psychological mechanism that developed through the processes of evolution. These additional EPMs are the by-product traits of a species’ evolutionary development, as well as the vestigial traits that no longer benefit the species’ fitness. It can be difficult to tell whether a trait is vestigial or not, so some literature is more lenient and refers to vestigial traits as adaptations, even though they may no longer have adaptive functionality. For example, xenophobic attitudes and behaviors, some have claimed, appear to have certain EPM influences relating to disease aversion, however, in many environments these behaviors will have a detrimental effect on a person's fitness. The principles of psychological adaptation rely on Darwin's theory of evolution and are important to the fields of evolutionary psychology, biology, and cognitive science.

Cognitive ethology is a branch of ethology concerned with the influence of conscious awareness and intention on the behaviour of an animal. Donald Griffin, a zoology professor in the United States, set up the foundations for researches in the cognitive awareness of animals within their habitats.

In psychology, mentalism refers to those branches of study that concentrate on perception and thought processes, for example: mental imagery, consciousness and cognition, as in cognitive psychology. The term mentalism has been used primarily by behaviorists who believe that scientific psychology should focus on the structure of causal relationships to reflexes and operant responses or on the functions of behavior.

A dog behaviourist is a person who works in modifying or changing behaviour in dogs. They can be experienced dog handlers, who have developed their experience over many years of hands-on experience, or have formal training up to degree level. Some have backgrounds in veterinary science, animal science, zoology, sociology, biology, or animal behaviour, and have applied their experience and knowledge to the interaction between humans and dogs. Professional certification may be offered through either industry associations or local educational institutions. There is however no compulsion for behaviourists to be a member of a professional body nor to take formal training.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Emotion in animals</span> Research into similarities between animal and human emotions

Emotion is defined as any mental experience with high intensity and high hedonic content. The existence and nature of emotions in non-human animals are believed to be correlated with those of humans and to have evolved from the same mechanisms. Charles Darwin was one of the first scientists to write about the subject, and his observational approach has since developed into a more robust, hypothesis-driven, scientific approach. Cognitive bias tests and learned helplessness models have shown feelings of optimism and pessimism in a wide range of species, including rats, dogs, cats, rhesus macaques, sheep, chicks, starlings, pigs, and honeybees. Jaak Panksepp played a large role in the study of animal emotion, basing his research on the neurological aspect. Mentioning seven core emotional feelings reflected through a variety of neuro-dynamic limbic emotional action systems, including seeking, fear, rage, lust, care, panic and play. Through brain stimulation and pharmacological challenges, such emotional responses can be effectively monitored.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal consciousness</span> Quality or state of self-awareness within an animal

Animal consciousness, or animal awareness, is the quality or state of self-awareness within a non-human animal, or of being aware of an external object or something within itself. In humans, consciousness has been defined as: sentience, awareness, subjectivity, qualia, the ability to experience or to feel, wakefulness, having a sense of selfhood, and the executive control system of the mind. Despite the difficulty in definition, many philosophers believe there is a broadly shared underlying intuition about what consciousness is.

Functional psychology or functionalism refers to a psychological school of thought that was a direct outgrowth of Darwinian thinking which focuses attention on the utility and purpose of behavior that has been modified over years of human existence. Edward L. Thorndike, best known for his experiments with trial-and-error learning, came to be known as the leader of the loosely defined movement. This movement arose in the U.S. in the late 19th century in direct contrast to Edward Titchener's structuralism, which focused on the contents of consciousness rather than the motives and ideals of human behavior. Functionalism denies the principle of introspection, which tends to investigate the inner workings of human thinking rather than understanding the biological processes of the human consciousness.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Marc Bekoff</span> American biologist

Marc Bekoff is an American biologist, ethologist, behavioural ecologist and writer. He was a professor of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at the University of Colorado Boulder for 32 years. He cofounded the Jane Goodall Institute of Ethologists for the Ethical Treatment of Animals, and he is Professor Emeritus of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at the University of Colorado Boulder.

Critical anthropomorphism refers to a perspective in the study of animal behavior that encompasses using the sentience of the observer to generate hypotheses in light of scientific knowledge of the species, its perceptual world, along with ecological and evolutionary history. The term is of particular relevance to mentalistic behavioral mechanisms, and its application involves using "natural history, our perceptions, intuitions, feelings, careful behavioral descriptions, identifying with the animal, optimization models, previous studies and so forth in order to generate ideas that may prove useful in gaining understanding and the ability to predict outcomes of planned (experimental) and unplanned interventions".

Gisela Kaplan is an Australian ethologist who primarily specialises in ornithology and primatology. She is a professor emeritus in animal behaviour at the University of New England, Australia, and also honorary professor of the Queensland Brain Institute.

References

Bibliography

Citations

  1. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Cambridge: The MIT Press. pp.  66. ISBN   9780262522083.
  2. Andrews, Kristin (6 May 2016). "Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  3. Shettleworth, Sara J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 4. ISBN   9780195319842.
  4. Chapman, James (2015). A New History of British Documentary. Basingstoke,UK: Palgrave macmillan. p. 177. ISBN   9781349352098.
  5. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  6. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of Psychology: From Antiquity to Modernity. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 312. ISBN   9780132929509.
  7. Andrews, Kristin (6 May 2016). "Introduction in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  8. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "Introduction in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  9. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Cambridge: The MIT Press. pp.  66. ISBN   9780262522083.
  10. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Cambridge: The MIT Press. pp.  66. ISBN   9780262522083.
  11. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "3.1 in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  12. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "Introduction in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  13. Keeley, Brian L. (2004). "Anthropomorphism, primatomorphism, mammalomorphism: understanding cross-species comparisons" (PDF). York University. p. 527. Archived (PDF) from the original on 17 December 2008. Retrieved 19 December 2008.
  14. Wynne, Clive D. L. (2001). Animal cognition: The Mental Lives of Animals. New York: Palgrave. p. 11. ISBN   9780333923962.
  15. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We smart Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are?. New York: W.W. Norton & Co. p. 41. ISBN   9780393353662.
  16. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New york: W.W. Norton & Co. p. 41. ISBN   9780393353662.
  17. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "3.1 in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  18. Shettleworth, Sara J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 17. ISBN   9780195319842.
  19. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "3.1 in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  20. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "3.2 in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  21. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New york: W.W. Norton & Co. p. 42. ISBN   9780393353662.
  22. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New York: W.W. Norton & Co. pp. 36–55. ISBN   9780393353662.
  23. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 253. ISBN   9780132929509.
  24. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of Psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 356. ISBN   9780132929509.
  25. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. pp. 361–362. ISBN   9780132929509.
  26. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Cambridge: The MIT Press. p.  66. ISBN   9780262522083.
  27. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of Psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 384. ISBN   9780132929509.
  28. Leahey, Thomas H. (2013). A History of Psychology: From Antiquity to Modernity. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 358. ISBN   9780132929509.
  29. Boakes, Robert (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 69–71. ISBN   9780521280129.
  30. Boakes, Robert (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 71. ISBN   9780521280129.
  31. Boakes, Robert (1984). in From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 72. ISBN   9780521280129.
  32. Boakes, Robert (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 72–83. ISBN   9780521280129.
  33. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 203. ISBN   9780132929509.
  34. Leahey, Thomas Hardy (2013). A History of psychology. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc. p. 384. ISBN   9780132929509.
  35. Page, George (1999). Inside the Animal Mind. New York: Doubleday. p. 37. ISBN   9780385492911.
  36. Norenzayan, Ara (2002). "Cultural preferences for formal verses intuitive reasoning". Cognitive Science. 26 (5): 653–684. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2605_4. hdl: 2027.42/91932 .
  37. Wood, Matthew (March 2019). "The Potential for Anthropomorphism in Communicating Science: Inspiration from Japan". Cultures of Science. 2 (1): 23–34. doi: 10.1177/209660831900200103 .
  38. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Colorado: The MIT Press. pp.  65–68. ISBN   9780262522083.
  39. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Colorado: The MIT Press. pp.  65–68. ISBN   9780262522083.
  40. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition . Colorado: The MIT Press. pp.  65–68. ISBN   9780262522083.
  41. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. p. 41. ISBN   9780393353662.
  42. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New York: W. W. Norton & Company Inc. p. 26. ISBN   9780393353662.
  43. Bekoff, Jamieson, Marc, Dale (1999). Readings in Animal Cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press. ISBN   9780262522083.
  44. Andrews, Kristen (6 May 2016). "3.3 Observable Methods in Animal Cognition". The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  45. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New York: W. W. Norton & Company Inc. p. 275. ISBN   9780393353662.
  46. de Waal, Frans (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are?. New York: W.W. Norton & Co. p. 42. ISBN   9780393353662.
  47. Shettleworth, Sara J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 19. ISBN   9780195319842.
  48. Kaplan, Gisela (2015). Bird Minds: Cognition and Behaviour of Australian Native Birds. Clayton South, Victoria: CSIRO Publishing. p. 132. ISBN   9781486300181.
  49. Gauld, Colin (December 1992). ""The Historical Anecdote as a "caricature": A Case Study."". Research in Science Education. 22 (1): 149. Bibcode:1992RScEd..22..149G. doi:10.1007/BF02356890. S2CID   143777248.
  50. Prodger, Phillip (2009). Darwin's camera art and photography in the theory of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 22. ISBN   9780199722303.
  51. Emery and Clayton, Nathan J.and Nicola S. (2005). "Animal Cognition". In Bolhuis and Giraldeau, Johan J. and Luc-Alain (ed.). The Behaviour of Animals: Mechanisms, Function and Evolution. Oxford: Blackwell publishing. p. 170. ISBN   9780631231257.
  52. Emery and Clayton, Nathan J. and Nicola S. (2005). "Animal Cognition". In Bolhuis and Giraldeau, Johan J. and Luc-Alain (ed.). The Behaviour of Animals: Mechanisms, Function and Evolution. Oxford: Blackwell publishing. p. 171. ISBN   9780631231257.
  53. Fernandez-Bellon, Kane, Dario, Adam (2020). "Natural History films raise species awareness- A big data approach". Conservation Letters. 13 (1): e12678. doi:10.1111/conl.12678. PMC   7074017 . PMID   32194654.
  54. Page, George (1999). Inside The Animal Mind. New York: Doubleday. ISBN   9780385492911.
  55. Townshend, Emma (2009). Darwin's Dogs: How Darwin's pets helped form a world-changing theory of evolution. London: Frances Lincoln. pp. Preface. ISBN   9781502623157.
  56. Harel, Kay (1 January 2001). "It's Dogged as Does It': A Biography of the Everpresent Canine in Charles Darwin's Days". Southwest Review. 93 (3): 368–378. JSTOR   43472917.
  57. Townshend, Emma (2009). Darwin's Dogs: How Darwin's pets helped form a world-changing theory of evolution. London: Frances Lincoln. pp. 1–6. ISBN   9780711230651.
  58. Harel, Kay (1 January 2008). "'It's Dogged as Does It': A Biography of the Everpresent Canine in Charles Darwin's Days". Southwest Review. 93 (3): 368 via JSTOR.
  59. Segedin, Kara (7 January 2016). "If David Attenborough could be an animal he'd be a sloth". earth BBC. Retrieved 14 April 2020.
  60. Mitchell, Megan (2017). "Contemporaries, Criticisms and Controversies". Jane Goodall: Primatologist and UN Messenger of Peace. New York: Cavendish. pp. 73–77. ISBN   9781502623157.
  61. Mitchell, Megan (2017). Jane Goodall: Primatologist and UN Messenger of Peace. New York: Cavendish. p. 69. ISBN   9781502623157.
  62. Nicholls, Henry (3 April 2014). "When I met Jane Goodall, she hugged me like a chimp". The Guardian. Retrieved 22 April 2020.
  63. Mitchell, Megan (2017). Jane Goodall: Primatologist and UN Messenger of Peace. New York: Cavendish Square publishing. p. 79. ISBN   9781502623157.
  64. DeKoven, Marianne (2006). "Women, Animals, and Jane Goodall: "Reason for Hope"". Tulsa Studies in Women's Literature. 25 (1): 141–51. doi: 10.2307/20455265 . JSTOR   20455265.

Further reading